共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Nicolas Giret Aurélie Albert Laurent Nagle Michel Kreutzer Dalila Bovet 《Acta ethologica》2012,15(1):39-46
A few animal species are capable of vocal learning. Parrots are well known for their vocal imitation abilities. In this study,
we investigated whether African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) emit specific vocalizations in specific contexts. We first described the vocal repertoire and its ontogenesis of four captive
grey parrots. After a comparison with vocalizations emitted by wild and other captive African grey parrots, we observed that
only three call categories were shared by all grey parrots populations, suggesting that isolated populations of parrots develop
population-specific calls. Then, we artificially provoked ten different contexts and recorded vocalizations of four captive
grey parrots in these situations. Parrots predominantly emitted call categories in some contexts: distress, protestation,
alarm, asking (i.e. emitted when a bird wanted something from an experimenter) and contact calls. These results suggest that
some calls are learned and can be used in specific contexts. 相似文献
2.
C Schloegl J Schmidt M Boeckle BM Weiß K Kotrschal 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2012,279(1745):4135-4142
Our ability to make logical inferences is considered as one of the cornerstones of human intelligence, fuelling investigations of reasoning abilities in non-human animals. Yet, the evidence to date is equivocal, with apes as the prime candidates to possess these skills. For instance, in a two-choice task, apes can identify the location of hidden food if it is indicated by a rattling noise caused by the shaking of a baited container. More importantly, they also use the absence of noise during the shaking of the empty container to infer that this container is not baited. However, since the inaugural report of apes solving this task, to the best of our knowledge, no comparable evidence could be found in any other tested species such as monkeys and dogs. Here, we report the first successful and instantaneous solution of the shaking task through logical inference by a non-ape species, the African grey parrot. Surprisingly, the performance of the birds was sensitive to the shaking movement: they were successful with containers shaken horizontally, but not with vertical shaking resembling parrot head-bobbing. Thus, grey parrots seem to possess ape-like cross-modal reasoning skills, but their reliance on these abilities is influenced by low-level interferences. 相似文献
3.
Nicolas Giret Franck Péron Laurent Nagle Michel Kreutzer Dalila Bovet 《Behavioural processes》2009,82(3):244-248
The ability to categorize elements of the environment is a fundamental aspect of information processing. Many experiments demonstrate the ability of birds and non-human primates to classify items according to their perceptual similarities. Few data are available regarding spontaneous classification of items according to a non-perceptual account in non-human animals. Here, we report unexpected results obtained with African grey parrots learning the referential use of French labels. Parrots did not learn the correct labels but they spontaneously produced more labels corresponding to food when a food item was presented to them and more labels corresponding to an object when shown an object item, although they were never rewarded for doing so. These results demonstrate a form of spontaneous categorization by using vocal imitation of the human language. 相似文献
4.
Living in social groups presents the opportunity to use information provided by other individuals. Several animal species emit specific vocalizations when they find food. Here, we investigate whether African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) use vocal and non-vocal information provided by a conspecific in order to find a hidden food source. One subject was attracted by the presence or the vocalizations of a subordinate conspecific, but not of a dominant, which brings us to hypothesize that parrots could be capable of individual vocal discrimination. 相似文献
5.
This study describes 12 microsatellite loci identified in the African grey parrot Psittacus erithacus. Eleven were polymorphic, with observed heterozygosities 42–94% (average 68) and exclusion powers of PE1 = 0.996 and PE2 = 0.999. Microsatellites have previously been developed for a number of other parrots but showed limited cross‐species polymorphism. Here high levels of cross‐species amplification were observed: 71% of 32 Psittacines (22 genera). At least seven loci, 58%, were polymorphic in other African parrots as well as Neotropical and Australasian parrots, which diverged from the African parrots c30.6 and over 41.4 million years ago, respectively. 相似文献
6.
Irene M. Pepperberg 《Ethology : formerly Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie》1981,55(2):139-160
An African grey parrot, after 26 months of speech training, has acquired a verbal vocabulary consisting of three color adjectives, two phrases for describing shape, 9 nouns, and functional use of the word “no”. The parrot is capable of correctly combining these vocalizations so as to identify proficiently, request, and/or refuse more than 30 objects by means of verbal labels. 相似文献
7.
