首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Durgin FH 《Current biology : CB》2008,18(18):R855-R856
Burr and Ross [1] have recently proposed that the visual dimension of number is itself directly adaptable. The aftereffect they describe is one that my colleagues and I previously used to investigate the perception of texture density [2-4]. Burr and Ross [1] argue that the effect is new because it concerns visual number, rather than texture density, but they did not report critical tests to support this claim. Here, I shall briefly describe the striking similarity between our prior work and that of Burr and Ross [1], and discuss how some of our results rule out Burr and Ross's [1] interpretation that numerosity, and not density, is at play. I shall also provide a new demonstration that confirms that these effects are based on density, using a display that explicitly dissociates density from numerosity. Taken together, this line of arguments suggests that Burr and Ross's [1] recent study may best be thought of as replicating support within a well-established line of work on texture density.  相似文献   

2.
A visual sense of number   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Evidence exists for a nonverbal capacity for the apprehension of number, in humans [1] (including infants [2, 3]) and in other primates [4-6]. Here, we show that perceived numerosity is susceptible to adaptation, like primary visual properties of a scene, such as color, contrast, size, and speed. Apparent numerosity was decreased by adaptation to large numbers of dots and increased by adaptation to small numbers, the effect depending entirely on the numerosity of the adaptor, not on contrast, size, orientation, or pixel density, and occurring with very low adaptor contrasts. We suggest that the visual system has the capacity to estimate numerosity and that it is an independent primary visual property, not reducible to others like spatial frequency or density of texture [7].  相似文献   

3.
Gebuis T  Reynvoet B 《PloS one》2012,7(5):e37426
Mainstream theory suggests that the approximate number system supports our non-symbolic number abilities (e.g. estimating or comparing different sets of items). It is argued that this system can extract number independently of the visual cues present in the stimulus (diameter, aggregate surface, etc.). However, in a recent report we argue that this might not be the case. We showed that participants combined information from different visual cues to derive their answers. While numerosity comparison requires a rough comparison of two sets of items (smaller versus larger), numerosity estimation requires a more precise mechanism. It could therefore be that numerosity estimation, in contrast to numerosity comparison, might rely on the approximate number system. To test this hypothesis, we conducted a numerosity estimation experiment. We controlled for the visual cues according to current standards: each single visual property was not informative about numerosity. Nevertheless, the results reveal that participants were influenced by the visual properties of the dot arrays. They gave a larger estimate when the dot arrays consisted of dots with, on average, a smaller diameter, aggregate surface or density but a larger convex hull. The reliance on visual cues to estimate numerosity suggests that the existence of an approximate number system that can extract numerosity independently of the visual cues is unlikely. Instead, we propose that humans estimate numerosity by weighing the different visual cues present in the stimuli.  相似文献   

4.
Kramer P  Di Bono MG  Zorzi M 《PloS one》2011,6(2):e17378

Background

Numerosity estimation is a basic preverbal ability that humans share with many animal species and that is believed to be foundational of numeracy skills.It is notoriously difficult, however, to establish whether numerosity estimation is based on numerosity itself, or on one or more non-numerical cues like—in visual stimuli—spatial extent and density. Frequently, different non-numerical cues are held constant on different trials. This strategy, however, still allows numerosity estimation to be based on a combination of non-numerical cues rather than on any particular one by itself.

Methodology/Principal Findings

Here we introduce a novel method, based on second-order (contrast-based) visual motion, to create stimuli that exclude all first-order (luminance-based) cues to numerosity. We show that numerosities can be estimated almost as well in second-order motion as in first-order motion.

