首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
The rearing condition necessary for behavioral compensation after sensory deprivation was investigated in the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus. The right-cercus-ablated cricket was reared in a glass vial with a slightly larger diameter than the body length of the cricket. After two weeks of rearing in the vial, the air-puff-evoked escape behavior of the cricket was investigated. The response rate (relative occurrence of the escape behavior after a standard air puff) obtained was identical with that of crickets reared in a large cage. On the other hand, unlike crickets reared in a large cage, the distorted escape directional property of the cricket reared in the vial was not compensated at all. Control experiments proved that the restraint in the vial did not affect the motor system, and the air motion from environments was not essential for the compensational recovery of the escape direction. Therefore, the ablated crickets required spontaneous walking in order to compensate the directionality of their escape. A self-generated wind caused by spontaneous walking appears necessary for the crickets to realize the defect of their sensory system and to compensate the related escape behavior. A hypothesis for the compensation mechanism based on the efference copy signal is proposed.  相似文献   

2.
Unilaterally cercus-ablated crickets, Gryllus bimaculatus, were reared in an apparatus that induced walking artificially. In the crickets that experienced different distances of enforced walking per day, the directionality of escape was investigated before and 1, 4, 7, 10, 13, and 16 days after the ablation of the cercus. The crickets that walked a longer distance per day showed a quicker and a higher degree of compensational recovery of the escape direction than the crickets that walked a shorter distance per day, even after walking the same distance. Thus, the time course and amount of compensational recovery from cercal ablation depend on when crickets experience walking during the recovery period. During the recovery period, unilaterally cercus-ablated crickets were subjected to walking at different times to determine the most effective period in which walking affects the compensational recovery of escape direction. The compensational recovery of the escape direction occurred only in crickets experiencing walking in early periods after the ablation. In particular, walking experienced 2-6 days after the ablation was most effective for the compensational recovery. On the other hand, no compensational recovery occurred in crickets experiencing walking in later periods after the ablation. These results suggest that there is a sensitive or critical period in which walking affects the compensational recovery of escape direction.  相似文献   

3.
The effects of self-generated wind on the compensational recovery of escape direction were investigated in unilaterally cercus-ablated crickets, Gryllus bimaculatus. To separate walking and self-generated wind during walking, unilaterally cercus-ablated crickets were placed on a styrofoam ball that was easily rotated by leg motion during walking. The stationary walking on the ball did not produce self-generated wind, because no body motion occurred. Crickets that were trained on the ball but given no artificial air puff for 14 days after cercal ablation did not show any compensational recovery of escape direction. Therefore, spontaneous walking itself was not sufficient to compensate the wind-evoked escape direction in the crickets. Artificial air puffs from the anterior direction synchronized with the stationary walking were effective for the compensational recovery of escape direction, but those from the posterior direction were not. As most of the spontaneous walking was directed to the forward direction, only an artificial air puff from the anterior direction coincided well with actual self-generated wind occurring during the onset of normal walking. Therefore, self-generated wind during walking seems essential for the compensational recovery of escape direction in unilaterally cercus-ablated crickets. When artificial air puffs were unsynchronized with walking, no compensational recovery was observed. This result suggests that artificial air puffs should be given just after the onset of spontaneous walking. Otherwise, the artificial air puffs may not be recognized as self-generated wind.  相似文献   

4.
One of a pair of cerci was ablated in the first-, fourth- and last-instar nymphs of the cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus. The insects were then reared until the final molt, after which the intensity-response (I-R) relationships for four giant interneurons (GIs) 8-1, 9-1, 9-2 and 9-3 with regard to a controlled air current stimulus were measured. In order to examine the functional changes during postembryonic development and the differences in the physiological plasticity of GIs between nymphs and adults, the obtained I-R curves for each GI were compared with those measured in intact and unilaterally cercus-ablated adult crickets. Each GI showed a distinctive change in response magnitudes after the long-term unilateral cercal ablation. In most cases, the I-R curves for each GI in the crickets ablated from nymphal periods were different from those in the adult crickets mentioned above. Moreover, the pattern of change in response magnitude was different from GI to GI. In contrast to these observations, it was reported that some important characteristics of the wind-evoked escape behavior such as relative occurrence and escape direction in unilaterally cercus-ablated crickets investigated after a long-term rearing were almost identical with those in intact crickets. Therefore, the results obtained in the present study suggest that functional changes occur not only in GIs but also in many other neural elements in the escape-eliciting system in order to maintain the features of wind-evoked escape behavior.  相似文献   

