首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Field observations were carried out on chimpanzees, pygmy chimpanzees and eastern gorillas. Since the communicative behaviors which appear in group ranging are closely related to the grouping of the animals and to the social structure, the communicative behaviors of group ranging were compared in the above three species in order to elucidate the common and different characters of their three much diversified social structures. The following conclusions were drawn: (1) the common ancestor ofGorilla andPan had the territorial call “hoot” and behavior of display, and males were antagonistic between each other in the society; (2) the common ancestral species did not have any special long distance cohesive calling: the society was a small compact one, moving on the ground; and (3) the social structure of the pygmy chimpanzee is very different from the common ancestral social structure when compared with those of the gorilla and chimpanzee, in that the pygmy chimpanzee has lost the behaviors of strong antagonistic character between adult males.  相似文献   

2.
An ethological study was conducted on a provisioned pygmy chimpanzee group in Wamba, Zaïre and was compared with the author's previous study on common chimpanzees, in Mahale Mountains with special reference to the evolution of behavior systems. The relationships between behavior patterns were investigated by analyses of the intra- or inter-individual sequential behaviors. Analysis of behavior pathways showed that “bipedal” was not found (i.e., observed frequency lower than expected value) in the course of the charging display in pygmy chimps, though it was an important element of the charging display in common chimps. Additionally, “bipedal” was found to be an initating behavior pattern in sexual behavior or dominant-subordinate behavior in pygmy chimps. These differences were related to the decrease of the role of charging display and to the increase of the roles of sexual and dominant-subordinate behaviors in pygmy chimps. Sexual behavior, “female genito-genital rubbing,” “mutual present” and frequent mounting and presenting between males were suggested to have evolved together as a system. These characteristics of behaviors correspond to the fact that pygmy chimps form more stable and larger temporary groups than common chimps.  相似文献   

3.
The characteristics of the epidermal ridge system were studied in a series of eighteen lesser or pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus). The general ridge alignments are very similar to those of the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes); Biegert ('61). On the average the pattern intensity (P.I.) of the palm configurations is considerably higher in the pygmy chimpanzee than in the chimpanzee, thus representing the highest total palm pattern intensity of all species of the Hominoidea. The sole configurations show parallel main results to those of the palm; however, the decreased sole pattern frequency of the pygmy chimpanzee is of a smaller predominance only as compared to the values of the other species of this superfamily. The preliminary data on the finger tip patterns, translated into P.I. values, are much higher than in chimpanzees and within the range of the mean values of gorillas (Brehme, '73), while those of the toes of pygmy chimpanzees seem to possess the lowest P.I. values of the African apes.  相似文献   

4.
A provisioned chimpanzee group in the Mahale Mountains was observed. The characteristic feature of chimpanzee behavior is that many types of behavior occur at one time as a bout of interactions. This can be seen in the booming situation in the wild. Similar situations were observed at the artificial feeding place. The behavior of the initiators of the interactions differed according to age, sex and social status. The chimpanzees' behavior was studied as a system. The relationships between each behavior pattern were investigated by analysis of the sequential behaviors of the same individual, and that of chains of signal-response behaviors, as well as that of behaviors which appeared when a third individual joined the initial interaction. Appeasement behaviors were more flexible, while others were more stereotyped. Part of the appeasement behavior resembled begging behavior. Appeasement behavior is thought to have a rather new phylogenetic origin. It is suggested that the various chimpanzee behaviors have evolved as a harmonious system which can be observed in the booming situation. The origin of chimpanzee society based on the evolution of their behavior system is discussed. Analysis of triplet interactions indicated that the types of triplet interaction in chimpanzees are quite different from those of multiple male group species such as macaques and baboons.  相似文献   

5.
The social group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) of Wamba   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
This is a sociological study of the pygmy chimpanzees of Wamba which have been provisioned sufficiently since 1977. The society of the pygmy chimpanzee is basically similar to that of the common chimpanzee in the flexibility in forming parties and in the existence of a definite social unit, i.e.,unit group. It also resembles in that young females play an important role in exchanging members among unit groups, thus, as transmitters of genes from group to group, while males do in integrating groups, delimiting the social boundaries of unit groups through their inter-group antagonism. Pygmy chimpanzees differ from common chimpanzees in fission and fusion pattern of parties. The former generally forms parties larger in size and more homogeneous in composition than the latter. Any party of the former has both reproductive and nursing functions, while the parties of the latter are categorized into various types according to functions and compositions. This uniformity in pygmy chimpanzee parties seems to be well maintained by the females' high sexual ability to manifest semi-continual estrus.  相似文献   

