首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
The reproductive behaviour of the blue gourami, Trichogaster trichopterus, was examined in 15 bisexual pairs and 22 trios maintained in 75-litre aquaria for 23 days. During this time, 74 spawning episodes occurred among 26 different pairs and trios. Sixty-nine of these spawnings were directly observed. Both males and females were capable of mating repeatedly with the same or different partners with as little as 20 to 24 h separating successive spawnings. Females often mated with two males in the same afternoon, while only one male succeeded in mating with two females in one day. It is concluded that blue gouramis probably do not form pair bonds and that both sexes will mate with differet partners when the physical environment permits two or more males to maintain territories in the same aquarium.  相似文献   

2.
The sensory basis of group cohesion in the weak-electric fish Gnathonemus petersii was investigated in a circular tank with groups of four fish each, interacting through a wide-meshed plastic screen with intact or operated conspecifics, or with other stimulus objects. We confined these stimuli to one or two peripheral holding compartments. The response measures were obtained from the free swimming fish and included (1) the time the fish spent together as a group, (2) the time they spent in front of the holding compartments, (3) the circular distribution of the fish's positions, and (4) the mean nearest neighbour distances. Under empty compartment conditions, four different groups were tested, consisting of either (1) intact, electrically active fish, or (2) electrically ‘silent’ fish (with their electric organ surgically rendered inoperative), or (3) blind, or (4) ‘silent’ and blind animals. The loss of either sensory modality, vision or feedback from electric organ discharge, led to changes of comparable size, decreasing the time spent as a group and increasing the mean nearest neighbour distance. In fish lacking both modalities, group cohesion was further impaired. With stimuli present in one or both holding compartments, the strength of social attraction depended on the nature of the stimulus: the more intact stimulus conspecifics were present, the more densely did the fish group in front of the stimulus compartment. ‘Wired-in’ electric organ discharges (simulating waveform and intensity) and electrically ‘silent’ fish were equally attractive, but only half as attractive as intact fish. Blind free swimming fish aggregated with intact and also with ‘silent’ conspecifics. Under dim light conditions, group cohesion was predominantly, though not exclusively, affected by electrosensory feedback from the electric organ discharge and visual input. Mechanical and olfactory cues may also be involved.  相似文献   

3.
4.
The role of sight in the dominance hierarchy of groups of eight pigs was studied by the induction of temporary blindness using opaque contact lenses or hoods which covered the face. The effect of blindness was examined in three social situations: (i) the simultaneous introduction of eight previously unacquainted, blinded pigs; (ii) groups of pigs with a known social order were blinded and (iii) a single pig within a group of known social order was blinded. The contact lenses had a minimal effect on the hierarchy formation whilst both the lenses and the hoods had no effect on the established groups. The hoods prevented hierarchy formation amongst the unacquainted pigs. It is suggested that sight alone is not necessary for hierarchy maintenance and that the hoods stopped the formation of a hierarchy by an effect other than the deprivation of sight, e.g. by covering pheromone producing areas.  相似文献   

5.
Gryllus campestris has two types of development: one consists of rapid growth followed by a diapause in the penultimate larval stage, the ‘protostage’, and the other of slow growth without diapause. Experiments were designed to find the details of the programme for the two types of development.Two main temperatures, 30 and 20°C, were chosen. The sensitive period of the life cycle at 20°C was detected by subjecting insects to this temperature for a determined and increasing number of days before rearing them at 30°C during the rest of larval life. Conversely, insects were reared first at 30°C for a determined and increasing number of days and then transferred to 20°C. All these experiments led to the conclusion that the sensitive period occurs in the three last larval stages before the protostage, and that the animal is capable of following only two types of development. Experiments in which the speed of growth was slowed down by rearing insects at a cooler temperature (12°C) show that the conditions of the formation programme are not simply linked to a retardation of growth.The main effect of changing the length of days is to synchronize the arrival of all the insects at the protostage before the autumn, no matter whether the data of birth was spring or summer, or whether the number of larval stages was influenced: this explains the univoltinism of this species. Further experiments consisting of changing the length of days in the opposite direction from that of the natural seasonal evolution (increasing: 10 to 16 hr light/day instead of decreasing 16 to 10 hr) show that the programme is disturbed and subsequently the morphology of the animals is affected.To understand the genetic background of the programme a non-diapausing strain was used. It is demonstrated that selection eliminates diapause when reared at 30°C as well as normal survival when reared at 20°C. Therefore, the hypothesis of a ‘unitary system’ is here proposed for these two types of development. Hybridization of the diapausing and non-diapausing strains shows that the expression of ‘diapause’ as a response to high temperature is influenced by photoperiodism; it is dominant in short days and recessive in long days.  相似文献   

