首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Recent studies have shown that elevated CO2 can affect the behaviour of larval and juvenile fishes. In particular, behavioural lateralization, an expression of brain functional asymmetries, is affected by elevated CO2 in both coral reef and temperate fishes. However, the potentially interacting effects of rising temperatures and CO2 on lateralization are unknown. Here, we tested the combined effect of near-future elevated-CO2 concentrations (930 µatm) and temperature variation on behavioural lateralization of a marine damselfish, Pomacentrus wardi. Individuals exposed to one of four treatments (two CO2 levels and two temperatures) were observed in a detour test where they made repeated decisions about turning left or right. Individuals exposed to current CO2 and ambient temperature levels showed a significant right-turning bias at the population level. This biased was reversed (i.e. to the left side) in fish exposed to the elevated-CO2 treatment. Increased temperature attenuated this effect, resulting in lower values of relative lateralization. Consequently, rising temperature and elevated CO2 may have different and interactive effects on behavioural lateralization and therefore future studies on the effect of climate change on brain functions need to consider both these critical variables in order to assess the potential consequences for the ecological interactions of marine fishes.  相似文献   

2.
Elevated carbon dioxide (CO(2)) has recently been shown to affect chemosensory and auditory behaviour, and activity levels of larval reef fishes, increasing their risk of predation. However, the mechanisms underlying these changes are unknown. Behavioural lateralization is an expression of brain functional asymmetries, and thus provides a unique test of the hypothesis that elevated CO(2) affects brain function in larval fishes. We tested the effect of near-future CO(2) concentrations (880 μatm) on behavioural lateralization in the reef fish, Neopomacentrus azysron. Individuals exposed to current-day or elevated CO(2) were observed in a detour test where they made repeated decisions about turning left or right. No preference for right or left turns was observed at the population level. However, individual control fish turned either left or right with greater frequency than expected by chance. Exposure to elevated-CO(2) disrupted individual lateralization, with values that were not different from a random expectation. These results provide compelling evidence that elevated CO(2) directly affects brain function in larval fishes. Given that lateralization enhances performance in a number of cognitive tasks and anti-predator behaviours, it is possible that a loss of lateralization could increase the vulnerability of larval fishes to predation in a future high-CO(2) ocean.  相似文献   

3.
A study exploiting the gravimetric method did not reveal identical development of paired parts of the roach (Rutilus rutilus) brain. In the studied sample, the proportion of fish with left or right asymmetry of this organ was 1: 1. The specificity of fluctuating asymmetry in studied plastic parameter depending on the fish age and growth was analyzed. In the ontogenetic sequence, from yearlings to nine-year-old fishes, the higher level of developmental instability was revealed in elder fish, six and more years old. Noted anatomical-morphological feature is likely determined by not only imperfect morphogenesis during early development: potential decrease in the threshold of noise immunity against unfavorable external and internal factors during later stages of fish development may contribute to the feature noted above. The analysis of sex-related traits revealed that in males fluctuating asymmetry of the brain bilateral parts is more pronounced.  相似文献   

4.
The underyearlings of roach, Rutilus rutilus, displayed a bilateral asymmetry of C-bend—the initial stage of escape behavior. Most individuals exhibited a significant bias to turn leftward or rightward after being stimulated by electrical current. This individual asymmetry was consistent when the same fish were retested ten days later. A significant correlation was revealed between the behavioral asymmetry and the bilateral asymmetry of surface area of frontal (positive correlation) and parietal (negative correlation) cranial bones. No significant correlation was found with the bilateral asymmetry of other morphological characteristics: numbers of pores of the seismosensory canals (praeopercular-mandibular, supraorbital, and supratemporal) in flat cranial bones (dental, praeopercular, frontal, and parietal numbers of lateral line pores, and numbers of rays in pectoral and ventral fins.  相似文献   

