首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
 Multistability in vision is an intriguing phenomenon that is currently not well understood. In this paper, we present a new, stochastic model for multistable visual perception. It is based on results of time series analysis of experimental data, yielding evidence for it being a linear, stochastic process. This is the outcome of testing for unstable periodic orbits and comparing the correlation dimension of the data to that of white noise. In the model, all degrees of freedom but one can be determined by general knowledge, thus resulting in a high degree of parsimony. The remaining parameter is used to model the individual characteristics that vary between subjects. Fitting simulations to the experimental data proves the parameter to be in a physiologically highly plausible range. Received: 12 September 2000 / Accepted in revised form: 17 May 2001  相似文献   

2.
A mathematical model of visual perception is presented with the intention of throwing some light on the problem of perceptual invariance. Two types of differential manifolds (receptive and effector) are associated with the repertoire which is the fundamental concept in the model. The elements of the repertoire carry weights which control the input-output relation in the repertoire and which can be modified by a learning process. It is shown that, under reasonable conditions, these repertoires possess good stability properties and can adjust to the various environments to which they may be subjected. In particular cases, it is shown that the stochastic learning process can be considered as deterministic to a first approximation.  相似文献   

3.
Jolij J  Meurs M 《PloS one》2011,6(4):e18861

Background

Visual perception is not a passive process: in order to efficiently process visual input, the brain actively uses previous knowledge (e.g., memory) and expectations about what the world should look like. However, perception is not only influenced by previous knowledge. Especially the perception of emotional stimuli is influenced by the emotional state of the observer. In other words, how we perceive the world does not only depend on what we know of the world, but also by how we feel. In this study, we further investigated the relation between mood and perception.

Methods and Findings

We let observers do a difficult stimulus detection task, in which they had to detect schematic happy and sad faces embedded in noise. Mood was manipulated by means of music. We found that observers were more accurate in detecting faces congruent with their mood, corroborating earlier research. However, in trials in which no actual face was presented, observers made a significant number of false alarms. The content of these false alarms, or illusory percepts, was strongly influenced by the observers'' mood.

Conclusions

As illusory percepts are believed to reflect the content of internal representations that are employed by the brain during top-down processing of visual input, we conclude that top-down modulation of visual processing is not purely predictive in nature: mood, in this case manipulated by music, may also directly alter the way we perceive the world.  相似文献   

4.
How does an animal conceal itself from visual detection by other animals? This review paper seeks to identify general principles that may apply in this broad area. It considers mechanisms of visual encoding, of grouping and object encoding, and of search. In most cases, the evidence base comes from studies of humans or species whose vision approximates to that of humans. The effort is hampered by a relatively sparse literature on visual function in natural environments and with complex foraging tasks. However, some general constraints emerge as being potentially powerful principles in understanding concealment—a ‘constraint’ here means a set of simplifying assumptions. Strategies that disrupt the unambiguous encoding of discontinuities of intensity (edges), and of other key visual attributes, such as motion, are key here. Similar strategies may also defeat grouping and object-encoding mechanisms. Finally, the paper considers how we may understand the processes of search for complex targets in complex scenes. The aim is to provide a number of pointers towards issues, which may be of assistance in understanding camouflage and concealment, particularly with reference to how visual systems can detect the shape of complex, concealed objects.  相似文献   

5.
Somewhere between the retina and our conscious visual experience, the majority of the information impinging on the eye is lost. We are typically aware of only either the most salient parts of a visual scene or the parts that we are actively paying attention to. Recent research on visual neurons in monkeys is beginning to show how the brain both selects and discards incoming visual information. For example, what happens to the responses of visual neurons when attention is directed to one element, such as an oriented colored bar, embedded among an array of other oriented bars? Some of this research shows that attention to the oriented bar restricts the receptive field of visual neurons down to this single element. However, other research shows that attention to this single element affects the responses of neurons with receptive fields throughout the visual field. In this review, these two seemingly contradictory results are shown to actually be mutually consistent. A simple computational model is described that explains these results, and also provides a framework for predicting a variety of additional neurophysiological, neuroimaging and behavioral studies of attention.  相似文献   

6.
7.
By measuring the contrast threshold for gratings of different waveform and spatial frequency, Campbell & Robson suggested in 1968 that there may be 'channels' tuned to different spatial frequencies. By using the technique of adapting to a high contrast grating, it was possible to measure the band-pass characteristics of these channels. Similar techniques were used to establish the orientational tuning of the channels. Reasons are put forward why it is advantageous to organize the visual system in this manner.  相似文献   

8.
9.
10.
Studies on the visual processing of primates, which have well developed visual systems, provide essential information about the perceptual bases of their higher-order cognitive abilities. Although the mechanisms underlying visual processing are largely shared between human and nonhuman primates, differences have also been reported. In this article, we review psychophysical investigations comparing the basic visual processing that operates in human and nonhuman species, and discuss the future contributions potentially deriving from such comparative psychophysical approaches to primate minds.  相似文献   

11.
There is now abundant evidence that the structure of the mammalian visual cortex is not innately determined but can be altered by visual experiences during a certain period at an early age. A model based on the evidence is proposed that will explain some aspects of the developmental process in the visual cortex. The model self-organizingly forms a simple cell which responds to bars and edges of a certains orientation and retinal position. The total system of the model corresponds to a hyper column which contains a complete cycle of orientational selectiviy. The model has the following three original points. i) A hyper column is finally formed in the model. ii) Most of the cells do not have orientation selectivity in the initial state. iii) It is hypothesized that the orientation continuity of the hyper column is caused by similar orientations of successive input stimuli. The results of computer simulation show that the model has the expected performance.  相似文献   

