首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
D M Wood  H Lal 《Life sciences》1987,41(11):1431-1436
Rats were trained to discriminate an injection of pentylenetetrazol (PTZ), 20 mg/kg, from saline using a two-lever operant procedure with food as a reinforcer. In substitution tests, rats selected the PTZ-appropriate lever after PTZ, but not after cocaine (20 mg/kg). A higher dose of cocaine (40 mg/kg) was behaviorally disruptive which resulted in no lever selection during the test session. Subsequently, training and testing were halted, and cocaine, 20 mg/kg/8-hr, was administered for 7 days. Following this chronic drug regimen, substitution of PTZ for the PTZ stimulus was increased. Furthermore, cocaine (40 mg/kg) substituted for the PTZ stimulus. Following redetermination of the PTZ and cocaine dose-response curves, chronic cocaine injections were terminated and spontaneous withdrawal was assessed by determining its substitution for the PTZ stimulus. Cocaine withdrawal progressively substituted for the PTZ stimulus reaching a peak 120 hrs after the last cocaine injection. Diazepam, 5 mg/kg, blocked the PTZ-like stimulus. These data demonstrate that 1) chronic administration of cocaine produced sensitization for the PTZ stimulus, 2) tolerance developed to the behaviorally disruptive effects of cocaine, and 3) cocaine withdrawal produced a PTZ-like stimulus which was blocked by diazepam.  相似文献   

2.
The dose-response relationships of diazepam and several of its metabolites were determined in rats trained to discriminate diazepam (3 mg/kg) from saline in a two-lever operant choice task. Generalization of the diazepam stimulus was found to occur with temazepam and oxazepam, which were nearly equipotent with diazepam, and also with desmethyldiazepam, which was about half as potent as diazepam. The hydroxylated metabolites, 4'-hydroxydiazepam and 4'-hydroxydesmethyldiazepam were inactive in doses up to 12 mg/kg. These results show that some diazepam metabolites are quite potent behaviorally and indicate the possibility that these metabolites may contribute to the pharmacological effect of diazepam in vivo.  相似文献   

3.
In an operant behavior procedure of lever pressing on an FR 10 schedule of food reinforcement, morphine dependent and nondependent rats were trained to respond on a lever on one side of the food tray after a morphine (10 mg/kg i.p.) injection and to respond on a lever on the alternate side after a saline injection. Following discrimination training, in both dependent and nondependent rats saline was generalized to various doses of clonidine (10, 30 and 50 micrograms/kg i.p.). A response inhibition of about 65% was obtained with the highest dose. It was concluded that, even if clonidine can suppress signs of narcotic withdrawal, the internal state induced by morphine in an abstinent rat does differ from the one induced by clonidine in the same animal.  相似文献   

4.
Because ethanol has N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) antagonist effects, we tested whether dizocilpine, an NMDA antagonist, reinstates ethanol-seeking behavior. Rats were trained to lever-press for a 10% ethanol/2% sucrose (EtOH) or a 3% sucrose (Suc) solution using a two-lever (one lever active) procedure (FR2). After extinction, rats were injected with ethanol (0.5 g/kg). The EtOH group emitted more active than inactive lever presses and the Suc group showed minimal responding. Thus, ethanol reinstated ethanol-seeking behavior in a specific manner. In contrast, dizocilpine (0.175 mg/kg) increased responding on both levers in both groups suggesting a loss of discriminative control. Dizocilpine fails to reinstate ethanol-seeking behavior. These data also demonstrate the necessity of using a discriminative, two-lever test for drug reinstatement.  相似文献   