5.
The objective of this study was to investigate the ability of horses (Equus caballus) to detour around symmetric and asymmetric obstacles. Ten female Italian saddle horses were each used in three detour tasks. In the first task, the ability to detour around a symmetrical obstacle was evaluated; in the second and third tasks subjects were required to perform a detour around an asymmetrical obstacle with two different degrees of asymmetry. The direction chosen to move around the obstacle and time required to make the detour were recorded. The results suggest that horses have the spatial abilities required to perform detour tasks with both symmetric and asymmetric obstacles. The strategy used to perform the task varied between subjects. For five horses, lateralized behaviour was observed when detouring the obstacle; this was consistently in one direction (three on the left and two on the right). For these horses, no evidence of spatial learning or reasoning was found. The other five horses did not solve this task in a lateralized manner, and a trend towards decreasing lateralization was observed as asymmetry, and hence task difficulty, increased. These non-lateralized horses may have higher spatial reasoning abilities.  相似文献   

6.
Escape performance is fundamental for survival in fish and most other animals. While previous work has shown that both intrinsic (e.g. size, shape) and extrinsic (e.g. temperature, hypoxia) factors can affect escape performance, the possibility that behavioural asymmetry may affect timing and locomotor performance in startled fish is largely unexplored. Numerous studies have found a relationship between brain lateralization and performance in several cognitive tasks. Here, we tested the hypothesis that behavioural lateralization may affect escape performance in a teleost, the shiner perch Cymatogaster aggregata. Escape responses were elicited by mechanical stimulation and recorded using high-speed video (250 Hz). A number of performance variables were analysed, including directionality, escape latency, turning rate and distance travelled within a fixed time. A lateralization index was obtained by testing the turning preference of each subject in a detour test. While lateralization had no effect on escape directionality, strongly lateralized fish showed higher escape reactivity, i.e. shorter latencies, which were associated with higher turning rates and longer distances travelled. Therefore, lateralization is likely to result in superior ability to escape from predator attacks, since previous work has shown that escape timing, turning rate and distance travelled are among the main determinants of escape success.  相似文献   

7.
The initial stage of the escape response (C-bend of the body) upon sudden impact of electric current on free-swimming fish was studied in young roach (Rutilus rutilus). More than half of the fish showed distinct behavioral laterality, i.e., a stabile tendency to bend either to the left or to the right, and retained it until the next test conducted 10 days later. The majority of fish bent to the left, but the difference between the proportions of individuals that showed opposite lateralities lacked statistical significance. This response and the type of laterality are stable and simple to record, which makes the C-bend a convenient experimental model for studying behavioral asymmetry in fishes.  相似文献   

8.
Estimation of the invasion character and distribution of the Diplostomum huronense metacercariae depending on the fish host age has been carried out in roach of Lake Ladoga. Distribution of D. huronense in the young roach (up to age 5+) is negative binomial. Aggregation of the parasite is caused not only by individual differences in the fish host resistance to the metacercariae invasion, but by a high mortality of hyperinfected fishes as well. In older individual hosts the host-parasite system is destroyed, and the parasite distribution approximates to normal. The parasite system of the diplostomids in roach is characterized by the spatial asymmetry, when most part of parasites inhabit few individual hosts, and probability of the case when a parasite get into previously infected host is higher than that of the invasion of parasite-free host. Thus, the Diplostomum huronense metacercaria is an important factor regulating the fish fry number both in direct (death of infected fry) and indirect (elimination of hyperinfected fishes by ichthyophagous birds) way.  相似文献   

9.
Juvenile roach Rutilus rutilus without any previous contact with predatory fishes reacted to the presence of alarm substances more intensively than fish with previous experience. Among several types of behavioural response observed, some, such as hiding, were not registered in experienced fish. The most pronounced difference between the two juvenile fish categories was manifested in feeding. Naïve fish used more time for feeding than experienced ones. The experiment showed that alarm substance originating from small and young and medium roach induced a stronger reaction than the substance extracted from large and oldéê fish.  相似文献   

