首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
The effects of various agonists and antagonists of both alpha and beta adrenoceptors on the acquisition of avoidance behaviour were investigated in the rat. Clonidine, a selective agonist of alpha 2 adrenoceptors depressed avoidance acquisition whilst yohimbine, an antagonist of these receptors produced an opposite effect. Prazosin which showed postsynaptic alpha 1 adrenoceptor blocking activity reduced avoidance behaviour. A similar effect was produced by propranolol, a non-selective antagonist of beta adrenoceptors. On the other hand, salbutamol preferentially stimulating beta 2 adrenoceptors facilitated avoidance behaviour. In general, the results show a fairly good correlation between avoidance acquisition and efficacy of noradrenergic neurotransmission.  相似文献   

2.
We reported previously that D-Pen2-[D-Pen5]enkephalin (DPDE), a delta-opioid receptor selective analog of Leu-enkephalin, impairs acquisition of an automated jump-up avoidance response in rats and acquisition of a one-way active avoidance response in mice. In the present study we investigated the effects of DPDPE on one-way avoidance conditioning in rats. The rats received two escape-only trials on day 1 and eight additional training trials on day 2. DPDPE (1.16 micrograms/kg IP) administered prior to training on day 2 impaired acquisition of the avoidance response. On the other hand, DPDPE (0.332 microgram/kg IP) administered following presentation of the two escape-only trials on day 1 significantly enhanced retention, as measured by improved one-way active avoidance performance on day 2. These results indicate that activation of delta-opioid receptors by DPDPE has a modulatory effect on acquisition and retention of aversively motivated performance.  相似文献   

3.
Vasopressin (AVP) levels were measured in plasma and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) of rats during acquisition and retention of a passive avoidance response. Only 5 min after the onset of the retention session a significantly higher level of AVP was found in plasma of animals which displayed a long latency, as compared with the levels of animals which showed a weak passive avoidance response (short latencies), or no passive avoidance behavior at all (controls). Moreover no changes in plasma AVP levels were found in plasma of rats submitted to acquisition or extinction of an active avoidance response. It is suggested that, although an elevated plasma AVP level is associated with strong retention of a passive avoidance response the peripheral circulation as well as the CSF are of minor importance for the transport of this neuropeptide to its site of behavioral action.  相似文献   

4.
The effects of peripherally administered cholecystokinin octapeptide sulphate ester and its unsulphated form on the active avoidance behaviour of rats were studied. The acquisition of avoidance behaviour was impaired, while extinction was facilitated, following cholecystokinin octapeptide sulphate ester or unsulphated cholecystokinin octapeptide treatment. These peptides had no action on open-field activity. It is concluded that peripherally administered cholecystokinin octapeptide influences acquisition and extinction of active avoidance behaviour and this effect is unrelated to general motor activity of the animals.  相似文献   

5.
Fear conditioning, escape and active avoidance reactions in two-way avoidance paradigm were compared in rats of different ages. Fear conditioning, but not escape and active avoidance reactions could be acquired on the 16-17th postnatal days, and the acquisition was more effective than in adults. Escape behavior matured beginning from the 18th postnatal day reaching the adult level within the 3d-4th postnatal weeks. Maturation of the mechanisms of Pavlovian (fear reaction) and instrumental (escape reaction) conditioning did not facilitate the acquisition of two-way avoidance until the 4th postnatal week, young animals displayed low acquisition in this period. The maturation of these memory processes is proposed to be related to developmental stages of different mechanisms of hippocampal plasticity.  相似文献   

