首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
The production of movement in a simple reaction time task can be separated into two time periods: the foreperiod, which is thought to include preparatory processes, and the reaction time interval, which includes initiation processes. To better understand these processes, transcranial magnetic stimulation has been used to probe corticospinal excitability at various time points during response preparation and initiation. Previous research has shown that excitability decreases prior to the “go” stimulus and increases following the “go”; however these two time frames have been examined independently. The purpose of this study was to measure changes in CE during both the foreperiod and reaction time interval in a single experiment, relative to a resting baseline level. Participants performed a button press movement in a simple reaction time task and excitability was measured during rest, the foreperiod, and the reaction time interval. Results indicated that during the foreperiod, excitability levels quickly increased from baseline with the presentation of the warning signal, followed by a period of stable excitability leading up to the “go” signal, and finally a rapid increase in excitability during the reaction time interval. This excitability time course is consistent with neural activation models that describe movement preparation and response initiation.  相似文献   

2.
Brain potentials were recorded from 12 normal subjects engaged in an auditory target detection task (target stimulus probability of 0.2, stimulus rate of 1 every 2 sec) when instructions were (1) to press a response button with the thumb of the dominant hand to each target or (2) to keep a mental count of each target. A pre-stimulus slow negative potential was identified before every stimulus except non-targets immediately after targets. The amplitude of the pre-stimulus negativity was significantly affected by task instructions and was up to 4 times larger during the button press than the mental count condition. In contrast, the amplitudes and latencies of the event-related components (N100, P200, N200 and P300), when slow potentials were removed by filtering, were not different as a function of press or count instructions. The immediately preceding stimulus sequence affected both the amplitude and onset latency of the pre-stimulus negativity; both measures increased as the number of preceding non-targets increased. The amplitude of the pre-stimulus negative shift to targets also increased significantly as RT speed decreased. The major portion of the pre-stimulus negative potential is considered a readiness potential (RP) reflecting preparations to make a motor response. The amplitude of the RP during the target detection task did not significantly lateralize in contrast to the RP accompanying self-paced movements.  相似文献   

3.
The neural pathways for generating willed actions have been increasingly investigated since the famous pioneering work by Benjamin Libet on the nature of free will. To better understand what differentiates the brain states underlying willed and forced behaviours, we performed a study of chosen and forced actions over a binary choice scenario. Magnetoencephalography recordings were obtained from six subjects during a simple task in which the subject presses a button with the left or right finger in response to a cue that either (1) specifies the finger with which the button should be pressed or (2) instructs the subject to press a button with a finger of their own choosing. Three independent analyses were performed to investigate the dynamical patterns of neural activity supporting willed and forced behaviours during the preparatory period preceding a button press. Each analysis offered similar findings in the temporal and spatial domains and in particular, a high accuracy in the classification of single trials was obtained around 200 ms after cue presentation with an overall average of 82%. During this period, the majority of the discriminatory power comes from differential neural processes observed bilaterally in the parietal lobes, as well as some differences in occipital and temporal lobes, suggesting a contribution of these regions to willed and forced behaviours.  相似文献   

4.
In 30 healthy subjects and 32 patients after the first episode of schizophrenia 19 channel-EEG was recorded during visual presentation of a random sequence of words and pseudo-words. In the first series of the experiments, subjects had to read the presented verbal stimuli, in the second series they had to press a button when seeing a word, and in the third series they were instructed to press the button when seeing a pseudo-word. We studied components N170, P300 and N400. In the group of healthy subjects, the amplitude of N170 increased to words in the situation of their relevance, which corresponds to the "recognition potential", whereas in the group of patients, the amplitude of N170 increased to pseudo-words when they were relevant. So it was a paradoxical response. The amplitude of the ERP later waves (P300 and N400) in the group of schizophrenic patients was smaller and the relevance effect was impaired when the target stimuli were pseudo-words. However, the incongruity effect consisting in an increase in N400 amplitude to a non-target stimulus remained intact in patients.  相似文献   

5.
Subjects viewed letters presented at 2 sec intervals and prepared a fast button press whenever an “O” appeared. If the next letter was an “X” the button press was executed (Go signal), but if the letter was a non-X character (T, H, Z) suppression of the response was required (NoGo cue). NoGo signals elicited a P300-like wave that was larger at central and frontal scalp sites contralateral to the prepared movement, compared to P300s elicited by Go cues which were symmetric about the sagittal midline and dominant at parietal sites. Subtraction of preparatory CNVs from the NoGo P300 did not remove differences in scalp topography, or reduce the amplitude of the NoGo P300 to that seen following control letters that required perceptual identification but did not call for suppression of prepared motor responses. Principal components analysis identified a middle positive wave following X-alone control stimuli whose topography resembled the NoGo P300. These findings suggest that the source of augmented NoGo P300s is a generator involved with sensorimotor inhibition. We discuss the mechanism of P300 waves and evidence linking these waves with inhibition in other task arrangements.  相似文献   