6.
7.
This paper describes competitive interaction between males of the salamandrid Cynops ensicauda popei. The sexual behaviour of the male is influenced by the presence of a second, rival, male. Competition for access to the female and other male interactions continuously interrupt ongoing courtships. Spermatophore pick-up success is lower than in encounters involving single pairs. During triad encounters, fewer spermatophores were deposited than in dyad encounters, due to interference during the creep stage of courtship. Two basic forms of male sexual interference were distinguished: ‘lure away’ during the creeping stage, from a position that is different from the direction of creep; and ‘female mimicry’, during which the male presses his snout alternately against the courting male's tail and the female's snout. In both forms, a rival male may shove one of the courting individuals away. The repertoire of sexual behaviour patterns of Cynops ensicauda popei appears to be less complex and varied than that of most Triturus species. The courtship of Cynops places less emphasis on display behaviour with tail and body, and a stronger emphasis on the creeping stage, where the male carefully leads the responsive female over a series of spermatophores, but during which rival males may interfere in a variety of ways.  相似文献   

8.
A number of primatologists have followed Coolidge (Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 18:1–57, 1933) in suggesting that 1) there are significant shape differences in scapula form between pygmy and common chimpanzees, 2) scapulae of P. paniscus resemble those of hylobatids more than do those of P. troglodytes, and 3) therefore pygmy chimpanzees may exhibit a greater component of arm-swinging and other arboreal behaviors than common chimpanzees. In this paper I utilize a comparative analysis of ontogenetic allometries of linear dimensions to determine shape differences in the scapulae of adult pygmy and common chimpanzees and to clarify size-related changes in shape resulting from ontogenetic scaling, i.e., the differential extension of common patterns of growth allometry. Results demonstrate that the scapulae of adult P. paniscus are relatively narrower (in a direction approximately perpendicular to the scapula spine) than those of P. troglodytes, supporting Coolidge's original claim. The allometric analysis further demonstrates, however, that the two chimpanzee species exhibit ontogenetic scaling for all proportions of the scapula examined. Thus, adult pygmy chimpanzees have the scapula proportions observed in small adult and subadult P. troglodytes of comparable scapula size. The implications of this finding for past claims concerning differences in locomotor behavior between the species are discussed. This work lends additional support to previous studies that have demonstrated a high frequency of ontogenetic scaling within the genus Pan and a pedomorphic or juvenilized morphology in the pygmy chimpanzee.  相似文献   

9.
Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania were discovered to show an expressive gesture using leaves, termed as “leaf-clipping display”. This behavioural signal is directed by an adult male to an estrous female as a possessive behaviour, or by an adolescent male as a courtship display, or by an estrous female to an adolescent male also as a solicitation of copulation. The signal also is used toward human observers as a signal of food-demanding. This behaviour pattern might originate in a displacement tool-making behaviour in conflict situations. The leaf-clipping display has not been observed in any other chimpanzee populations studied, and may probably be one example of the tradition drift in wild chimpanzees.  相似文献   

10.
The paper describes facial displays and body motor patterns observed in a group of five captive pigtailed macaques during dyadic aggressive and courtship interactions. Correlations between facial displays and body motor patterns were studied quantitatively in order to establish the predictive value of a facial display for the behaviour of the performer. Results indicate that each of the facial displays is largely restricted to a certain type of interaction. Within an interaction of a certain type, the facial displays provided a rather precise prediction of how the performer continued the interaction. Furthermore, the predictive value varied quantitatively between subjects addressing the same partner as well as within the subject when addressing different partners. It is argued that various faces probably function as much as a signal to the interaction partner as to the other individuals which are likely to be nearby and might interfere with the interaction.  相似文献   