Conclusions/Significance

The results show that numerosity estimation need not be based on first-order spatial filtering, first-order density perception, or any other processing of luminance-based cues to numerosity. Our method can be used as an effective tool to control non-numerical variables in studies of numerosity estimation.  相似文献   

5.
There is strong evidence that magnitudes in different dimensions can interfere. A majority of previous studies on the interaction of temporal magnitudes on numerosity showed no interfering effect, while many studies have reported the interference of numerosity on judgement of temporal magnitudes. We speculated that this one-way interference is confounded by the magnitudes used in the studies. We used a methodology that allowed us to study this interaction reciprocally. Moreover, we selected magnitudes for two dimensions that enabled us to detect their interfering effects. Participants had to either judge which of two successive sets of items was more numerous (numerosity judgement task), or which set of items was presented longer (duration judgement task). We hypothesised that a longer presentation of a set will be judged as being more numerous, and vice versa, a more numerous set will be judged as being presented longer. Results confirmed our hypothesis. A positive correlation between duration of presentation and judged numerosity as well as a positive correlation between the number of items and judged duration of presentation was found. This observation supports the idea that duration and numerosity judgements are not completely independent and implies the existence of (partly) generalised and abstract components in the magnitude representations.  相似文献   

6.
Much evidence has accumulated to suggest that many animals, including young human infants, possess an abstract sense of approximate quantity, a number sense. Most research has concentrated on apparent numerosity of spatial arrays of dots or other objects, but a truly abstract sense of number should be capable of encoding the numerosity of any set of discrete elements, however displayed and in whatever sensory modality. Here, we use the psychophysical technique of adaptation to study the sense of number for serially presented items. We show that numerosity of both auditory and visual sequences is greatly affected by prior adaptation to slow or rapid sequences of events. The adaptation to visual stimuli was spatially selective (in external, not retinal coordinates), pointing to a sensory rather than cognitive process. However, adaptation generalized across modalities, from auditory to visual and vice versa. Adaptation also generalized across formats: adapting to sequential streams of flashes affected the perceived numerosity of spatial arrays. All these results point to a perceptual system that transcends vision and audition to encode an abstract sense of number in space and in time.  相似文献   

7.
The relationship between environment and mutation is complex [1]. Claims of Lamarkian mutation [2] have proved unfounded [3], [4] and [5]; it is apparent, however, that the external environment can influence the generation of heritable variation, through either direct effects on DNA sequence [6] or DNA maintenance and copying mechanisms [7], [8], [9] and [10], or as a consequence of evolutionary processes [11], [12], [13], [14], [15] and [16]. The spectrum of mutational events subject to environmental influence is unknown [6] and precisely how environmental signals modulate mutation is unclear. Evidence from bacteria suggests that a transient recombination-dependent hypermutational state can be induced by starvation [5]. It is also apparent that chnages in the mutability of specific loci can be influenced by alterations in DNA topology [10] and [17]. Here we describe a remarkable instance of adaptive evolution in Salmonella which is caused by a mutation that occurs in intermediate-strength osmotic environments. We show that the mutation is not ‘directed’ and describe its genetic basis. We also present compelling evidence in support of the hypothesis that the mutational event is constrained by signals transmitted from the external environment via changes in the activity of DNA gyrase.  相似文献   

8.
The relationship between environment and mutation is complex [1]. Claims of Lamarkian mutation [2] have proved unfounded [3-5]; it is apparent, however, that the external environment can influence the generation of heritable variation, through either direct effects on DNA sequence [6] or DNA maintenance and copying mechanisms [7-10], or as a consequence of evolutionary processes [11-16]. The spectrum of mutational events subject to environmental influence is unknown [6] and precisely how environmental signals modulate mutation is unclear. Evidence from bacteria suggests that a transient recombination-dependent hypermutational state can be induced by starvation [5]. It is also apparent that changes in the mutability of specific loci can be influenced by alterations in DNA topology [10,17]. Here we describe a remarkable instance of adaptive evolution in Salmonella which is caused by a mutation that occurs in intermediate-strength osmotic environments. We show that the mutation is not 'directed' and describe its genetic basis. We also present compelling evidence in support of the hypothesis that the mutational event is constrained by signals transmitted from the external environment via changes in the activity of DNA gyrase.  相似文献   