5.
This experiment tested whether benzodiazepine withdrawal could be detected in an animal model of anxiety. Rats were trained in operant chambers using food reward to press one lever after pentylenetetrazol (PTZ), 20 mg/kg, injection and the other lever after saline injection. Previously, the PTZ cue has been shown to be simulated by anxiogenic drugs and blocked by anxiolytic drugs. After rats reliably performed this discrimination, they were injected with diazepam, 20 mg/kg, from 1 to 4 times a day for six days. For one group of subjects, on the third, fourth and sixth days, they were also injected with 40 mg/kg of RO 15-1788, a benzodiazepine receptor antagonist, and tested for lever selection: 50–80% of the subjects selected the PTZ lever; these results are in contrast to those obtained prior to chronic diazepam treatment in which RO 15-1788 did not generalize to PTZ. A second group of subjects was also injected for six days with diazepam and then allowed to withdraw spontaneously for eight days: PTZ lever selection over this period varied from 20 to 60% of rats. These data indicate that animals trained to discriminate a PTZ cue: 1) generalize the benzodiazepine withdrawal state to the PTZ cue, and 2) discriminate the withdrawal state for long periods of time, agreeing with clinical observations of long-lasting anxiety signs during benzodiazepine withdrawal.  相似文献   

6.
Kanoski SE  Walls EK  Davidson TL 《Peptides》2007,28(5):988-1002
The present studies assessed the extent to which the adiposity signal leptin and the brain-gut hormone cholecystokinin (CCK), administered alone or in combination, give rise to interoceptive sensory cues like those that are produced by a low (1h) level of food deprivation. Rats were trained with cues arising from 1 to 24-h food deprivation as discriminative stimuli. For one group, 24-h food deprivation predicted the delivery of sucrose pellets, whereas 1-h food deprivation did not. Another group received the reversed deprivation level-sucrose contingency. After asymptotic performance was achieved, the effects of leptin and CCK on food intake and on discrimination performance were tested under 24-h food deprivation. In Experiment 1a, leptin administered into the third cerebroventricle (i3vt) at 3.5 or 7.0 microg doses had little effect, compared to saline on food intake or discriminative responding. In Experiment 1b, leptin (7.0 microg, i3vt) combined with CCK-8 (2 microg/kg, i.p.) reduced food intake significantly, but the findings indicated that CCK-8 alone produces interoceptive discriminative cues more like those produced by 1- than 24-h food deprivation. Experiment 2a tested rats with i.p. leptin (0.3 and 0.5mg/kg). Although neither dose suppressed intake, the 0.3mg/kg dose produced interoceptive cues like 1-h food deprivation. Experiment 2b tested two doses of CCK-8 (2 and 4 mg/kg, i.p.) and found significant intake suppression and generalization of discrimination with both doses of CCK-8. These findings suggest a role for both leptin and CCK in the production of sensory consequences that correspond to "satiety".  相似文献   

7.
R J Hamm  J S Knisely 《Life sciences》1986,39(17):1509-1515
The analgesia produced by 24 hr of food deprivation was examined in 4-mo, 14-mo, and 24-mo old rats. To assess opioid and hormonal involvement in food deprivation induced analgesia, different groups of rats from each age group were injected with naltrexone (7 mg/kg), dexamethasone (0.4 mg/kg), or equivolume saline. Results revealed that food deprivation produced an equivalent analgesic response in each saline-treated age group. Also, naltrexone and dexamethasone were equally potent in blocking food deprivation induced analgesia in each age group. These results demonstrated that food deprivation activates an endogenous opioid-mediated analgesic system that involves hormonal factors and that this system does not change in function with increasing age.  相似文献   

8.
In two experiments, the effects of Pavlovian or discriminative conditioned inhibitors on operant responding were investigated in rats. Experiment 1 found that a Pavlovian conditioned inhibitor for food suppressed food-reinforced lever pressing more than a non-differentially trained control stimulus did. Experiment 2 demonstrated that an operant discriminative inhibitor produced greater suppression of lever pressing than a Pavlovian conditioned inhibitor. Experiment 2 also found that compounding an operant discriminative stimulus (SD) for food-reinforced responding with another SD for food-reinforced responding resulted in more additive summation than when an SD was compounded with a Pavlovian conditioned excitor for food. The results of these experiments support two-factor theories that postulate that incentive and response discriminative processes summate algebraically when the processes are inhibitory or excitatory.  相似文献   