6.
Field studies suggest that killdeer pairs form on the breeding grounds. Males advertise both on the ground and in the air with loud ‘killdeer’ and other calls and with scraping (‘sham’ nesting). In scrape exchanges the scraper faces away, breast dipped, tail raised and spread over the incoming bird. On the ground killdeers chase with body horizontal, back feathers ruffled and trilled calls. Fleers are silent and may end runs with sudden crouched tilting of the dorsum toward the pursuer, exposing the reddish rump. Pairs defend their nest area and young from conspecifics.  相似文献   

7.
The development of dusk and dawn migratory behaviours was investigated in French and white grunts (Haemulon flavolineatum and H. plumieri) at St. Croix, U.S. Virgin Islands. Four juvenile stages were recognized: small, medium, transitional, and large. Each stage differed in age, size, coloration, habitat preference, diel foraging patterns, and twilight migratory behaviour. Comparisons between medium (15 to 30 mm long, 30 to 50 days old) and large juveniles (40 to 120 mm, 80 to 700 days) were emphasized. Medium juveniles occurred in small, diurnally feeding groups near sea urchins in the sand halo around a reef. Group composition varied during the day. They migrated at 15 min after sunset, moving hesitantly from halo to grassbed. Migration routes remained constant over a month, but differed over two years. Large juveniles fed nocturnally, formed daytime resting schools over coral heads, and migrated at 25 min after sunset. Compared with medium fish, resting site constancy was greater during a day, migration activities were significantly less variable, and migration routes remained relatively constant for two years.Ontogenetic differences in constancy of daytime school locales and migration routes may result from learning, facilitated perhaps by greater overlap of age cohorts in large juveniles. Other differences in behavioural variability may result from ontogenetic development of the visual apparatus, plus stabilizing selection due to greater predation on smaller, behaviourally variable fish.  相似文献   

8.
9.
Social organization and reproductive behaviour of Abudefduf zonatus were studied at Heron Island, Great Barrier Reef. This species is closely associated with the sand and coral substrate of the inner reef flat zone. Territorial, clustering, nest-digging, breeding and parental care behaviours are described. Mature males maintain territories for several weeks or longer around empty clam shells in which periodic spawning occurs. Females and sub-adult males hold territories interspersed among those of adult males. No pair bonds are formed, and eggs are guarded only by males. Breeding is roughly synchronized among fish of one area. Clustering behaviour may contribute to breeding syndrony in local populations.  相似文献   

10.
Territorial behaviour of Rana clamitans was studied in an experimental pond containing natural vegetation and artificial shelters. Males defended territories from June through August. Five vocalizations were used in territorial advertisement and agonistic encounters. Agonistic behaviour included patrolling, splashing displays, chases, attacks and wrestling. About 23% of all encounters ended in wrestling bouts. Most bouts were less than 30 s long, but some lasted up to 45 min. Most fights were won by residents. Intruders were most successful in fights when they were larger than residents or previously had been residents of other territories. Weight loss by large males enabled some smaller males to oust residents from territories. Possible costs of fighting include energetic costs and exposure to predation. Large male body size in R. clamitans and other territorial frogs may be an adaptation for fighting.  相似文献   

11.
A characteristic electric organ discharge display in social encounters between mormyrid fish is a temporary discharge cessation. Using this response, we have investigated the useful range of electrocommunication under different water conductivity conditions in the mormyrid Brienomyrus niger. An individual fish was confined to a porous ceramic shelter tube and moved from a starting distance of 380 cm toward a similarly confined conspecific until discharge, cessation occurred. The moved fish was subsequently returned to its original, position. Water conductivity affects the peak-to-peak source voltage of the electric organ and the sensitivity of the fish's electroreceptors. Within a range of 10 to 36 000 μS/cm, the peak-to-peak amplitude of the electric organ discharge declined as a power function. At 120 μS/cm, the amplitude was 50%, and at 300μS/cm, 30% of the 10 μS/cm value. The interfish distance at which discharge cessation occurred and the associated electric field gradients were dependent on water conductivity and upon the spatial orientation of the two fish (end-to-end or parallel orientations of their shelter tubes). The respective ranges were from 135 cm and 0.02 mV/cm at 52 μS/cm (parallel orientation) to 22 cm and 0.36 mV/cm at 678 μS/cm (end-to-end orientation). When the data for both tube orientations were combined, the relationship between water conductivity (x) and the distance at which discharge cessation occurred (y) could be expressed by a power function, y=K·xa (with K=102.97 and a=?0.56). When an electrically ‘silent’ fish was moved away from its conspecific, a discharge resumption in the form of a high-frequency rebound occasionally effected changes in the other fish's discharge activity at distances up to 157 cm (with an associated electric, field gradient of 0.01 mV/cm under the lowest conductivity condition).  相似文献   