9.
许多研究表明 ,大气 CO2 浓度 ([CO2 ])的升高会导致植物气孔密度 (Stom atal Density,SD)和气孔指数 (Stom atal Index,SI)降低。这一关系成为推测地质历史时期大气 [CO2 ]变化的重要古生物指标之一。但是 ,[CO2 ]不是唯一影响 SD和 SI的环境因素。研究利用温度梯度和温度 [CO2 ]梯度技术 ,以 7种美国中西部地区弃耕地常见草本植物和 3种美国东部落叶阔叶林优势木本植物为材料 ,其中草本包含豆科、非豆科 C3和 C4 功能型 ,就它们的 SD,SI,表皮细胞密度 (Epidermal Cell Density,ECD)和气孔孔径长度 (Stomatal Aperture L ength,APL)对 [CO2 ]和温度升高的反应进行了研究。结果表明 ,沿 [CO2 ]梯度 ,所研究物种的 SD比 SI反应敏感 ,SD显示出与 [CO2 ]正相关、负相关和无显著相关性 ,SI显示出与 [CO2 ]正相关和无显著相关性 ;沿温度梯度 ,所研究物种的 SI比 SD反应敏感 ,SI显示出与温度正相关、负相关和无显著相关性 ,SD显示出与温度正相关和无显著相关性。 ECD和 APL对 [CO2 ]和温度梯度也有不同的响应。这说明 ,除 [CO2 ]外 ,温度也对 SD,SI,ECD和 APL有显著的影响。所以在用气孔特征重建地质历史时期 [CO2 ]的变化趋势时 ,除准确建立气孔参数与 [CO2 ]关系外 ,还应考虑大气温度对这一关系的影响  相似文献   

10.
Ecological and epidemiological invasions occur in a spatial context. We investigated how these processes correlate to the distance dependence of spread or dispersal between spatial entities such as habitat patches or epidemiological units. Distance dependence is described by a spatial kernel, characterized by its shape (kurtosis) and width (variance). We also developed a novel method to analyse and generate point-pattern landscapes based on spectral representation. This involves two measures: continuity, which is related to autocorrelation and contrast, which refers to variation in patch density. We also analysed some empirical data where our results are expected to have implications, namely distributions of trees (Quercus and Ulmus) and farms in Sweden. Through a simulation study, we found that kernel shape was not important for predicting the invasion speed in randomly distributed patches. However, the shape may be essential when the distribution of patches deviates from randomness, particularly when the contrast is high. We conclude that the speed of invasions depends on the spatial context and the effect of the spatial kernel is intertwined with the spatial structure. This implies substantial demands on the empirical data, because it requires knowledge of shape and width of the spatial kernel, and spatial structure.  相似文献   

11.
K Mano  A Akbarzadeh  R G Townley 《Life sciences》1979,25(22):1925-1930
It has been observed that glucocorticoids potentiate beta-adrenergic stimulation of cardiovascular and airway tissues. In order to investigate the mechanism of this potentiating action, we examined the effect of glucocorticoids on the number and affinity of beta-adrenergic receptors in animal lung tissues, by a direct binding technique using [125]I-Iodohydroxybenzylpindolol ([125]I-HYP), a potent beta-adrenergic receptor antagonist. Specific binding of [125]I-HYP to rat lung membranes was saturable with 386 fmol of [125]I-HYP/mg protein at saturation. The apparent equilibrium dissociation constant of [125]I-HYP for beta-receptors was 221 nM. Chronic administration of hydrocortisone increased the density of beta-adrenergic receptors by 70% from 386 fmol to 657 fmol/mg with some decrease in the affinity of [125]I-HYP for beta-adrenergic receptors. By contrast, adrenalectomy produced a 29% fall in the number of beta-adrenergic receptors without altering the affinity of [125]I-HYP for beta-receptors, and this change was reversed by exogenous adminstration of hydrocortisone. The present study suggests that glucocorticoids may participate in regulating the density of beta-adrenergic receptors, and may potentiate beta-adrenergic receptors stimulation, at least in part by increasing beta-receptor density in tissue membranes.  相似文献   