11.
Courtship displays are typically thought to have evolved via female choice, whereby females select mates based on the characteristics of a display that is expected to honestly reflect some aspect of the male’s quality. Honesty is typically enforced by mechanistic costs and constraints that limit the level at which a display can be performed. It is becoming increasingly apparent that these costs may be energetic costs involved in the production of dynamic, often repetitive displays. A female attending to such a display may thus be assessing the physical fitness of a male as an index of his quality. Such assessment would provide information on his current physical quality as well as his ability to carry out other demanding activities, qualities with which a choosy female should want to provision her offspring. In the current study we use courtship interactions in the Cuban burrowing cockroach, Byrsotria fumigata to directly test whether courtship is associated with a signaler’s performance capacity. Males that had produced courtship displays achieved significantly lower speeds and distances in locomotor trials than non-courting control males. We also found that females mated more readily with males that produced a more vigorous display. Thus, males of this species have developed a strategy where they produce a demanding courtship display, while females choose males based on their ability to produce this display. Courtship displays in many taxa often involve dynamic repetitive actions and as such, signals of stamina in courtship may be more widespread than previously thought.  相似文献   

12.
Courtship behaviour and songs of six Zaprionus species show differences not only between species but also between sexes. Courtship behaviour differs from that in the related genus Drosophila. In most species, males produce two song types which may alternate or be repeated. Singing occurs during courtship displays, mounting, after copulation and briefly during male-to-male interactions. Females produce a loud whine and body rocking movements in refusal but have a species-specific pulsed song produced during normal courtship which differs from the conspecific male song. One species produces an irregular male song but a regular female song. Sex-specific songs may have selective advantages but pose problems for the sensory template hypothesis.  相似文献   

13.
Bray S  Amrein H 《Neuron》2003,39(6):1019-1029
Propagation in higher animals requires the efficient and accurate display of innate mating behaviors. In Drosophila melanogaster, male courtship consists of a stereotypic sequence of behaviors involving multiple sensory modalities, such as vision, audition, and chemosensation. For example, taste bristles located in the male forelegs and the labial palps are thought to recognize nonvolatile pheromones secreted by the female. Here, we report the identification of the putative pheromone receptor GR68a, which is expressed in chemosensory neurons of about 20 male-specific gustatory bristles in the forelegs. Gr68a expression is dependent on the sex determination gene doublesex, which controls many aspects of sexual differentiation and is necessary for normal courtship behavior. Tetanus toxin-mediated inactivation of Gr68a-expressing neurons or transgene-mediated RNA interference of Gr68a RNA leads to a significant reduction in male courtship performance, suggesting that GR68a protein is an essential component of pheromone-driven courtship behavior in Drosophila.  相似文献   

14.
Ellis LL  Carney GE 《Genetics》2011,187(1):157-169
Behavior is influenced by an organism's genes and environment, including its interactions with same or opposite sex individuals. Drosophila melanogaster perform innate, yet socially modifiable, courtship behaviors that are sex specific and require rapid integration and response to multiple sensory cues. Furthermore, males must recognize and distinguish other males from female courtship objects. It is likely that perception, integration, and response to sex-specific cues is partially mediated by changes in gene expression. Reasoning that social interactions with members of either sex would impact gene expression, we compared expression profiles in heads of males that courted females, males that interacted with other males, or males that did not interact with another fly. Expression of 281 loci changes when males interact with females, whereas 505 changes occur in response to male-male interactions. Of these genes, 265 are responsive to encounters with either sex and 240 respond specifically to male-male interactions. Interestingly, 16 genes change expression only when a male courts a female, suggesting that these changes are a specific response to male-female courtship interactions. We supported our hypothesis that socially-responsive genes can function in behavior by showing that egghead (egh) expression, which increases during social interactions, is required for robust male-to-female courtship. We predict that analyzing additional socially-responsive genes will give us insight into genes and neural signaling pathways that influence reproductive and other behavioral interactions.  相似文献   

15.
The fluorochrome pattern produced by DA/DAPI double staining in Pan paniscus chromosomes is reported. The location of DA/DAPI prominent bands differs from that reported for all other hominoid species. However, the pattern in the pygmy chimpanzee is most similar to that seen in Pan troglodytes. Comparison of the DA/DAPI pattern of the other hominoid species allows the construction of a proposed hominoid ancestral karyotype and a preliminary phylogenetic reconstruction of DA/DAPI bands for the great apes and man.  相似文献   