5.
The effects of rearing conditions on the functional recovery of wind-sensitive giant interneurons (GIs) after unilateral cercal ablation were investigated in the cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus. Crickets were reared in a glass vials to prohibit free walking for 14 days after unilateral cercal ablation ("14-day vial" crickets). Other crickets were reared in an apparatus called a "walking inducer" (WI) to increase the walking distance during the same 14-day period ("14-day WI" crickets). In these crickets, the response properties of GIs 8-1, 9-1, 9-2, and 9-3 to air currents from various directions were investigated. From the intensity-response curves obtained, directionality curves expressed in terms of threshold velocity and response magnitude were made independently. To understand changes in the functional recovery of GIs more thoroughly, the directional characteristics of GIs in crickets 1 day after unilateral cercal ablation ("1-day free" crickets) were also compared. Between the 1-day free and 14-day vial crickets, all the GIs showed differences in both threshold velocity and response magnitude for some stimulus directions. Between the 14-day vial and 14-day WI crickets, differences in the threshold velocities of GIs 9-1, 9-2, and 9-3, and in the response magnitudes of GIs 8-1, 9-1, and 9-3 were detected. Because the rearing condition after unilateral cercal ablation largely affects the compensatory recovery in some parameters of wind-evoked escape behavior, such as relative occurrence and escape direction, we discuss the functional differences in GIs revealed here in relation to the roles of GIs in the neural system that controls escape behavior.  相似文献   

6.
The wind-evoked escape behavior of the cricket Gryllodes sigillatus was investigated using an air puff stimulus. A high velocity air puff elicited the escape behavior in many crickets. The crickets tended to escape away from the stimulus source, but the direction was not accurately oriented 180 degrees from the stimulus. After bilateral cercal ablation, only a few crickets showed wind-evoked escape behavior, and their response rates did not increase even 19 days after ablation. Therefore, information on air motion detected by cercal filiform hairs is essential for triggering wind-evoked behavior. After unilateral cercal ablation, the 81.3% response rate of intact crickets decreased to 16.5%, that is, it decreased to almost 20% that of intact crickets. One week after unilateral cercal ablation, the response rate recovered to more than 60% that of intact crickets. However, the accuracy rate of the escape direction of G. sigillatus showed no change even immediately after the unilateral cercal ablation. Therefore, both cerci are not necessarily required to determine the escape direction. The behavioral characteristics of wind-evoked escape of G. sigillatus are compared with those of another species of cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus. The two species of cricket employ different strategies for wind-evoked escape.  相似文献   

7.
We studied behavioral responses of the cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus, to mechanical stimulation of the hindwing tip using three different kinds of stimuli: touching, bending and pinching. The most characteristic was a sequence of initial jump-like movements and subsequent running steps, that is referred to as escape behavior in this study. Touching stimulus elicited the escape behavior in 52% of resting animals tethered on a treadmill, whereas bending elicited the same behavior in 94% or 98% depending on the bend direction. Pinching was effective in all tested animals. The effectiveness of pinching stimulus in eliciting the escape behavior depends on the ongoing activity in the animal. Video and electromyographic recordings have revealed that, in the initial jump-like movements, forelegs and hindlegs move simultaneously on both sides while midlegs remain on the ground, followed by simultaneous movements of bilateral midlegs. The subsequent stepping was characterized by out-of-phase rhythmical activities of the leg muscles. Touching stimulus evoked tonic afferent responses of small amplitude in the second nerve root of the metathoracic ganglion. Bending stimulus evoked tonic responses of large units that showed rapid habituation and medium units that persisted during repeated stimulation. Pinching stimulus elicited only phasic responses of large and medium amplitude in the R2 afferents. The results suggest that touching, bending and pinching stimuli are transmitted to the metathoracic ganglion via different sensory systems having different effectiveness in activating the escape motor system.  相似文献   