12.
When [Na] was suddenly introduced to single muscle fibers (Xenopus or frog), which had been pretreated with Na-free solution (Tris- substituted), the time-course of twitch recovery was very variable, half-time ranging from less than 1 S to 5 S. The [Na] vs. twitch height relationship was also variable. In small Xenopus fibers, decreases of [Na] to 50% increased the twitch, while in large Xenopus fibers twitch height remained constant or decreased as [Na] was decreased to 50%. The apparent diffusion constant (D') of Na+ or K+, calculated from the time- course of twitch recovery and the [Na] vs. twitch relation, and from the time-course of the slow repolarization upon sudden reduction of [K] was about 1-1.5 X 10(-6) cm2/S. This is one order of magnitude smaller than the diffusion constants in an aqueous solution. Even if the tortuosity factor of the T system is taken into account, there remains a substantial discrepancy. Although our value of D' is subject to various errors, if we accept the value, the twitch recovery is predicted to be either very quick or slow depending upon the variation of [Na]-twitch relation and fiber size. Thus, both quick and slow twitch recoveries can be explained by the diffusion time of Na+ in the T system, and therefore the results are consistent with the idea that the T system is excitable.  相似文献   

13.
Co-incubation of rat cortical membranes with 10(-4) M GTP results in a competitive inhibition of 5-hydroxytryptamine1A (5-HT1A) receptor binding sites labeled by [3H]8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)tetralin [( 3H]8-OH-DPAT). Preincubation of cortical membranes with 10(-4) M GTP does not significantly change either KD or Bmax values, indicating that the effect of GTP is reversible. By contrast, GTP gamma S and 5'-guanylylimidodiphosphate (GppNHp) are nonhydrolyzable analogues of GTP which lengthen the time course of guanine nucleotide activation of guanine nucleotide binding proteins (G proteins) and thereby alter G protein-receptor interactions. These nonhydrolyzable GTP analogues were used to characterize the effects of persistent alterations in G proteins on [3H]8-OH-DPAT binding to 5-HT1A receptors. Co-incubation of rat cortical membranes with either 10(-4) M GTP gamma S or GppNHp results in a decrease in both the affinity and apparent density of 5-HT1A binding sites. Co-incubation with the nonhydrolyzable nucleotides reduces the affinity of [3H]8-OH-DPAT binding by 65-70% and lowers the density of the binding site by 53-61%. Similarly, preincubation of membranes with a 10(-4) M concentration of either GTP gamma S or GppNHp significantly increases the KD value and reduces the Bmax value of [3H]8-OH-DPAT binding. These results indicate that GTP gamma S and GppNHp induce persistent changes in 5-HT1A receptor-G protein interactions that are reflected as a decrease in the density of binding sites labeled by [3H]8-OH-DPAT.  相似文献   