Nicolas Giret Franck Péron Lenka Tichotová Michel Kreutzer Dalila Bovet 《Behavioural processes》2010,85(2):90-877
Some African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus), the most famous being Pepperberg's parrot Alex, are able to imitate human speech and produce labels referentially. In this study, the aim was to teach ten African grey parrots from two laboratories to label items. Training three parrots from the first laboratory for several months with the Model/Rival method, developed by Pepperberg, in which two humans interact in front of the subject to demonstrate the use of a label, led to disappointing results. Similarly, seven parrots from the second laboratory, having been trained with several variants of Model/Rival attained little success. After the informal observation of the efficiency of other methods (i.e. learning to imitate labels either spontaneously or with specific learning methods and use of these labels referentially), four different teaching methods were tested with two birds: the Model/Rival; Repetition/Association which consisted of repeating a label and presenting the item only when the parrot produced the label; Intuitive in which the experimenter handled an item and repeated its name in front of the subject; Diffusion in which labels with either variable or flat intonation were played back daily to parrots. One bird learned three labels, one of which was used referentially, with the Repetition/Association method. He learned one label non-referentially with the Model/Rival but no labels were acquired using the other methods. The second bird did not learn any labels. This study demonstrates that different methods can be efficient to teach labels referentially and it suggests that rearing conditions and interindividual variability are important features when assessing learning ability of African grey parrots. 相似文献
8.
Comparative genome analysis promises to provide an insight into avian species that have been very little studied. To test the feasibility of this approach, we investigated the use of heterologous primers to generate single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNP) in the African grey parrot, Psittacus erythacus, using primers specific for chicken and turkey DNA fragments. Three of the primers were specific for three expressed sequence tagged sites in the turkey and the fourth for a chicken proteoglycan core protein-like DNA sequence. A total of about 2200 bp of the parrot genome was evaluated for DNA sequence variation. Seven SNPs were identified and confirmed by Mendelian segregation. The frequency distribution of the most common nucleotide at each SNP locus in an unrelated group of parrots ranged from 0.84 to 0.97. The percent similarity of each parrot sequence to the reference sequence was inconsistent and ranged from zero to 100%. The primers as well as the nucleotide variants described represent valuable resources for genetic analysis in the parrot. This revised version was published online in July 2006 with corrections to the Cover Date. 相似文献
9.
de Kloet SR 《Journal of molecular evolution》2001,53(2):135-143
This study describes the results of an analysis using Southern blotting, the polymerase chain reaction, and sequencing which
shows that the African grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus) lacks the W-chromosomal gene for the alpha subunit of mitochondrial ATP synthase (ATP5A1W). Additional evidence shows that
in other psittacines a fragment of the ATP5A1W gene contains five times as many nonsynonymous nucleotide replacements as the
homologous fragment of the Z gene. Therefore, whereas in these other psittacines the corresponding ATP5A1Z protein fragment
is highly conserved and varies by only a few, moderately conservative amino acid substitutions, the homologous ATP5A1W fragments
contain a considerable number of, sometimes highly nonconservative, amino acid replacements. In one of these species, the
ringneck parakeet (Psittacula krameri), the ATP5A1W gene is present in an inactive form because of the presence of a nonsense codon. Other changes, possibly leading
to an inactive ATP5A1W gene product, involve the substitution of arginine residues by cysteine in the ATP5A1W protein of the
mitred conure (Aratinga mitrata) and the blue and gold macaw (Ara ararauna). The data suggest also that although the divergence of the psittacine ATP5A1W and ATP5A1Z genes preceeded the origin of
the psittacidae, this divergence occurred independently of a similar process in the myna (Gracula religiosa), the outgroup used in this study.
Received: 6 September 2000 / Accepted: 7 March 2001 相似文献
10.
11.