6.
Estradiol accelerates extinction of LiCl-induced conditioned taste avoidance when it is present continuously before and during acquisition. We have suggested that the effect of estradiol on extinction is due to its illness-associated, rather than learning-associated, properties. If this were the case, then one would expect estradiol to act before but not during acquisition. This expectation is based on previous work showing attenuation of learned taste avoidance when rats are given distal preexposure (greater than 24 h before conditioning) or proximal preexposure (less than 24 h before conditioning) to the illness-inducing agent LiCl before acquisition of a LiCl-induced conditioned taste avoidance. In three separate experiments, estradiol was administered during three different time periods via subcutaneous implantation of a 10-mm estradiol-filled capsule. In each experiment, the extinction of estradiol-treated females was compared to that of females implanted with empty capsules. In the first experiment, female rats were given distal exposure to estradiol before acquisition. Capsules were implanted 11 days before acquisition and were removed 2 days before acquisition. In the second experiment, female rats were given proximal exposure to estradiol before acquisition. Capsules were implanted 2.5 h before LiCl was paired with a sucrose solution and were removed 16.5 h later. In the third experiment, female rats were given exposure to estradiol during acquisition. Capsules were implanted at the same time as LiCl administration and were removed 18 h later. The only estradiol-treated females to show accelerated extinction were those given distal preexposure to estradiol in Experiment 1. These data do not support a learning-associated hypothesis and only partially support an illness-associated hypothesis. The failure to find accelerated extinction following proximal preexposure may reflect an inappropriate choice of the parameters used in the experiment or a difference in the stimulus properties of LiCl and estradiol that allow each to serve as conditioning and preexposure agents in conditioned taste avoidance paradigms [corrected].  相似文献   

7.
Chlorpromazine injected into the amygdala, septum, or caudate delayed the acquisition of a one-way active avoidance response. Injections into nine other brain areas were inactive. Following a standard dose of chlorpromazine at its ED50 for delaying avoidance acquisition, tissue levels of chlorpromazine from those animals displaying reduced acquisition were significantly higher in the caudate and amygdala than from animals not demonstrating a drug effect.  相似文献   

8.
Both [Leu]enkephalin and DPen2-[DPen5]enkephalin, a delta opioid receptor selective analog of [Leu]enkephalin, impaired acquisition of an automated shelf-jump response in rats. A similar level of impairment was produced by equimolar doses of the two enkephalins. As is seen for [Leu]enkephalin when tested in a one-way active avoidance task, the dose-response function for the impairment produced by DPen2-[DPen5]enkephalin in the automated shelf-jump task is U-shaped. These results, together with our previous findings that DPen2-[DPen5]enkephalin and [Leu]enkephalin both impair acquisition of a one-way active avoidance response in mice, and that [Leu]enkephalin impairs acquisition of that same response in rats, support our suggestion that delta opioid receptors are implicated in the effects of [Leu]enkephalin on conditioning. In addition, these results indicate that the involvement of delta opioid receptors in acquisition impairment extends to two species of rodents and to two different avoidance conditioning tasks.  相似文献   

9.
The lateral habenula (LHb) is an epithalamic structure involved in signaling reward omission and aversive stimuli, and it inhibits dopaminergic neurons during motivated behavior. Less is known about LHb involvement in the acquisition and retrieval of avoidance learning. Our previous studies indicated that brief electrical stimulation of the LHb, time-locked to the avoidance of aversive footshock (presumably during the positive affective “relief” state that occurs when an aversive outcome is averted), inhibited the acquisition of avoidance learning. In the present study, we used the same paradigm to investigate different frequencies of LHb stimulation. The effect of 20 Hz vs. 50 Hz vs. 100 Hz stimulation was investigated during two phases, either during acquisition or retrieval in Mongolian gerbils. The results indicated that 50 Hz, but not 20 Hz, was sufficient to produce a long-term impairment in avoidance learning, and was somewhat more effective than 100 Hz in this regard. None of the stimulation parameters led to any effects on retrieval of avoidance learning, nor did they affect general motor activity. This suggests that, at frequencies in excess of the observed tonic firing rates of LHb neurons (>1–20 Hz), LHb stimulation may serve to interrupt the consolidation of new avoidance memories. However, these stimulation parameters are not capable of modifying avoidance memories that have already undergone extensive consolidation.  相似文献   