16.
Among extant hominoids degrees of sexual dimorphism and combined-sex coefficients of variation of canine teeth dimensions are highly correlated. Based on this relationship and coefficients of variation of four species of the genus Australopithecus, we predict degrees of canine dimorphism for these extinct hominids. The estimates show that A. afarensis is as dimorphic as the pygmy chimpanzee, A. boisei slightly less dimorphic than the pygmy chimpanzee, A. robustus slightly more dimorphic than the lar gibbon, while A. africanus overiaps with the lar gibbon as well as a modern human sample. These estimates represent degrees of canine dimorphism substantially lower than results based upon prior sexing of individual specimens. The relationship between canine dimorphism and body weight dimorphism is also analyzed. All four species of Australopithecus are considerably less dimorphic in canine size for their body weight dimorphism than expected. This dissociation of canine size dimorphism and body weight dimorphism is shared with modern humans, and thus represents a unique hominid trait. We interpret the moderate to strong body weight dimorphism in australopithecines as the result of intra- and intersexual selection typical of a polygynous mating structure, while the rather mild canine dimorphism is interpreted in terms of the “developmental crowding” model for reduction in canine size.  相似文献   

17.
Signals transmit information to receivers about sender attributes, increase the fitness of both parties, and are selected for in cooperative interactions between species to reduce conflict [1, 2]. Marine cleaning interactions are known for stereotyped behaviors [3-6] that likely serve as signals. For example, "dancing" and "tactile dancing" in cleaner fish may serve to advertise cleaning services to client fish [7] and manipulate client behavior [8], respectively. Cleaner shrimp clean fish [9], yet are cryptic in comparison to cleaner fish. Signals, therefore, are likely essential for cleaner shrimp to attract clients. Here, we show that the yellow-beaked cleaner shrimp [10] Urocaridella sp. c [11] uses a stereotypical side-to-side movement, or "rocking dance," while approaching potential client fish in the water column. This dance was followed by a cleaning interaction with the client 100% of the time. Hungry cleaner shrimp, which are more willing to clean than satiated ones [12], spent more time rocking and in closer proximity to clients Cephalopholis cyanostigma than satiated ones, and when given a choice, clients preferred hungry, rocking shrimp. The rocking dance therefore influenced client behavior and, thus, appears to function as a signal to advertise the presence of cleaner shrimp to potential clients.  相似文献   

18.
19.
Male courtship behavior and displays influence female mating decisions, and therefore affect mating success in a diverse range of organisms. While there is substantial evidence confirming that females prefer males who invest more in courtship, less attention has been paid to the relative importance of individual behaviors, and the discrete sequences of courtship that result in mating success. The small hairy maggot blow fly, Chrysomya varipes, performs stereotyped courtship behaviors, involving orienting, tapping, waving, arching, wing vibration and mounting. This study aimed to quantify male investment in specific courtship behaviors, and compare courtship investment and behavioral transitions between males who gained mating success (successful males) and those that did not (unsuccessful males). Our results show that mating success was influenced by the behaviors orienting, tapping, arching and mounting. Behavioral transitions revealed a distinct pattern of behaviors leading to a mating attempt, and some differences were observed between successful and unsuccessful males. Overall, our findings suggest that female mating decisions were based on differences in specific male courtship behaviors. This detailed observational study has quantified multiple courtship behaviors for the first time in C. varipes, and highlights the importance of considering multiple behavioral traits when exploring the influence of male courtship on mating success.  相似文献   

20.
Social interactions require knowledge of the environment and status of others, which can be acquired indirectly by observing the behavior of others. When being observed, animals can also alter their signals based on who is watching. Here we observed how male cichlid fish (Astatotilapia burtoni) behave when being watched in two different contexts. In the first, we show that aggressive and courtship behaviors displayed by subordinate males depends critically on whether dominant males can see them, and in the second, we manipulated who was watching aggressive interactions and showed that dominant males will change their behavior depending on audience composition. In both cases, when a more dominant individual is out of view and the audience consists of more subordinate individuals, those males signal key social information to females by displaying courtship and dominant behaviors. In contrast, when a dominant male is present, males cease both aggression and courtship. These data suggest that males are keenly aware of their social environment and modulate their aggressive and courtship behaviors strategically for reproductive and social advantage.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号