12.
Synopsis The early development of the southern mouthbrooder,Pseudocrenilabrus philander, is documented from activation until the early stages of the juvenile period. The duration of the embryonic period is about 14 days at 25°C. Development is direct and there is accelerated exogenous feeding into the embryonic period. The pattern of development and the timing of ontogenetic events and structure formation are a reflection of both internal and external environmental conditions. During mouthbrooding, oxygen uptake is facilitated by embryonic respiratory plexuses and flapping of the pectoral fins. At the time of first release from the buccal cavity, the embryos are in an advanced state of development. The switch-over from the temporary embryonic respiratory system to the adult branchial system has occurred. The yolksac serves as a supplemental source of nutrition as the embryos develop their external food-gathering abilities. The skeletal and sensory systems are sufficiently developed to allow the young to return to the safety of the female's buccal cavity. Pigmentation may provide disruptive colouration. The rate and pattern of development of another mouthbrooding cichlid,Oreochromis mossambicus, is similar to that ofP. philander despite their phylogenetic differences, and may be a consequence of similar life-history styles.  相似文献   

13.
Female gerbils were bilaterally bulbectomized, unilaterally bulbectomized or sham-operated at 2 days of age (Birth DAY = 0). As adults, the occurrence of scent marking and aggressive behavior was measured prior to and following administration of either testosterone propionate (TP) or oil. TP-treated sham operates showed increased scent marking; bilaterally bulbectomized and unilaterally bulbectomized females treated with TP did not. The display of aggressiveness increased for all females following TP. Neonatally bulbectomized animals treated with TP, however, were the most aggressive. Although androgen augments these species-typical response patterns, it seems dependent upon the integrity of the olfactory bulbs.  相似文献   

14.
15.
16.
During frontal threatening, individuals of the fire-mouth cichlid (Cichlasoma meeki) dilate their opercula, thereby demonstrating two conspicous ‘eye-spots’. In a hostile situation, a fish confronted with an opponent whose eye-spots have been experimentally removed is more likely to react with overt aggression towards the other. Fights also follow a more violent course than does a ‘normal’ fight between two intact males. A tentative explanation to these results is advanced which supposes that the eye-spots may exert an ‘intimidating’ effect upon the opponent during threat and fighting. This hypothesis is also supported by an experiment where males were confronted with a mirror. In this situation the distance between the mirror and the fish during frontal threat was significantly shorter in fishes lacking eye-spots, indicating that the intimidating effect emerging from the mirror image was diminished. In a third set of experiments when two males (one intact and one without eye-spots) competed for the only suitable territory site in an aquarium, the intact male eventually dominated the other in five of six aquariums.  相似文献   

17.
Six hand-reared great kiskadees (Pitangus sulphuratus) were given still and moving models of prey. Series A bore various combinations of eyes, tail, neck and motion; eyes and tail were the most strongly directing. Series B bore various colours and patterns, both as covering the whole model, and as covering only one-third (middle or end). The birds were not inhibited by any plain colour tested but strongly avoided patterns resembling coral snakes. Given sufficient contrast, kiskadees were attracted to the painted section of the partly painted models, regardless of location (middle or end). This attraction to the contrasting section, plus the directed responses to eyes, tail and motion, should allow kiskadees to take small vertebrates like lizards quickly and efficiently.  相似文献   

18.
The goldsinny, Ctenolabrusrupestris, on the Swedish west-coast has been shown to be a facultative cleaner with the ballan wrasse, Labrusberggylta, as the cleanee. Twenty-four cleanings were observed during 1975 to 1981. The cleaning pattern of the symbiosis is described and the low rate of cleaners within the species is discussed.  相似文献   

19.
Behaviours that always appear playful (play markers) are distinguished from behaviours that appear playful in some contexts, but not others (context-dependent play components). Age changes in the frequency of performance of both kinds of playful behaviours are described, as are age changes in the frequency with which context-dependent play components accompany play markers in baboon social interactions. Some quantitative properties of social interactions containing and lacking play markers are compared. Interactions with play markers last longer and have a higher rate of change of constituent behaviours than interactions without. Animals are also more persistent in the face of changes in their partner's behaviour in interactions with play markers. It is suggested that an attempt to produce a definition of play is not a useful enterprise, but that it is important to investigate causal mechanisms underlying behaviours that appear playful to human observers, and to clarify the relationships among those mechanisms. Data are presented suggesting that at least two sets of mechanisms, not totally separate, underly the performance of baboon social play. Functions of the behaviours controlled by these mechanisms are discussed.  相似文献   

20.
Synopsis The behaviour of free-living pairs ofAequidens vittatus was observed in Surinam, South America. An ethogram of breeding behaviour is presented, based on those observations and on aquarium-held fish. This is a biparental, substrate-guarding species that spawns on a movable platform. Sexual differentiation of parental roles was more pronounced when the young were in the embryonic interval than when they were mobile juveniles. Females spent more time near their brood, attacked juvenile conspecifics more often, and fanned and mouthed embryos more than males did. Males were more involved than females in territorial spacing of pairs. The movable spawning leaf provides support for eggs on detritus substrate and may also provide protection against predators and rapid water level changes.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号