14.
Properties of the penicillin-binding proteins of Escherichia coli K12,.   总被引:177,自引:0,他引:177  
Benzyl[14C]penicillin binds to six proteins with molecular weights between 40000 and 91000 in the inner membrane of Escherichia coli. Two additional binding proteins with molecular weights of 29000 and 32000 were sometimes detected. All proteins were accessible to benzyl[14C]penicillin in whole cells. Proteins 5 and 6 released bound benzyl[14C]penicillin with half times of 5 and 19 min at 30 degrees C but the other binding proteins showed less than 50% release during a 60-min period at 30 degrees C. The rate of release of bound penicillin from some of the proteins was greatly stimulated by 2-mercaptoethanol and neutral hydroxylamine. Release of benzyl[14C]penicillin did not occur if the binding proteins were denatured in anionic detergent and so was probably enzymic. No additional binding proteins were detected with two [14C]cephalosporins. These beta-lactams bound to either all or some of those proteins to which benzyl[14C]penicillin bound. No binding proteins have been detected in the outer membrane of E coli with any beta-[14C]lactam. The binding of a range of unlabelled penicillins and cephalosporins were studied by measuring their competition for the binding of benzyl[14C]penicillin to the six penicillin-binding proteins. These results, together with those obtained by direct binding experiments with beta-[14C]lactams, showed that penicillins bind to all six proteins but that at least some cephalosporins fail to bind, or bind very slowly, to proteins 2, 5 and 6, although they bind to the other proteins. Since these cephalosporins inhibited cell division and caused cell lysis at concentrations where we could detect no binding to proteins 2, 5 and 6, we believe that these latter proteins are not the target at which beta-lactams bind to elicit the above physiological responses. The binding properties of proteins 1, 3, and 4 correlate reasonably well with those expected for the above killing targets.  相似文献   

15.
Using [3H]corticosterone as a probe, corticosteroid-binding protein (CBP) was detected in eight out of eight isolates of Candida albicans, of both A and B serotypes. The apparent dissociation constant (Kd) in the various isolates ranged between 8 and 19 nM; the binding capacity varied from 122 to over 2400 fmol (mg cytosol protein)-1. There was no correlation between the amount or affinity of CBP and isolate virulence for murine hosts. Further analysis revealed demonstrable CBP in six out of six Candida species other than C. albicans. One isolate of C. tropicalis has been identified which fails to bind [3H]corticosterone. Saccharomyces cerevisiae, Neurospora crassa and Paracoccidioides brasiliensis also failed to bind [3H]corticosterone. Preliminary attempts were made to determine functions mediated by CBP in Candida, but in vitro growth, phase conversion and glucose oxidation by Candida were unaffected by the addition of a variety of steroid hormones. These data indicate that the presence of CBP in Candida does not correlate with either virulence or serotype. The physiological significance of CBP remains to be determined.  相似文献   

16.
For the maintenance of "killer" M1 double-stranded RNA in Saccharomyces cerevisiae, more than 30 chromosomal genes are required. The requirement for some of these genes can be completely suppressed by a cytoplasmic element, [B] (for bypass). We have isolated a mutant unable to maintain [B] (mab) and found that it is allelic to MAK10, one of the three chromosomal MAK genes required for the maintenance of L-A. The heat curing of [B] always coincided with the loss of L-A. To confirm that [B] is located on L-A, we purified viral particles containing either L-A or M1 from strains with or without [B] activity and transfected these purified particles into a strain which did not have either L-A or M1. The transfectants harboring L-A and M1 from a [B] strain showed the [B] phenotype, but the transfectants with L-A and M1 from a [B-o] strain did not show the [B] phenotype. Furthermore, the transfectants having L-A from a [B] strain and M1 from a [B-o] strain also showed the [B] phenotype. Therefore, we concluded that [B] is a property of a variant of L-A. In the transfection experiment, we also proved that the superkiller phenotype of the [B] strain is a property of L-A and that L-A with [B] activity can maintain a higher copy number of M1 regardless of the source of M1 viruslike particles. These data suggest that MAK genes whose mutations are suppressed by [B] are concerned with the protection of M1 (+) single-stranded RNA or the formation of M1 viruslike particles and that an L-A with more efficient production of M1 viruslike particles can completely dispense with the requirement for those MAK genes.  相似文献   