6.
In this study, we investigated a motor strategy for increasing the amplitude of movement in rapid extensions at the elbow joint. This study focused on the changes in a triphasic electromyographic (EMG) pattern, i.e., the first agonist burst (AG1), the second agonist burst (AG2) and the antagonist burst (ANT), for increasing the amplitude of movement required after the initiation of movement. Subjects performed 40° (Basic task) and 80° of extension (Wide task). These tasks were performed under two conditions; performing a predetermined task (SF condition) and performing a task in response to a visual stimulus immediately after movement commencement (ST condition). Kinematic parameters and EMG activity from the agonist (triceps brachii) and the antagonist (biceps brachii) muscles were recorded. As a result, the onset latency of AG1 and AG2 and the duration of AG1 were longer under the ST condition than the SF condition. No difference was observed between the SF and ST condition with respect to ANT activity. It is concluded that the motor strategy for increasing the amplitude of movement after the initiation of movement was to control the movement velocity and the timing to stop movement by the coactivation duration of AG1 and ANT and to stop the desired position accurately by AG2 activity.  相似文献   

7.
The role of relative spike timing on sensory coding and stochastic dynamics of small pulse-coupled oscillator networks is investigated physiologically and mathematically, based on the small biological eye network of the marine invertebrate Hermissenda. Without network interactions, the five inhibitory photoreceptors of the eye network exhibit quasi-regular rhythmic spiking; in contrast, within the active network, they display more irregular spiking but collective network rhythmicity. We investigate the source of this emergent network behavior first analyzing the role of relative input to spike–timing relationships in individual cells. We use a stochastic phase oscillator equation to model photoreceptor spike sequences in response to sequences of inhibitory current pulses. Although spike sequences can be complex and irregular in response to inputs, we show that spike timing is better predicted if relative timing of spikes to inputs is accounted for in the model. Further, we establish that greater noise levels in the model serve to destroy network phase-locked states that induce non-monotonic stimulus rate-coding, as predicted in Butson and Clark (J Neurophysiol 99:146–154, 2008a; J Neurophysiol 99:155–165, 2008b). Hence, rate-coding can function better in noisy spiking cells relative to non-noisy cells. We then study how relative input to spike–timing dynamics of single oscillators contribute to network-level dynamics. Relative timing interactions in the network sharpen the stimulus window that can trigger a spike, affecting stimulus encoding. Also, we derive analytical inter-spike interval distributions of cells in the model network, revealing that irregular Poisson-like spike emission and collective network rhythmicity are emergent properties of network dynamics, consistent with experimental observations. Our theoretical results generate experimental predictions about the nature of spike patterns in the Hermissenda eye.  相似文献   

8.
In experiments on awake cats, we recorded the activity of 79 putative serotonergic (STE) neurons localized within the region of the brainstem dorsal and anterior central raphe nuclei. The animals were trained to perform a self-initiated (voluntary) movement, to press a pedal by the forelimb; an additional limitation was to perform the movement not earlier than after a definite time interval. Changes in the activity of STE neurons related to the preparation for and performance of the movement and reactions to presentation of a feedback conditioning signal preceding the reward and receipt of the food reward were most clearly manifested. More than 50% of the units changed their activity before the movement initiation. Most neurons responded to presentation of a positive conditioning signal by phasic activation, while a negative signal informing them of the absence of the reward evoked considerably weaker reactions. We hypothesize that reactions of STE neurons forestalling the movement initiation can provide activation of the neocortex necessary for the movement performance within a preset time interval. Activating and inhibitory reactions observed within the period of expectation of a feedback conditioning signal and developing after presentation of this signal can be related to a noticeable role of the STE system in the formation of memory engrams and development of emotional states.  相似文献   

9.
The spike responses of the motor cortex neurons (area 4) associated with forelimb movement were studied in awake cats earlier trained to perform placing motor reactions. Responses produced by the same neurons were compared in two situations: 1) when a sound-click conditioning stimulus (CS) was applied in isolation; 2) when a CS followed a preliminary warning stimulus (WS), a light flash, with a 100–1000 msec delay. During the reflex initiation by combined action of the WS and CS, response components that occurred prior to the placing movement (PM) performance under isolated CS action weakened and arrived 50–150 msec later; yet, response components that appeared in the same situation simultaneously with PM onset or later remained unchanged. PM latent periods were not changed when WS was applied. The temporal interval between WS and CS was characterized by depression of neuronal activity; depression duration was determined by the interstimulus delay. It is conceivable that the described transformations in spike responses of cortical neurons occurred due to changes in the sensory direction of the animal's attention; this direction, in all cases, is a crucial factor in the formation of neuronal activity in the cortex.Translated from Neirofiziologiya, Vol. 25, No. 1, pp. 21–27, January–February, 1993.t  相似文献   