10.
Intraspecific differentiation in response to divergent natural selection between environments is a common phenomenon in some northern freshwater fishes, especially salmonids and stickleback. Understanding why these taxa diversify and undergo adaptive radiations while most other fish species in the same environments do not, remains an open question. The possibility for intraspecific diversification has rarely been evaluated for most northern freshwater fish species. Here, we assess the potential for intraspecific differentiation between and within lake populations of roach (Rutilus rutilus)—a widespread and abundant cyprinid species—in lakes in which salmonids have evolved endemic adaptive radiations. Based on more than 3,000 polymorphic RADseq markers, we detected low but significant genetic differentiation between roach populations of two ultraoligotrophic lakes and between these and populations from other lakes. This, together with differentiation in head morphology and stable isotope signatures, suggests evolutionary and ecological differentiation among some of our studied populations. Next, we tested for intralacustrine diversification of roach within Lake Brienz, the most pristine lake surveyed in this study. We found significant phenotypic evidence for ecological intralacustrine differentiation between roach caught over a muddy substrate and those caught over a rocky substrate. However, evidence for intralacustrine genetic differentiation is at best subtle and phenotypic changes may therefore be mostly plastic. Overall, our findings suggest roach can differ between ecologically distinct lakes, but the extent of intralacustrine ecological differentiation is weak, which contrasts with the strong differentiation among endemic species of whitefish in the same lakes.  相似文献   

11.
The present study investigated, for the first time, a non‐invasive technique for monitoring cortisol levels in fishes that had been implanted with electronic transmitters. This technique involved the measurement of cortisol released into the water by individual fish. Carp Cyprinus carpio and roach Rutilus rutilus that had been tagged with miniature dummy acoustic transmitters responded to the surgical implantation of the tags with an immediate (1 to 4 h) increase in cortisol concentrations in the water. These amounts of cortisol were higher than in 'handled only' and 'handled and anaesthetized' groups. Estimated cortisol release rates for both species correlated well with the direct measurements in the water. In both species, water cortisol concentration and cortisol release rates returned to pre‐stress levels within 12 h of tag‐insertion, and remained low for the remainder of the experiment. The calculation of cortisol release rates highlighted a difference of a factor of about four between peak release rates in carp and roach. There was no evident long‐term cortisol stress response to the presence of a tag in the body cavity in either species.  相似文献   

12.
Cerebral lateralization, an evolutionarily ancient and widespread phenomenon among vertebrates, is thought to bestow cognitive advantages. The advantages of lateralization at the individual-level do not necessarily require that the entire population share the same pattern of lateralization. In fact, directional bias in lateralization may lead to behavioural predictability and enhanced predator success or prey evasion. Recent theory has suggested that population-level lateralization may be favored if individuals are better able to perform coordinated behaviours, providing a distinct advantage in cooperative contexts. Here we test whether the highly social, cooperatively breeding cichlid fish Neolamprologus pulcher shows lateralized responses to a social stimulus. We found population-level biases in males; on average male N. pulcher use their right eye/left hemisphere to view their mirror image. Individual females had a preferred hemisphere, but these preferences appeared not to be directionally aligned among females. We discuss these results in the context of coordinated social behaviour and suggest future research directions.  相似文献   

13.
We examine lateralization of lateral displays in convict cichlids, Amatitlania nigrofasciata, and show a population level preference for showing the right side. This enables contesting pairs of fish to align in a head-to-tail posture, facilitating other activities. We found individuals spent a shorter mean time in each left compared with each right lateral display. This lateralization could lead to contesting pairs using a convention to align in a predictable head-to-tail arrangement to facilitate the assessment of fighting ability. It has major implications for the common use of mirror images to study fish aggression, because the 'opponent' would never cooperate and would consistently show the incorrect side when the real fish shows the correct side. With the mirror, the 'normal' head-to-tail orientation cannot be achieved.  相似文献   