8.
ABSTRACT. The precision of auditory lateralization was determined behaviourally for the cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus L. A forced-choice Y-maze test was devised in which the cricket, on entering the test arena, could not — in contrast to free phonotactic approaches — change its walking direction until after it had passed through a narrow wire-mesh tunnel. For a sound frequency of 4.7 kHz, matching the species' calling frequency, the minimum audible angle for correct side discrimination was 15°. For stimulus angles smaller than 15° from the longitudinal body axis, the crickets walked randomly to either side; stimulus angles greater than 25° resulted in all crickets turning correctly. These data reveal a sharply tuned lateral sensitivity for the auditory pathway of crickets, with an optimum at the species' calling frequency of 4.7 kHz (when compared with 3.5 and 6.0 kHz). The results for the forced-choice test are compared with the walking pattern during free phonotactic approaches, in order to determine the possible strategy underlying the acoustic orientation behaviour of the cricket.  相似文献   

9.
Tethered, flying crickets perform stereotyped steering movements when stimulated with sound. Electrophysiological recordings show that identifiable motoneurones respond to acoustic stimulation in a manner which reflects the phonotactic behaviour of the intact cricket. Both steering behaviour and its neural correlates reflect the fine temporal structure of the acoustic stimulus. Decapitation of the cricket eliminates steering movements in response to sound.  相似文献   

10.
A chamber to monitor mole cricket behavior was designed using two different soil-filled containers and photosensors constructed from infrared emitters and detectors. Mole crickets (Scapteriscus spp.) were introduced into a center tube that allowed them to choose whether to enter and tunnel in untreated soil or soil treated with Beauveria bassiana (Balsamo) Vuillemin. Each time the cricket passed through the photosensor located near the entrance of soil-filled containers, the infrared light was blocked and the exact moment that this occurred was logged onto a computer using custom-written software. Data examined included the first photosensor trigger, total number of sensor triggers, presence of tunneling, and final location of the cricket after 18 h. These behaviors were analyzed to discern differences in mole cricket behavior in the presence of different treatments and to elucidate the mechanism that mole crickets use to detect fungal pathogens. The first study examined substrate selection and tunneling behavior of the southern mole cricket, Scapteriscus borellii Giglio-Tos, to the presence of five strains of B. bassiana relative to a control. There were no differences between the first sensor trigger and total number of triggers, indicating the mole crickets are not capable of detecting B. bassiana at a distance of 8 cm. Changes in mole cricket tunneling and residence time in treated soil occurred for some strains of B. bassiana but not others. One of the strains associated with behavioral changes in the southern mole cricket was used in a second experiment testing behavioral responses of the tawny mole cricket, S. vicinus Scudder. In addition to the formulated product of this strain, the two separate components of that product (conidia and carrier) and bifenthrin, an insecticide commonly used to control mole crickets, were tested. There were no differences in mole cricket behavior between treatments in this study. The differences in behavioral responses between the two species could suggest a more sensitive chemosensory recognition system for southern mole crickets.  相似文献   

11.
Since the discovery of adult neurogenesis, a major issue is the role of newborn neurons and the function-dependent regulation of adult neurogenesis. We decided to use an animal model with a relatively simple brain to address these questions. In the adult cricket brain as in mammals, new neurons are produced throughout life. This neurogenesis occurs in the main integrative centers of the insect brain, the mushroom bodies (MBs), where the neuroblasts responsible for their formation persist after the imaginal molt. The rate of production of new neurons is controlled not only by internal cues such as morphogenetic hormones but also by external environmental cues. Adult crickets reared in an enriched sensory environment experienced an increase in neuroblast proliferation as compared with crickets reared in an impoverished environment. In addition, unilateral sensory deprivation led to reduced neurogenesis in the MB ipsilateral to the lesion. In search of a functional role for the new cells, we specifically ablated MB neuroblasts in young adults using brain-focused gamma ray irradiation. We developed a learning paradigm adapted to the cricket, which we call the "escape paradigm." Using this operant associative learning test, we showed that crickets lacking neurogenesis exhibited delayed learning and reduced memory retention of the task when olfactory cues were used. Our results suggest that environmental cues are able to influence adult neurogenesis and that, in turn, newly generated neurons participate in olfactory integration, optimizing learning abilities of the animal, and thus its adaptation to its environment. Nevertheless, odor learning in adult insects cannot always be attributed to newly born neurons because neurogenesis is completed earlier in development in many insect species. In addition, many of the irradiated crickets performed significantly better than chance on the operant learning task.  相似文献   