9.
Although caffeine-phenylethylamine combinations are widely available as over-the-counter medications or as "legal" stimulants, little information is available concerning their behavioral pharmacology or abuse potential. In the present study, rats were trained in a food-reward, two-lever operant drug discrimination paradigm to differentially respond after saline or 0.5 mg/kg amphetamine injections. Tests for generalization to the amphetamine cue indicated only modest amphetamine-lever responding at various doses of caffeine alone or at various doses of ephedrine/phenylpropanolamine (PPA) combinations, but complete generalization to the training cue was found with higher doses of the triple combination (caffeine, ephedrine, and PPA) or with caffeine-ephedrine or caffeine-PPA combinations. All drugs produced response rate decreases at higher doses. These data clearly indicate that certain "legal" stimulants mimic the amphetamine cue and suggest that caffeine may interact additively with phenylethylamines to produce the cue.  相似文献   

10.
The abuse of PCP continues to be an important medical problem in many urban areas. The probability that dependence on PCP may contribute to its compulsive use and relapse is supported by animal studies demonstrating its dependence liability. In the present study, five rats were housed in operant chambers and trained to respond on a lever under a fixed-ratio 30 schedule of food presentation. They obtained all their daily food during four 30-min response periods occurring every 6 hr. After stable baselines of behavior were established the rats were injected with PCP (3.0-7.5 mg/kg/injection), i.p., 1 hr before each response session for 7-10 days. Following chronic dosing, the drug injections were replaced with saline injections for 10 days. Disruptions in behavior were observed upon cessation of relatively brief chronic exposure to PCP (as little as 7 days) and at relatively low doses (5.6 mg/kg/6 hr = 22.4 mg/kg/day). The behavioral disruption was not accompanied by overt signs of abstinence and persisted for up to 48 hr.  相似文献   

11.
D G Spencer  H Lal 《Life sciences》1983,32(20):2329-2333
Recent neurochemical data on the effects of activation and blockade of adenosine A1 receptors has suggested a direct role of adenosine in neurotransmission. The present research used a drug discrimination procedure to test the hypotheses that A1 adenosine receptor activation could serve as a discriminative stimulus and that caffeine, a drug believed to be an A1 receptor antagonist, could block the adenosine discrimination. Food-deprived rats were trained to press one of two levers on an FR 10 schedule of food-pellet delivery. Responses on one lever were reinforced following i.p. injection of N6 - (L-phenylisopropyl) adenosine (L-PIA); responses on the other lever were reinforced following i.p. injection of saline. L-PIA training dose was increased from 0.064 to 0.08 mg/kg L-PIA in the course of the study. Subjects required an average of 91 sessions to acquire this discrimination. Stimulus control by L-PIA was dose-dependent, with the ED-50 being approximately 0.03 mg/kg. 2-Chloroadenosine (2CA) generalized to L-PIA with a tenth the potency. Caffeine blocked L-PIA-induced lever selection. These results indicate that 1) rats can be trained to discriminate L-PIA from saline in a two-lever food-reinforced task and 2) the discriminative stimuli produced by L-PIA are based on its agonistic action at the adenosine A1 receptor.  相似文献   

12.
Passive immunization against nicotine attenuates nicotine discrimination   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Ten rats were trained in a two lever operant chamber to press different levers after a nicotine injection (0.14 mg/kg s.c.) or a saline injection on an FR10 schedule. The rats were then injected i.p. with either 150 mg nicotine-specific IgG or the same amount of control IgG from non-immunized rabbits. On successive days, they were retested with both levers active after a saline injection, a full training dose of nicotine and a half dose of nicotine (0.07 mg/kg s.c.). After saline injection, both groups pressed the saline lever almost exclusively. After each of the nicotine doses, the immunized rats performed a significantly lower percentage of their lever presses on the nicotine lever than did non-immunized rats. The results suggest that passive immunization can interfere with the stimulus properties of nicotine.  相似文献   