12.
 Although the extrapolation of past perceptual history into the immediate and distant future is a fundamental phenomenon in everyday life, the underlying processing mechanisms are not well understood. A network model consisting of interacting excitatory and inhibitory cell populations coding for stimulus position is used to study the neuronal population response to a continuously moving stimulus. An adaptation mechanism is proposed that offers the possibility to control and modulate motion-induced extrapolation without changing the spatial interaction structure within the network. Using an occluder paradigm, functional advantages of an internally generated model of a moving stimulus are discussed. It is shown that the integration of such a model in processing leads to a faster and more reliable recognition of the input stream and allows for object permanence following occlusion. The modeling results are discussed in relation to recent experimental findings that show motion-induced extrapolation. Received: 19 December 2001 / Accepted: 26 November 2002 / Published online: 3 April 2003 Correspondence to: W. Erlhagen (e-mail: wolfram.erlhagen@mct.uminho.pt) Acknowledgements. The author would like to thank D. Jancke for useful discussions and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments and suggestions on a previous version of this paper. This research was supported by a European grant (IST-2000-29689) and by the Portuguese Science Foundation (POSI/SRI/38051/2001).  相似文献   

13.
Recent developments have led to a greater insight into the complex processes of perception of visual motion. A better understanding of the neuronal circuitry involved and advances in electrophysiological techniques have allowed researchers to alter the perception of an animal with a stimulating electrode. In addition, studies have further elucidated the processes by which signals are combined and compared, allowing a greater understanding of the effects of selective brain damage.  相似文献   

14.
Vagaries of visual perception in autism   总被引:16,自引:0,他引:16  
Dakin S  Frith U 《Neuron》2005,48(3):497-507
Three classes of perceptual phenomena have repeatedly been associated with autism spectrum disorder (ASD): superior processing of fine detail (local structure), either inferior processing of overall/global structure or an ability to ignore disruptive global/contextual information, and impaired motion perception. This review evaluates the quality of the evidence bearing on these three phenomena. We argue that while superior local processing has been robustly demonstrated, conclusions about global processing cannot be definitively drawn from the experiments to date, which have generally not precluded observers using more local cues. Perception of moving stimuli is impaired in ASD, but explanations in terms of magnocellular/dorsal deficits do not appear to be sufficient. We suggest that abnormalities in the superior temporal sulcus (STS) may provide a neural basis for the range of motion-processing deficits observed in ASD, including biological motion perception. Such an explanation may also provide a link between perceptual abnormalities and specific deficits in social cognition associated with autism.  相似文献   

15.
A model of paradoxical odour mixture perception   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0  
Gregson  R.A.M. 《Chemical senses》1980,5(3):257-269
An identification experiment on a series of 10 graded mixturesof musk and n-propanol, with constrained (approximately constant)total concentration was modelled by a psychophysical functionwhich reproduced up to two local discontinuities in the perceptionof the intensity of one component; such results can be paralleledby analogous perceptual processes in vision. The model usedis an extension of previous models on odour cross-adaptation,with additional boundary conditions added.  相似文献   

16.
In continuation of previous studies of the mathematical biophysics of visual perception in relation to the aesthetic evaluation of visual patterns, an expression for the total intensity of excitation in a discriminating center as a function of the intensity of the peripheral stimulus is derived. This expression is applied to the case of aesthetic judgments of similar polygons of different sizes. The theoretical conclusions are tested experimentally by use of standard psychological scaling methods. The theoretical predictions are found to be in agreement with the experimental results.  相似文献   

17.
18.
A model of visual backward masking   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Bugmann G  Taylor JG 《Bio Systems》2005,79(1-3):151-158
When two successive stimuli are presented within 0-200 ms intervals, the recognition of the first stimulus (the target) can be impaired by the second (the mask). This backward masking phenomenon has a form called metacontrast masking where the target and the mask are in close spatial proximity but not overlapping. In that case, the masking effect is strongest for interval of 60-100 ms. To understand this behaviour, activity propagation in a feedforward network of leaky integrate and fire neurons is investigated. It is found that, if neurons have a selectivity similar to that of V1 simple cells, activity decays layer after layer and ceases to propagate. To combat this, a local amplification mechanism is included in the model, using excitatory lateral connections, which turn out to support prolonged self-sustained activity. Masking is assumed to arise from local competition between representations recruited by the target and the mask. This tends to interrupt sustained firing, while prolonged retinal input tends to re-initiate it. Thus, masking causes a maximal reduction of the duration of the cortical response to the target towards the end of the retinal response. This duration exhibits the typical U-shape of the masking curve. In this model, masking does not alter the propagation of the onset of the response to the target, thus preserving response reaction times and enabling unconscious priming phenomena.  相似文献   

19.
Visual anisotropy has been demonstrated in multiple tasks where performance differs between vertical, horizontal, and oblique orientations of the stimuli. We explain some principles of visual anisotropy by anisotropic smoothing, which is based on a variation on Koenderink's approach in [1]. We tested the theory by presenting gaussian elongated luminance profiles and measuring the perceived orientations by means of an adjustment task. Our framework is based on the smoothing of the image with elliptical gaussian kernels and it correctly predicted an illusory orientation bias towards the vertical axis. We discuss the scope of the theory in the context of other anisotropies in perception.  相似文献   

20.
To examine the phyletic distribution and ontogeny of ‘head-cocking’ (rotating the cranium about the longitudinal body axis while orienting in a fixed direction) in primates, I conducted observations on 229 individuals of 40 different species. Head-cocking in primates typically occurs during visual inspection of objects. The response is primarily characteristic of diminutive species that lack ocular dominance columns in the visual striate cortex (e.g. marmosets, squirrel monkeys), and is most frequently observed during infancy.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号