10.
Three days after the subcutaneous implant of a dexamethasone pellet, which depletes both corticosterone and ACTH, normal rats showed impaired acquisition of a two-way avoidance task. Rats who had received systemic 6-hydroxydopamine at birth to lesion the forebrain noradrenergic terminals from the locus coeruleus did not show this impairment. After a single injection of metyrapone, which inhibits corticosterone synthesis and increases ACTH release, both intact and norepinephrine (NE)-depleted rats showed impaired avoidance acquisition. After the seventh injection, however, acquisition in normal rats was no longer impaired by the drug while the NE-depleted rats were still deficient. These results indicate that the simple combination of forebrain NE loss with reduced corticosterone levels does not necessarily retard avoidance acquisition. Rather, they suggest that the NE efferents from the locus coeruleus are essential for the brain's adaptation to at least some behavioral consequences of changes in the circulating level of ACTH.  相似文献   

11.
12.
Neonatal male rats were injected with 1.25 mg of testosterone propionate (TP) and compared with oil-injected controls on the acquisition of an active and passive avoidance response at 25 days of age. The TP treated animals acquired the active avoidance response significantly faster than controls, but no differences were found between groups tested on the step-down passive avoidance task. The active-avoidance paradigm was repeated at 70 days of age, with experimental and control animals receiving the same neonatal treatment as the prepubescent subjects. Again the TP group showed facilitated acquisition of the active avoidance response. The TP treatment also produced an increase in activity levels and aversion threshold to footshock in the prepubescent animals. Therefore the active avoidance effect may be interpreted more parsimoniously as a reflection of these latter effects, rather than learning per se.  相似文献   

13.
Adult male rats subjected to a two-way avoidance task emitted ultrasonic vocalizations (20-30 kHz) both during the presentation of the conditioned stimulus and the intertrial interval. The rate of ultrasonic calling decreased during the 75-trial session indicating that acquisition of the conditioned avoidance response (CAR) was inversely correlated with the rate of vocalization. The rate of acquisition of the CAR was most rapid in those rats that did not emit any vocalization during learning. These data suggest that ultrasonic calling during stressful situations may be sensitive indicator of underlying emotional states that interfere with the acquisition of a complex task.  相似文献   

14.
The inter-relationship between central vasopressin and the pineal gland in the modulation of active avoidance behavior was investigated. In sham-operated (SO) rats, intracerebroventricular (i.c.v) application of 10 ng arginine vasopressin (AVP) after both the last acquisition and the first extinction trials prolonged the extinction of the active avoidance response; application of 50 ng of the V1 antagonist, d(CH2)5Tyr(Me)AVP (AAVP) was without effect in both experiments. In contrast to the SO in pinealectomized (PX) rats neither AVP nor AAVP influenced the extinction of the avoidance response. Intraseptal infusion of 200 pg AVP or 5 ng AAVP either after the last acquisition or the first extinction trial was without effect in both SO and PX rats. Comparison of the acquisition trials revealed no differences between SO and PX rats.  相似文献   

15.
The interstrain differences in passive avoidance conditioning were studied in male and female KHA (Koltushi High Avoidance) and KLA (Koltushi Low Avoidance) rats. These strains were selected for the rate of acquisition of active avoidance in a shuttle box. It was shown that the passive avoidance was substantially better acquired in the KLA strain than in the KHA. In females KHA rats the capability for passive avoidance conditioning depended on the estrus phase: the conditioning was impossible in proestrus.  相似文献   

16.
Disgust can be thought of as an affective system that has evolved to detect signs of pathogens, parasite and toxins as well as to stimulate behaviors that reduce the risk of their acquisition. Disgust incorporates social cognitive mechanisms to regulate exposure to and, or anticipate and avoid exposure to pathogens and toxins. Social cognition entails the acquisition of social information about others (ie, social recognition) and from others (ie, social learning). This involves recognizing and assessing other individuals and the pathogen/parasite/contamination/toxin threat they pose and deciding about when and how to interact with and, or avoid them. Social cognition provides a frame‐work for examining the expression of disgust and the associated neurobiological mechanisms. Here, we briefly consider the relations between social cognition and pathogen/parasite/toxin avoidance behaviors. We briefly discuss aspects of: (1) the odor mediated social recognition of actual and potentially infected individuals and the impact of parasite/pathogen threat on disgust mate and social partner choice; (2) the roles of “out‐groups” (strangers, unfamiliar individuals) and “in‐groups” (familiar individuals) in the expression of disgust and pathogen avoidance behaviors; (3) individual and social learning of disgust and empathy for disgust; (4) toxin elicited disgust and anticipatory disgust; (5) the neurobiological mechanisms, and in particular the roles of the nonapeptide, oxytocin and estrogenic mechanism associated with social cognition and the expression of disgust. These findings on the social neuroscience of disgust have a direct bearing on our understanding of the roles of disgust in shaping human and nonhuman social behavior.  相似文献   