Pathogen-driven balancing selection maintains high genetic diversity in many vertebrates, particularly in the major histocompatibility complex (MHC) immune system gene family, which is often associated with disease susceptibility. In large natural populations where subpopulations face different pathogen pressures, the MHC should show greater genetic differentiation within a species than neutral markers. We examined genetic diversity at the MHC-DQB locus and nine putatively neutral microsatellite markers in grey seals (Halichoerus grypus) from eight United Kingdom (UK) colonies, the Faeroe Islands and Sable Island, Canada. Five DQB alleles were identified in grey seals, which varied in prevalence across the grey seal range. Among the seal colonies, significant differences in DQB allele and haplotype frequencies and in average DQB heterozygosity were observed. Additionally, the DQB gene exhibited greater differentiation among colonies compared with neutral markers, yet a weaker pattern of isolation by distance (IBD). After correcting for the underlying IBD pattern, subpopulations breeding in similar habitats were more similar to one another in DQB allele frequencies than populations breeding in different habitats, but the same did not hold true for microsatellites, suggesting that habitat-specific pathogen pressure influences MHC evolution. Overall, the data are consistent with selection at MHC-DQB loci in grey seals with both varying selective pressures and geographic population structure appearing to influence the DQB genetic composition of breeding colonies. 相似文献
12.
By following the solute exclusion technique, we determined the pore characteristics of 3 hardwood high-yield pulps (aspen, birch, maple). The fiber saturation point (FSP) was 1.40, 1.36 and 1.19 g water/g pulp, for aspen, birch, maple, respectively, which is lower than that of chemical pulps. Different fractions obtained from the Bauer–McNett classifier showed that the HYP fines have much more pore volume than their long fiber counterparts. The effects of beating, drying and re-wetting on the pore characteristics of HYP were also studied. Beating led to increased total pore volumes. Upon drying and re-wetting, much of the small pores from the HYP underwent permanent closure while the big pores were only slightly affected. Finally, the relationship between the water retention value (WRV) and FSP for HYP was examined. 相似文献
13.
Fertility-advertisement calls in females are predicted to occur in nonmonogamous species where males and females are widely separated in space. In African elephants, low-frequency vocalizations have thus been suggested as a reproductive strategy used by fertile females to attract mates. This study examined the use of low-frequency vocalizations with respect to different phases of the estrous cycle in African elephants by simultaneously monitoring vocalizations, behavior, and hormonal profiles. Subjects were one male and six female African elephants housed at Disney's Animal Kingdom. No acoustically distinct vocalizations were restricted to the ovulatory follicular phase. However, overall rate of low-frequency vocalization as well as the rate of one acoustically distinct vocalization changed over the estrous cycle, with highest rates of calling related to the first period of follicular growth, or anovulatory follicular phase. Elevated rates of vocalization thus were not restricted to behavioral estrus and occurred much earlier in the estrous cycle than in most species that produce fertility-advertisement calls. Both herd composition and elephant identity also affected rates of vocalization. Vocalizations therefore may not be reliable signals of actual fertility. However, the increase in vocalizations in advance of estrus may attract males to the herd prior to ovulation, facilitating both male-male competition and female choice. Once present in the herd, males may then switch strategies to use more reliable chemical and visual cues to detect ovulating females. 相似文献
14.
Resistance to Rhipicephalus appendiculatus, Amblyomma variegatum and Amblyomma hebraeum was investigated in the laboratory by infesting rabbits with adults of each of the three species followed by homospecific or heterospecific secondary infestations. Significantly lower female engorged weights and egg mass weights were taken as evidence of protective immunity. Following a single infestation with adults, rabbits developed homospecific protective immunity (resistance) to only R. appendiculatus and A. hebraeum; primary infestation with A variegatum did not protect against secondary infestation with the same species. There was no cross-resistance (heterospecific protective immunity) between the species except for one-way protection between R. appendiculatus and A. variegatum; primary infestation with R. appendiculatus protected against secondary infestation with A. variegatum, but not vice versa. The results from ELISA did not indicate any correlation between serum antibodies to soluble antigens from salivary gland extracts and protective immunity. Post-infestation sera from rabbits infested with each of the three species reacted strongly to their respective salivary gland extracts. Despite the high reactivity of A. variegatum serum with salivary gland antigens from all three species, A. variegatum-infested rabbits did not show any homospecific or heterospecific immunity; on the other hand, although R. appendiculatus serum did not react positively to A. variegatum antigens, infestation with R. appendiculatus protected against a subsequent A. variegatum infestation. 相似文献
15.