14.
We investigated whether the development of spatial behaviour of the domestic chicken is influenced by light exposure of the embryo, as is known to be the case for some other lateralized visual functions. Ninety-six chicks were incubated in the dark or exposed to light during the final days of incubation. Half of the chicks in each group had the experience of moving behind opaque screens from 10 to 12 days of age. The other half were given transparent screens as a control. Chicks were tested in a detour test and a rotated floor test and their dispersal in groups was observed in larger pens. In the rotated floor test, chicks that had had experience with opaque screens used distal cues significantly more often than chicks that had experience with transparent screens (P = 0.042), regardless of whether they had been exposed to light before hatching or incubated in the dark. There were no significant differences between treatments in the detour test or in the dispersal behaviour. Hence, visual lateralization has no influence on the development of the spatial behaviour that we tested, whereas the occlusion experience is quite specific and results in shifted attention to distal spatial cues.  相似文献   

15.
The modern ichthyofauna of the Lena River includes 46 species and subspecies: 45 bony fishes and one lamprey. In the upper reaches, 24 species have been recorded, 38 in the middle reaches, 36 in the lower reaches, and 24 in the delta. Four of the seven species intentionally introduced into water bodies of the Lena River basin have successfully naturalized; the presence of the others is unconfirmed. All areas of the river are dominated by fishes of the orders Salmoniformes and Cypriniformes. The ranges of the Amur carp Cyprinus rubrofuscus, common bream Abramis brama, ide Leuciscus idus, roach Rutilus rutilus, pike Esox lucius, and perch Perca fluviatilis have expanded over the last few years. The causes and possible ways of the historical formation of the Lena River fish population are discussed in the light of data on the Pleistocene paleogeographical events and patterns of the distribution of species in the river and in adjacent basins.  相似文献   

16.
In recent years a considerable decrease in the abundance of predatory fishes has been observed in spawning tributaries of the Rybinsk Reservoir caused by their intensive catching. The lack of encounters with predators before the downstream migration of young fish hampers the development of necessary skills of defensive behavior in the absence of predation experience. As a result, after downstream migration, the juveniles are incapable of adapting to the predation pressure in the reservoir and are subjected to intensive elimination. The adaptive potential of the roach Rutilus rutilus L. was experimentally studied in siblings raised from the larvae to the late fry stage both in the presence and absence of a predator. It has been found that the fry that was raised under different conditions differed in their adaptive potential in new environment conditions.  相似文献   

17.
The preferential use of one hand over the other is considered the primary behavioral expression of structural and functional asymmetry in cerebral structures, which is a decisive factor in human evolution. We present the first analysis of manual laterality in a form of object play—stone handling (SH) behavior—in a free-ranging group of Japanese macaques. Defined as a stone-directed manipulative activity, and comprised of multiple one-handed SH patterns (e.g., grabbing a stone in one hand and cradling it against its chest), as well as coordinated two-handed SH patterns with manual role differentiation (e.g., holding a stone with one hand and rubbing it with the other), SH behavior is a good candidate for the study of hand lateralization. We systematically followed the methodological framework developed by McGrew and Marchant (1997) to measure and analyze the presence, strength, and direction of manual preference in the performance of SH behavior and in various SH patterns, both at the individual and group level. Some individuals showed a significant manual lateral bias on a single SH pattern (hand preference), whereas others showed consistency in laterality across all or most of the SH patterns they performed (hand specialization). At the group level, we found that, although their collective distribution of left versus right remained random, most subjects were either significantly but incompletely lateralized, or completely lateralized within particular SH patterns (pattern specialization), but not across all SH patterns (no handedness for SH behavior as a whole). As predicted by the task-complexity model, hand specialization and handedness were stronger in the coordinated bimanual SH patterns than in the unimanual patterns. We discuss the implications of our findings for the evolution of manual preferences in noninstrumental object manipulation versus stone tool use in nonhuman primates and hominins.  相似文献   