10.
On 42 subject three experimental series were carried out: in the first (12 persons) and second (24 persons) series the presented interval was limited by two short clicks, in the third series (6 persons)--by electrocutaneous stimuli. Duration of the stimuli was 1 ms. There were three regimes of work in the first and third series: the intervals successively increased from 100 to 500 ms with a step of 100 ms (1), decreased from 5000 to 100 ms (2) or varied in a random order (3). In the second series only the regime 3 was applied. In all series the method of temporal intervals reproduction was used. The means of the reproduction varied: in the second and third series the interval was reproduced by button pressing according to the presented duration: in the first series the end of the interval was marked by a short button push, and the beginning was the moment of the second stimulus presentation. With the first means a considerable overreproduction was observed of the presented duration at all intervals and all regimes. At the second and third series a phasic character of the reproduction duration was noted: up to 1000 ms the interval mostly was overestimated, over 2000 ms--it was significantly underestimated. It is suggested that as the estimation of the temporal interval implies some motor reaction, the afferent flow of signals from the active muscles can change the value of the reproduced duration. In the first series, the subjects probably do not take into account the time necessary for the realized signal perception.  相似文献   

11.
In dogs with electrodes chronically implanted in the hippocampus, the conditioned activity, heart rate frequency and spectral characteristics of the hippocampal theta-rhythm were studied in conditions of irregular presentation after a preparatory stimulus now of alimentary and now of defensive triggering conditioned stimuli. It is shown that uncertainty of alimentary or defensive reinforcement is a stronger emotiogenic factor than the action of the triggering defensive conditioned stimulus. Changes in electrical hippocampal activity and autonomous activity depend, along with other factors, on forecasted volume of the forthcoming motor activity. The speed of instrumental conditioned reflexes formation correlates with the hippocampal theta-rhythm frequency, typical for the given animal.  相似文献   

12.
We examined the local field potential of the hippocampus to monitor brain states during a conditional discrimination task, in order to elucidate the relationship between ongoing brain states and a conditioned motor reflex. Five 10-week-old Wistar/ST male rats underwent a serial feature positive conditional discrimination task in eyeblink conditioning using a preceding light stimulus as a conditional cue for reinforced trials. In this task, a 2-s light stimulus signaled that the following 350-ms tone (conditioned stimulus) was reinforced with a co-terminating 100-ms periorbital electrical shock. The interval between the end of conditional cue and the onset of the conditioned stimulus was 4±1 s. The conditioned stimulus was not reinforced when the light was not presented. Animals successfully utilized the light stimulus as a conditional cue to drive differential responses to the identical conditioned stimulus. We found that presentation of the conditional cue elicited hippocampal theta oscillations, which persisted during the interval of conditional cue and the conditioned stimulus. Moreover, expression of the conditioned response to the tone (conditioned stimulus) was correlated with the appearance of theta oscillations immediately before the conditioned stimulus. These data support hippocampal involvement in the network underlying a conditional discrimination task in eyeblink conditioning. They also suggest that the preceding hippocampal activity can determine information processing of the tone stimulus in the cerebellum and its associated circuits.  相似文献   

13.

Background

Behavioral studies have provided evidence for an action–sentence compatibility effect (ACE) that suggests a coupling of motor mechanisms and action-sentence comprehension. When both processes are concurrent, the action sentence primes the actual movement, and simultaneously, the action affects comprehension. The aim of the present study was to investigate brain markers of bidirectional impact of language comprehension and motor processes.

Methodology/Principal Findings

Participants listened to sentences describing an action that involved an open hand, a closed hand, or no manual action. Each participant was asked to press a button to indicate his/her understanding of the sentence. Each participant was assigned a hand-shape, either closed or open, which had to be used to activate the button. There were two groups (depending on the assigned hand-shape) and three categories (compatible, incompatible and neutral) defined according to the compatibility between the response and the sentence. ACEs were found in both groups. Brain markers of semantic processing exhibited an N400-like component around the Cz electrode position. This component distinguishes between compatible and incompatible, with a greater negative deflection for incompatible. Motor response elicited a motor potential (MP) and a re-afferent potential (RAP), which are both enhanced in the compatible condition.