13.
Ten rats were trained to discriminate between the stimulus properties of subcutaneously (SC) administered MSH/ACTH4-10 and saline in a two-lever, food-motivated operant task. After 12 weeks of discriminative training with 100 micrograms/kg MSH/ACTH4-10, half the rats received 200 micrograms/kg MSH/ATCH4-10, whereas the other half were administered 400 micrograms/kg, for 6 additional weeks. Subsequently, all rats continued training on 50 micrograms/kg ORG 2766 (SC) and, after 12 weeks of training, were randomly assigned to receive either 100 or 200 micrograms/kg ORG 2766. The results of this extensive 36 week training schedule indicate that only 1 of the 10 rats learned to discriminate the interoceptive cues produced by the ACTH analogs. However, this rat's performance was so sustained and errorless that the possibility exists that it was relatively more sensitive to the effects of MSH/ACTH4-10 and its analogs and that these substances may support discriminative learning in the rat.  相似文献   

14.
Gatch MB  Jung ME  Wallis CJ  Lal H 《Life sciences》2002,71(22):2657-2665
Male Long-Evans rats were trained to discriminate mCPP (1.4 mg/kg, i.p.) from saline, using a two-lever, food-reinforced operant task. The GABA(A) antagonist, bicuculline (0.16-0.64 mg/kg), partially substituted for mCPP, whereas the benzodiazepine antagonist, flumazenil (1-10 mg/kg), and the benzodiazepine inverse agonist, Ro 15-4513 (0.25-2.5 mg/kg), failed to substitute for mCPP. Bicuculline produced no change in response rate, whereas Ro 15-4513 dose-dependently decreased responding. Flumazenil produced a small increase in response rates. Flumazenil (10 mg/kg), Ro 15-4513 (1.25 mg/kg), and the benzodiazepine agonists alprazolam (0.64 mg/kg) and diazepam (5 mg/kg) full agonist all failed to block the mCPP discriminative stimulus. When given in combination with mCPP, Ro15-4513 and alprazolam both produced lower response rates than did mCPP alone, whereas flumazenil and diazepam did not significantly alter response rates. These findings provide evidence that GABA(A) antagonists modulate the discriminative stimulus effects of mCPP, but that these effects are not mediated by activity at the benzodiazepine site.  相似文献   

15.
In this protocol, social motivation is measured in mice through a pair of operant conditioning paradigms. To conduct the experiments, two-chambered shuttle boxes were equipped with two operant levers (left and right) and a food receptacle in one chamber, which was then divided from the second chamber by an automated guillotine door covered by a wire grid. Different stimulus mice, rotated across testing days, served as a social stimulus behind the wire grid, and were only visible following the opening of the guillotine door. Test mice were trained to lever press in order to open the door and gain access to the stimulus partner for 15 sec. The number of lever presses required to obtain the social reward progressively increased on a fixed schedule of 3. Testing sessions ended after test mice stopped lever pressing for 5 consecutive minutes. The last reinforced ratio or breakpoint can be used as a quantitative measure of social motivation. For the second paradigm, test mice were trained to discriminate between left and right lever presses in order to obtain either a food reward or the social reward. Mice were rewarded for every 3 presses of each respective lever. The number of food and social rewards can be compared as a measurement of the value placed upon each reward. The ratio of each reward type can also be compared between mouse strains and the change in this ratio can be monitored within testing sessions to measure satiation with a given reward type. Both of these operant conditioning paradigms are highly useful for the quantification of social motivation in mouse models of autism and other disorders of social behavior.  相似文献   

16.
D R Meyer  S B Sparber 《Life sciences》1977,21(8):1087-1093
Rats were trained to lever press for food pellets under a 20 response fixed ratio (FR 20) schedule of reinforcement. A single injection of 15 mg morphine SO4/kg suppressed operant behavior for 112–312hrs, after which time responding resumed at a reduced rate. When 0.25 mg naloxone HCl/kg was given during the recovery phase, the behavioral depressant effect of the narcotic was immediately reversed and operant performance returned to predrug rates. In contrast, when 0.5 mg naloxone/kg was given at this time, operant behavior was abolished for at least 1 hr. Naloxone, at these doses, did not affect responding in drug-naive subjects. These results suggest that a single, relatively low dose of morphine can induce transient dependence which is detectable for several hrs after drug administration, at a time when the acute pharmacological actions of morphine are still apparent.  相似文献   