18.
The structure of gyrodactylid assemblages in individual fishes of two species of cyprinid was determined. A total of 100 specimens of minnow, Phoxinus phoxinus, and 137 specimens of roach, Rutilus rutilus, were investigated for presence of gyrodactylids. Host specificity, specialists vs. generalists, was noted in each host fish. A nested pattern was recorded in parasite assemblages of minnow, the host with a dominant number of specialist gyrodactylids. A non-nested pattern was observed in parasite assemblages of roach, the host with a dominant number of generalist gyrodactylids. The host specificity appears to be a meaningful factor that determines the pattern of gyrodactylid assemblages of both fish hosts.  相似文献   

19.
Lateralization is the function specialization between left and right brain hemispheres. It is now ascertained in ectotherms too, where bias in eye use for different tasks, i.e., visual lateralization, is widespread. The lateral eye position on the head of ectotherm animals, in fact, allows them to observe left/right stimuli independently and allows lateralized individuals to carry out left and right perceived tasks at the same time. A recent study conducted on common wall lizards, Podarcis muralis, showed that lizards predominantly monitor a predator with the left eye while escaping. However, this work was conducted in a controlled laboratory setting owing to the difficulty of carrying out lateralization experiments under natural conditions. Nevertheless, field studies could provide important information to support what was previously found in the laboratory and demonstrate that these traits occur in nature. In this study, we conducted a field study on the antipredatory behavior of P. muralis lizards. We simulated predatory attacks on lizards in their natural environment. We found no lateralization in the measure of eye used by the lizard to monitor the predator before escaping from it, but the eye used was probably determined by the relative position of the lizard and the predator just before the attack. This first eye used did not affect escape decisions; lizards chose to escape toward the nearest refuge irrespective of whether it was located to the lizard’s left or right side. However, once they had escaped to a refuge, lizards had a left eye–mediated bias to monitor the predator when first emerging from the refuge, and this bias was likely independent of other environmental variables. Hence, these field findings support a left eye–mediated observation of the predator in P. muralis lizards, which confirms previous findings in this and other species.  相似文献   

20.
We examined the morphological peculiarities of Mauthner neurons, MNs, in goldfishes with a phenotypically different or an experimentally modified preference to perform rightward vs leftward turnings in the course of motor behavior; this preference was characterized by values of the motor asymmetry coefficient (MAC). 3D reconstruction of MNs was performed based on several histological sections; volumes of the soma, lateral and ventral dendrites (LD and VD, respectively), initial segment of the axon, as well as full volumes of the right and left neurons, were calculated. Differences between the above parameters were expressed as structural asymmetry coefficients (SACs). It was shown that clear orientation asymmetry of motor behavior of the fish is accompanied by differences in the dimensions of MNs and their compartments; MNs localized contralaterally with respect to the preferred turning side were considerably bigger than ipsilateral neurons. Experimental influences inducing inversion of the motor asymmetry of fishes inverted structural asymmetry of their MNs. In fishes with no phenotypical preference of the turning side and in individuals whose motor asymmetry was smoothed due to experimental influences (rotational stimulations), structural asymmetry of the MNs was also smoothed. Changes of the structural proportions developed, as a rule, due to decreases in the dimensions of one or both MNs and their compartments. The MAC value was in direct correlation with the value of SAC of the MNs and with values of this coefficient for the soma and the sum soma + LD. At the same time, reciprocal relations were found for the MAC and structural asymmetry of the VD; the decrease in the volume of VD was related to an increase in the preference of the contralateral turning side by the fish, and vice versa. In general, the results of our study demonstrate that both morphological and functional peculiarities of MNs correlate to a significant extent with such a form of motor behavior of fishes as realization of spontaneous turnings. Neirofiziologiya/Neurophysiology, Vol. 38, No. 1, pp. 18–31, January–February, 2006.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号