17.
Abstract: [3H]Aniracetam bound to specific and saturable recognition sites in membranes prepared from discrete regions of rat brain. In crude membrane preparation from rat cerebral cortex, specific binding was Na+ independent, was still largely detectable at low temperature (4°C), and underwent rapid dissociation. Scatchard analysis of [3H]aniracetam binding revealed a single population of sites with an apparent KD value of ~70 nM and a maximal density of 3.5 pmol/mg of protein. Specifically bound [3H]aniracetam was not displaced by various metabolites of aniracetam, nor by other pyrrolidinone-containing nootropic drugs such as piracetam or oxiracetam. Subcellular distribution studies showed that a high percentage of specific [3H]aniracetam binding was present in purified synaptosomes or mitochondria, whereas specific binding was low in the myelin fraction. The possibility that at least some [3H]aniracetam binding sites are associated with glutamate receptors is supported by the evidence that specific binding was abolished when membranes were preincubated at 37°C under fast shaking (a procedure that substantially reduced the amount of glutamate trapped in the membranes) and could be restored after addition of either glutamate or α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionate (AMPA) but not kainate. The action of AMPA was antagonized by DNQX, which also reduced specific [3H]aniracetam binding in unwashed membranes. High levels of [3H]aniracetam binding were detected in hippocampal, cortical, or cerebellar membranes, which contain a high density of excitatory amino acid receptors. Although synaptosomal aniracetam binding sites may well be associated with AMPA-sensitive glutamate receptors, specifically bound [3H]aniracetam could not be displaced by cyclothiazide or GYKI 52466, which act as a positive and negative modulator of AMPA receptors, respectively.  相似文献   

18.
19.
Although elevated plasma concentrations of lipoprotein [a] (Lp[a]) are considered to be a risk factor for atherosclerosis, the mechanisms by which Lp[a] mediates its pathogenic effects have not been conclusively determined. The apolipoprotein [a] (apo[a]) component of Lp[a] confers unique structural properties to this lipoprotein, including the ability to bind to lysine residues in biological substrates. It has been shown, however, that only a fraction of plasma Lp[a] (Lp[a]-Lys(+)) binds to lysine-Sepharose in vitro. The nature of the non-lysine-binding Lp[a] fraction in plasma (Lp[a]-Lys(-)) is currently unknown. In the present study, the Lp[a]-Lys(+) fraction was determined in the plasma of six unrelated individuals; the Lp[a]-Lys(+) fraction in these plasma samples ranged from approximately 37 to approximately 48%. Interestingly, purification of the Lp[a] by density gradient ultracentrifugation followed by gel filtration and ion-exchange chromatography resulted in progressive increases in the Lp[a]-Lys(+) fraction. Addition of either purified low density lipoprotein (LDL) or fibronectin to the purified Lp[a] at a 1:1 molar ratio reduced the Lp[a]-Lys(+) fraction (maximal decrease of 34 and 20%, respectively) whereas addition of both fibronectin and LDL to the purified Lp[a] resulted in a further decrease (45% maximally) in this fraction. Similar results were obtained by using a recombinant expression system for apo[a]: addition of a 4-fold molar excess of either LDL or fibronectin to conditioned medium containing metabolically labeled recombinant apo[a] reduced the Lys(+) fraction by 49 and 23%, respectively.Taken together, our data suggest that the lysine-binding heterogeneity of plasma Lp[a] is not primarily an intrinsic property of the lipoprotein, but rather results in large part from its ability to noncovalently associate with abundant plasma components such as LDL and fibronectin. These interactions appear to mask the lysine-binding site in apo[a] kringle IV type 10, which mediates the interaction of Lp[a] with lysine-Sepharose. The contribution of these interactions to the function of Lp[a] in vivo remains to be investigated.  相似文献   

20.
《Inorganica chimica acta》1986,122(2):193-198
Transition metal compounds having liquid crystalline properties can be interesting materials for practical applications. Attempting to correlate mesomorphic properties with molecular structure and crystal packing mode, we have investigated some complexes obtained from Schiff bases of long chain aliphatic amines and salicylaldehyde or 2,3-dihydroxybenzaldehyde derivatives. The X-ray structural analysis of bis[N-(n-butyl)(3-benzyloxy)-2-salicylideneaminato] palladium [II] is also reported.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号