12.
ABSTRACT. Freely walking crickets were filmed from above during their visual orientation towards a black stripe. A frame-by-frame analysis enabled head and body movements to be recorded. The animals walk in 200ms bouts (runs) separated by pauses of similar duration. During each run, rotations of the body axis are observed and some corrections of the course direction occur between successive runs. Generally, the crickets do not walk straight ahead but slightly sideways. Because no lateral head movements were observed during visually orientated locomotion, retinal scanning results from both rotations of the body axis and translation of the head. While walking, one of the target edges is maintained by the cricket on a relatively limited area of the retina, generally between 10 and 25 laterally. Thus, the cricket often records three pieces of information about each edge: one in the monocular visual field, and two in the binocular visual field. Nevertheless, between two pauses, the images of each edge shift asymmetrically on the retinae. Such movement could prevent receptor adaptation by modulation of the ommatidial excitation, or by stimulation of the neighbouring ommatidia. It is also suggested that antennal movements are influenced by the positions of the visually fixated target edges.  相似文献   

13.
The response properties of four wind-sensitive giant interneurons (GIs) 8-1, 9-1, 9-2 and 9-3 in the fourth-instar nymphs of the cricket Gryllus bimaculatus were investigated to clarify the differences and/or similarities of the escape eliciting neural system between nymphs and adults. Air current was presented to the animal from 12 different directions in the horizontal plane, and the intensity-response curves for each GI were obtained at each stimulus direction. The intensity-response curves showed that the response magnitudes of GI 8-1 in the fourth-instar crickets increased with stimulus velocity up to 300 mm/s regardless of the stimulus direction. The response magnitudes of GI 9-1 in the nymphs reached a plateau at a stimulus velocity of 30 mm/s in most stimulus directions. The response magnitudes of GIs 9-2 and 9-3 increased with stimulus velocity up to 300 mm/s regardless of the stimulus direction. The directional sensitivity curves plotted on the basis of threshold velocities revealed that the preferential directions of the GIs were the ipsilateral-side in GI 8-1, the ipsilateral-front and contralateral-rear in GI 9-1, the ipsilateral-rear in GI 9-2 and the ipsilateral-front in GI 9-3, designated with respect to the side of the ventral nerve cord containing the axons. Although the GIs in nymphs occasionally showed higher threshold velocities and larger response magnitudes, the directional sensitivities, i.e., the preferred directions, of the GIs were basically the same with those of adults.  相似文献   

14.
ABSTRACT. Cave-crickets of the genus Troglophilus exhibit a slope dependent gating of locomotor behaviour (Kastberger, 1982), which is mainly expressed by a suppression of evasive jumping on inclined surfaces. A similar dependence of the jumpting rate on the ground inclination to that seen in the cave-cricket can be shown in the African cricket Phaeophyllacris and in the grasshopper Chorthippus. They differ only in the basic motivation for jumping. In this study, free-moving and tethered cave-crickets were studied to elucidate the inclination-sensitive control of gating locomotor activity. Cave-crickets with cauterized tarsal nerves exhibit only a 20% reduction in jumpting rate at slopes up to 45 compared with intact crickets. Freely moving crickets with compensated body weights show a suppression of evasive jumping which is complete if the vertical force overrides the body weight. Dorsally tethered cave-crickets which contact a rotary globe with their tarsi, show a significant increase in the latency of jerking beyond 60 inclination. Crickets placed on a floating boat and partially fixed respond to both static and dynamic forces applied to the longitudinal axis of the cricket. Static towing forces induce passive movements and the rate of active balancing are linearly correlated to the force applied. Under dynamic forces the rate of balancing is dependent on the longitudinal position of the legs and on the direction of force. This basic relation is changed by additional stimulation such as light and vibration. Locomotor activity is gated in a different mode, if the light interval is placed during forward or during backward forcing. Light-off suppresses locomotor activity. The results of dynamic forcing suggest the existence of a gain control which might be responsible for modal and temporal effects in gating locomotion.  相似文献   