Conclusions/Significance

The present findings provide the first ACE cortical measurements of semantic processing and the motor response. N400-like effects suggest that incompatibility with motor processes interferes in sentence comprehension in a semantic fashion. Modulation of motor potentials (MP and RAP) revealed a multimodal semantic facilitation of the motor response. Both results provide neural evidence of an action-sentence bidirectional relationship. Our results suggest that ACE is not an epiphenomenal post-sentence comprehension process. In contrast, motor-language integration occurring during the verb onset supports a genuine and ongoing brain motor-language interaction.  相似文献   

14.
We recorded cortical potentials evoked by painful CO2 laser stimulation (pain SEP), employing an oddball paradigm in an effort to demonstrate event-related potentials (ERP) associated with pain. In 12 healthy subjects, frequent (standard) pain stimuli (probability 0.8) were delivered to one side of the dorsum of the left hand while rare (target) pain stimuli (probability 0.2) were delivered to the other side of the same hand. Subjects were instructed to perform either a mental count or button press in response to the target stimuli. Two early components (N2 and P2) of the pain SEP demonstrated a Cz maximal distribution, and showed no difference in latency, amplitude or scalp topography between the oddball conditions or between response tasks. In addition, another positive component (P3) following the P2 was recorded maximally at Pz only in response to the target stimuli with a peak latency of 593 msec for the count task and 560 msec for the button press task. Its scalp topography was the same as that for electric and auditory P3. The longer latency of pain P3 can be explained not only by its slower impulse conduction but also by the effects of task difficulty in the oddball paradigm employing the pain stimulus compared with electric and auditory stimulus paradigms. It is concluded that the P3 for the pain modality is mainly related to a cognitive process and corresponds to the P3 of electric and auditory evoked responses, whereas both N2 and P2 are mainly pain-related components.  相似文献   

15.
When a flashed stimulus is followed by a backward mask, subjects fail to perceive it unless the target-mask interval exceeds a threshold duration of about 50 ms. Models of conscious access postulate that this threshold is associated with the time needed to establish sustained activity in recurrent cortical loops, but the brain areas involved and their timing remain debated. We used high-density recordings of event-related potentials (ERPs) and cortical source reconstruction to assess the time course of human brain activity evoked by masked stimuli and to determine neural events during which brain activity correlates with conscious reports. Target-mask stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) was varied in small steps, allowing us to ask which ERP events show the characteristic nonlinear dependence with SOA seen in subjective and objective reports. The results separate distinct stages in mask-target interactions, indicating that a considerable amount of subliminal processing can occur early on in the occipito-temporal pathway (<250 ms) and pointing to a late (>270 ms) and highly distributed fronto-parieto-temporal activation as a correlate of conscious reportability.  相似文献   

16.
Performing tasks activates relevant brain regions in adults while deactivating task‐irrelevant regions. Here, using a well‐controlled motor task, we explored how deactivation is shaped during typical human development and whether deactivation is related to task performance. Healthy right‐handed children (8–11 years), adolescents (12–15 years), and young adults (20–24 years; 20 per group) underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging with their eyes closed while performing a repetitive button‐press task with their right index finger in synchronization with a 1‐Hz sound. Deactivation in the ipsilateral sensorimotor cortex (SM1), bilateral visual and auditory (cross‐modal) areas, and bilateral default mode network (DMN) progressed with development. Specifically, ipsilateral SM1 and lateral occipital deactivation progressed prominently between childhood and adolescence, while medial occipital (including primary visual) and DMN deactivation progressed from adolescence to adulthood. In adults, greater cross‐modal deactivation in the bilateral primary visual cortices was associated with higher button‐press timing accuracy relative to the sound. The region‐specific deactivation progression in a developmental period may underlie the gradual promotion of sensorimotor function segregation required in the task. Task‐induced deactivation might have physiological significance regarding suppressed activity in task‐irrelevant regions. Furthermore, cross‐modal deactivation develops to benefit some aspects of task performance in adults.  相似文献   

17.
We have previously shown that, in early stages of Parkinson's disease (PD), patients with higher reaction times are also more impaired in visual sequence learning, suggesting that movement preparation shares resources with the learning of visuospatial sequences. Here, we ascertained whether, in patients with PD, the pattern of the neural correlates of attentional processes of movement planning predict sequence learning and working memory abilities. High density Electroencephalography (EEG, 256 electrodes) was recorded in 19 patients with PD performing reaching movements in a choice reaction time paradigm. Patients were also tested with Digit Span and performed a visuomotor sequence learning task that has an important declarative learning component. We found that attenuation of alpha/beta oscillatory activity before the stimulus presentation in frontoparietal regions significantly correlated with reaction time in the choice reaction time task, similarly to what we had previously found in normal subjects. In addition, such activity significantly predicted the declarative indices of sequence learning and the scores in the Digit Span task. These findings suggest that some motor and non motor PD signs might have common neural bases, and thus, might have a similar response to the same behavioral therapy. In addition, these results might help in designing and testing the efficacy of novel rehabilitative approaches to improve specific aspects of motor performance in PD and other neurological disorders.  相似文献   