Interspecific competition between red squirrels and grey squirrels was investigated by comparing the population demography, spacing behavior and habitat use of red squirrels in two large conifer plantations in northern England: one site had only red squirrels (the red-only site), in the other both red and grey squirrels occurred (the red–grey site). Despite more abundant food at the red–grey site, red squirrel densities (0.26 ha–1 at the red–grey site, 0.29 ha–1 at the red-only site), adult survival rates and the breeding rates of females were similar at both study sites. Grey squirrels at the red–grey site occurred at higher densities (0.92–1.1 ha–1) than did the reds and tended to have higher breeding rates. In the presence of grey squirrels, the recruitment pattern of red squirrels changed and there was little recruitment of subadults. The juvenile recruitment rate in the red–grey site (13%) was much lower than in the red-only site (50%). Grey squirrels, in contrast, had higher juvenile recruitment rates at the red–grey site (41%). The core areas of the home ranges of red squirrels in the red–grey site were more strongly overlapped by grey squirrels than by conspecifics. Red squirrels did not select the habitat with the best tree seed crop (Scots pine) but preferred dense Sitka spruce plantations; they appeared to avoid the Scots pine area with its high grey squirrel density. Data on foot length and body condition indicated decreased body growth in young red squirrels when grey squirrels were present. Our data suggest that adult red squirrels suffered little from interspecific competition with grey squirrels and that the key factor is decreased juvenile recruitment in red squirrels. 相似文献
16.
Parrots are frequently cited for their sophisticated problem-solving abilities, but cases of habitual tool use among psittacines are scarce. We report the first evidence, to our knowledge, of tool use by greater vasa parrots (Coracopsis vasa). Several members of a captive population spontaneously adopted a novel tool-using technique by using pebbles and date pits either (i) to scrape on the inner surface of seashells, subsequently licking the resulting calcium powder from the tool, or (ii) as a wedge to break off smaller pieces of the shell for ingestion. Tool use occurred most frequently just prior to the breeding season, during which time numerous instances of tool transfer were also documented. These observations provide new insights into the tool-using capabilities of parrots and highlight the greater vasa parrot as a species of interest for studies of physical cognition. 相似文献
17.
W A Palmer 《Australian Journal of Entomology》2005,44(1):1-1
Abstract Contrary to the statements by Morris and Mound (2004), Scirtothrips aurantii were first found outside the quarantine facility on mother-of-million plants ( Bryophyllum spp.) sourced from the western suburbs of Brisbane and in January 2003 the distribution of the thrips in south-eastern Queensland was from at least 20 km south-east and 70 km west of the detection site. Some records in South Africa of thrips from mother-of-million plants may not refer to this species. 相似文献
18.
19.
Four chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) monitored the movement of hidden items in arrays of opaque cups. A chocolate candy was hidden in an array of four cups and temporarily presented paper markers indicated the location of the candy (which otherwise was not visible). These markers were either non-symbolic or symbolic (lexigram) stimuli that in other contexts acted as a label for the hidden candy, and the array was either rotated 180° after the marker was removed or the array remained in the same location. For three of four chimpanzees, performance was better than chance in all conditions and there was no effect of the type of marker. These experiments indicate that chimpanzees can track the movement of a hidden item in an array of identical cups even when they never see the item itself, but only see a temporarily presented marker for the location of that item. However, there was no benefit to the use of symbolic as opposed to non-symbolic stimuli in this performance. 相似文献
20.
通过应用灰色系统理论和方法,对彭泽县1960~1986年第三代棉红铃虫百株孵量构成的离散数列进行数据处理,用微分方程进行拟合,并建立棉红铃虫灰色系统GM(1,1)长期灾变预测模型:x(t)=53.60952e0.17851(i-1)1.97246e0.03736(i-1)-3.56687e-0.19052(i-1)-46.93949。应用该模型进行回测和预测,回测符合率可达100%,1987~1991年的预测结果与实况相吻合。 相似文献