17.
The use of reversible lesion techniques in memory research was pioneered in the laboratory of Jan Bures and Olga Buresova. We use the occasion of Jan's 75th birthday to briefly review the experimental utility of this approach. Two experiments from our current research are reported in which reversible lesioning methods are used to ask otherwise experimentally untenable questions about memory retrieval. The first experiment used intra-hippocampal injections of tetrodotoxin to temporarily inactivate the hippocampus during retrieval of a well-learned place avoidance navigation memory. This revealed that the hippocampus is necessary for place avoidance retrieval but that the extinction of place avoidance can occur independently of retrieving the memory and intact hippocampal function. The second experiment used KCl-induced cortical spreading depression in an interhippocampal transfer paradigm to demonstrate that a Y-maze memory that is learned by only one cortical hemisphere can be made to transfer to the other hemisphere by forcing the rat to swim, a unique stressful experience that occurred in a different apparatus, different behavioral context, and involved different behaviors than the Y-maze training. This demonstrates, we believe for the first time behaviorally, that memories can be activated outside of the behavioral context of their acquisition and expression in rats.  相似文献   

18.
Sucrose consumption by male and female rats during active avoidance acquisition was measured in two rat strains: KLA (Koltushi low avoidance) and KHA (Koltushi high avoidance) selected for divergent performance in a shuttlebox. Under resting condition, there were no interstrain difference in sucrose consumption by males, but KHA females consumed significantly less sucrose than KLA females. Active avoidance acquisition during five consecutive days decreased sucrose consumption in KLA males and did not change sucrose consumption in KHA males. Within a week after exposure to the stress, the sucrose consumption by KLA males returned to its normal values, and KHA males consumed significantly more sucrose. The active avoidance conditioning did not affect sucrose consumption in females of both strains. Substitution of 32% solution for 4% produced on the first day a sharp decrease in sucrose consumption in males of both strains, while females sharply increased consumption of the diluted solution over the next four days of observation. During this time, males returned to consumption of the same volume of the solution despite its decreased concentration. The findings suggest that the exposure to the escapable stress induces the negative affect only in KLA males.  相似文献   

19.
Male Wistar rats sustaining prefrontal cortex aspiration or sham operation at 6 days or 30 days of age were submitted to the following behavioural tests: open-field, acquisition and retention of two-way active as well as passive avoidance tasks. In the open-field the locomotor activity proved enhanced in all the aspirated animals and this enhancement lasted for 30 days. In the two-day active avoidance task, an acquisition deficit was observed in both aspirated groups; but when retrained one month later, they were able to acquire the avoidance task like sham-operated rats and no difference appeared between the groups aspirated at 6 or at 30 days of age. Concerning the passive avoidance task, no difference could be detected between aspirated and sham-operated animals of both groups except that the rats aspirated at an early age (6 days) seemed to display a better avoidance ability in the retention test. These behavioural alterations (hyperactivity and impairment of the acquisition of the 2-way active avoidance) resulted from the prefrontal cortex aspiration, at whatever age this aspiration was performed (6 days or 30 days). They disappeared after a postoperative recovery period of about one month, as evidenced by this longitudinal study.  相似文献   

20.
The effects of cholecystokinin octapeptide sulfate ester (CCK-8-SE) and unsulfated cholecystokinin (CCK-8-NS) were studied following intraventricular administration on active avoidance and conditioned feeding behaviour of rats. In the CCK-8-NS and CCK-8-SE treated animals the acquisition of active avoidance and conditioned feeding behaviour were considerably impaired compared to the control; furthermore, these peptides caused a facilitated extinction of active avoidance and conditioned feeding behaviour. The data suggest that cholecystokinin octapeptide is capable of modifying the fear and hunger motivated behaviours of rats.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号