18.
Dynamics of neuronal interaction recorded by microelectrodes were examined in 90 arrays of cells of the human thalamic reticular nucleus (Rt) during stereotaxic surgical procedures. Cooperative interaction between adjacent neurons was found to occur in neuronal arrays after presentation of verbal (or sensory-cum-acoustic) functionally significant stimulus (FSS) as well as at stages of initiation and performance of goal-directed movement. Specialized dynamics were noted in the pattern of interaction between neuronal arrays of two types (A and B) with irregular background activity and 2–5 Hz bursting rhythm (types A and B respectively). This dynamic local neuronal interaction correlates with the stage of significant verbal stimulus presentation and that of performing goal-directed movements. The matching transient correlation between activity of A and B cell arrays reflects matching operation of two sequences of regulatory and control processes involved in processing of functionally significant verbal (or sensory) information and performance of goal-directed movement.Institute of Chemical Physics, Academy of Sciences of the USSR, Moscow Institute of Neurosurgery, Academy of Medical Sciences of the USSR, Moscow. Translated from Neirofiziologiya, Vol. 22, No. 4, pp. 451–459, June–July, 1990.  相似文献   

19.
Heart rate, EEG, and motor responses were recorded following presentation of a series of 6–10 sound stimuli (2.5-s tones of 1000, 4000, and 250 Hz, 70 dB, interstimulus intervals 18–25 s) in neonates aged 9 to 22 weeks during stage 2–3 sleep. The infants (17 of 19) revealed heart rate (HR) changes in response to tone stimuli that consisted in an expanded form of three phases: (1) short-latency (at 1 s after tone presentation) HR deceleration, (2) HR acceleration with a maximum at 3–5 s, and (3) late HR deceleration at 6–9 s of the poststimulus interval. The occurrence rate of the first two phases of cardiac response is relatively constant during a series of stimuli, whereas the likelihood of late HR deceleration is the highest following the first tone presentation and decreases significantly when the stimulus is repeated. Differences in the dynamics and statistical analysis allow a relative independence of all the three response phases to be suggested. The HR acceleration phase is dramatically enhanced in association with the motor response elicited by the sound stimulus. The late HR deceleration phase occurs not only after the first presentation of stimuli, but also when they are repeated if they evoke EEG reaction (vertex potentials) in response to both the beginning and end of the tone sound. Possible mechanisms of the three phases of poststimulus HR changes are: the vagal cardiac reflex associated with the acoustic (adaptive) reflex, activation of sympathetic efferents in combination with the startle reflex, and secondary vagal deceleration of sinus rhythm likely to be associated with the processes of perception (detection) of a “novel” stimulus and to serve as an indirect sign of an orienting reaction.  相似文献   

20.
We recorded the activity of cerebellar Purkinje cells (PCs), primary motor cortical (M1) neurons, and limb EMG signals while monkeys executed a sequential reaching and button pressing task. PC simple spike discharge generally correlated well with the activity of one or more forelimb muscles. Surprisingly, given the inhibitory projection of PCs, only about one quarter of the correlations were negative. The largest group of neurons burst during movement and were positively correlated with EMG signals, while another significant group burst and were negatively correlated. Among the PCs that paused during movement most were negatively correlated with EMG. The strength of these various correlations was somewhat weaker, on average, than equivalent correlations between M1 neurons and EMG signals. On the other hand, there were no significant differences in the timing of the onset of movement related discharge among these groups of PCs, or between the PCs and M1 neurons. PC discharge was modulated largely in phase, or directly out of phase, with muscle activity. The nearly synchronous activation of PCs and muscles yielded positive correlations, despite the fact that the synaptic effect of the PC discharge is inhibitory. The apparent function of this inhibition is to restrain activity in the limb premotor network, shaping it into a spatiotemporal pattern that is appropriate for controlling the many muscles that participate in this task. The observed timing suggests that the cerebellar cortex learns to modulate PC discharge predictively. Through the cerebellar nucleus, this PC signal is combined with an underlying cerebral cortical signal. In this manner the cerebellum refines the descending command as compared with the relatively crude version generated when the cerebellum is damaged.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号