17.
To examine the role of glycineB receptors in the stimulus effects induced by psychostimulants, separate groups of rats were trained to discriminate amphetamine (AMPH; 1 mg/kg) from saline (SAL), or cocaine (COC; 10 mg/kg) from SAL, using a two-lever operant procedure. Substitution studies showed that neither 1-aminocyclopropanecarboxylic acid (ACPC; 200 mg/kg) nor 7-chloro-4-hydroxy-3-(3-phenoxy)phenyl-(H)quinolone (L-701,324; 3 mg/kg), being a partial agonist or an antagonist at glycineB receptors, respectively, generalized for the training drugs. Combination tests of glycineB ligands demonstrated that injection of a fixed dose of ACPC (200 mg/kg) or L-701,324 (3 mg/kg) together with different doses of AMPH or COC practically did not modify dose-response curves of the psychostimulants, nor did it affect their ED50 values. Our results indicate that glycineB receptors do not play a role in the discriminative effects of AMPH and COC.  相似文献   

18.
Fenfluramine (2.5 and 5 mg/kg) significantly suppressed the food intake of rats following food deprivation, administration of 2-deoxy-D-glucose (2DG), and during tail pressure. This suggests that fenfluramine has relatively general anorectic potency. Other "serotonergic" anorectics were studied for comparison. In a second experiment we determined that norfenfluramine and quipazine greatly suppressed food intake following food deprivation but, at the same doses, had relatively small effects on water intake following water deprivation. This was true for intraperitoneal and cerebroventricular routes of administration. The data have relevance for specificity of action of these agents and for the possible contribution of dopamine antagonist properties.  相似文献   

19.
The controls of food intake differ in male and female rats. Daily food intake is typically greater in male rats, relative to female rats, and a decrease in food intake, coincident with the estrous stage of the ovarian reproductive cycle, is well documented in female rats. This estrous-related decrease in food intake has been attributed to a transient increase in the female rat's sensitivity to satiety signals generated during feeding bouts. Here, we investigated whether sex or stage of the estrous cycle modulate the satiety signal generated by fenfluramine, a potent serotonin (5-HT) releasing agent. To examine this hypothesis, food intake was monitored in male, diestrous female, and estrous female rats after intraperitoneal injections of 0, 0.25, and 1.0 mg/kg D-fenfluramine. The lower dose of fenfluramine decreased food intake only in diestrous and estrous females, suggesting that the minimally effective anorectic dose of fenfluramine is lower in female rats, relative to male rats. Although the larger dose of fenfluramine decreased food intake in both sexes, the duration of anorexia was greater in diestrous and estrous female rats, relative to male rats. Moreover, the magnitude of the anorectic effect of the larger dose of fenfluramine was greatest in estrous rats, intermediate in diestrous rats, and least in male rats. Thus our findings indicate that the anorectic effect of fenfluramine is modulated by gonadal hormone status.  相似文献   

20.
Rats were trained in a two-lever drug discrimination paradigm to discriminate midazolam (0.32 mg/kg, i.p. or 1.0 mg/kg, i.p.) from the no-drug condition. After completion of i.p. and s.c. midazolam generalization gradients (0.032-1.0 mg/kg), rats were surgically implanted with unilateral cannulae into the lateral ventricles. Intracerebroventricular (i.c.v.) doses of 1.1-44.2 micrograms midazolam were delivered to unrestrained rats. Midazolam produced dose-dependent increases in drug-appropriate responding by all three routes of administration, but was 2.4- to 4.3-fold more potent when given i.c.v. than when given s.c. or i.p. Midazolam, over the dose range tested, did not produce substantial decreases in response rate by any route of administration. The discriminative-stimulus effect of i.c.v. midazolam was blocked by peripherally administered flumazenil, and such antagonism was surmounted by a 2- to 5-fold increase in the i.c.v. midazolam dose. Taken together, these data suggest that the discriminative-stimulus effects of midazolam are mediated via central benzodiazepine (BZ) receptors.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号