15.
Learning appears to be ubiquitous among animals, as it plays a key role in many behaviors including foraging and reproduction. Although there is some genetic basis for differences in learning ability and memory retention, environment also plays an important role, as it does for any other trait. For example, adult animals maintained in enriched housing conditions learn faster and remember tasks for longer than animals maintained in impoverished conditions. Such plasticity in adult learning ability has often been linked to plasticity in the brain, and studies aimed at understanding the mechanisms, stimuli, and consequences of adult behavioral and brain plasticity are numerous. However, the role of experiences during post-embryonic development in shaping plasticity in adult learning ability and memory retention remain relatively unexplored. Using the house cricket (Acheta domesticus) as a model organism, we developed a protocol to allow the odor preference of a large number of crickets to be tested in a short period of time. We then used this new protocol to examine how enrichment or impoverishment at two developmental stages (either the last nymphal instar or young adult) affected adult memory. Our results show that regardless of nymphal rearing conditions, crickets that experienced an enriched rearing condition as young adults performed better on a memory task than individuals that experienced an impoverished condition. Older adult crickets (more than 1 week post adult molt) did not demonstrate differences in memory of the odor task, regardless of rearing condition as a young adult. Our results suggest that environmentally-induced plasticity in memory may be restricted to the young adult stage.  相似文献   

16.
Phenotypic integration and plasticity are central to our understanding of how complex phenotypic traits evolve. Evolutionary change in complex quantitative traits can be predicted using the multivariate breeders’ equation, but such predictions are only accurate if the matrices involved are stable over evolutionary time. Recent study, however, suggests that these matrices are temporally plastic, spatially variable and themselves evolvable. The data available on phenotypic variance‐covariance matrix ( P ) stability are sparse, and largely focused on morphological traits. Here, we compared P for the structure of the complex sexual advertisement call of six divergent allopatric populations of the Australian black field cricket, Teleogryllus commodus. We measured a subset of calls from wild‐caught crickets from each of the populations and then a second subset after rearing crickets under common‐garden conditions for three generations. In a second experiment, crickets from each population were reared in the laboratory on high‐ and low‐nutrient diets and their calls recorded. In both experiments, we estimated P for call traits and used multiple methods to compare them statistically (Flury hierarchy, geometric subspace comparisons and random skewers). Despite considerable variation in means and variances of individual call traits, the structure of P was largely conserved among populations, across generations and between our rearing diets. Our finding that P remains largely stable, among populations and between environmental conditions, suggests that selection has preserved the structure of call traits in order that they can function as an integrated unit.  相似文献   

17.
The extent to which diet and environment influence gut community membership (presence or absence of taxa) and structure (individual taxon abundance) is the subject of growing interest in microbiome research. Here, we examined the gut bacterial communities of three cricket groups: (1) wild caught field crickets, (2) laboratory‐reared crickets fed cat chow, and (3) laboratory‐reared crickets fed chemically defined diets. We found that both environment and diet greatly altered the structure of the gut bacterial community. Wild crickets had greater gut microbial diversity and higher Firmicutes to Bacteroidetes ratios, in contrast to laboratory‐reared crickets. Predictive metagenomes revealed that laboratory‐reared crickets were significantly enriched in amino acid degradation pathways, while wild crickets had a higher relative abundance of peptidases that would aid in amino acid release. Although wild and laboratory animals differ greatly in their bacterial communities, we show that the community proportional membership remains stable from Phylum to Family taxonomic levels regardless of differences in environment and diet, suggesting that endogenous factors, such as host genetics, have greater control in shaping gut community membership.  相似文献   

18.
When attacked, crickets may shed or ‘autotomize’ an entrapped limb in order to escape a would‐be predator. We examined the relationship between limb autotomy, running speed and susceptibility to future predation in house crickets (Acheta domestica). Hind limb autotomy resulted in a significant reduction in escape speed and ability to jump during the escape run, and greater predation by both lizards (striped skink Mabuya striata punctatissima) and mice (pouched mouse Saccostomus campestris). Although limb autotomy may enable a house cricket to escape a predatory encounter, autotomy of even one hind limb results in immediate costs to escape speed in crickets and makes the animal more vulnerable to subsequent predator encounters.  相似文献   

19.
The mangrove cricket Apteronemobius asahinai shows a circatidal rhythm in its locomotor activity, and this rhythm was shown to be entrained to artificial tidal cycles in the laboratory. To examine the Zeitgeber for this rhythm, in the present study, crickets were fixed with insect pins to prevent their body locomotion and a water stimulus was given to them by soaking in water, while recording their locomotor activities. A single water stimulus delayed the phase when given in the middle subjective low tide and advanced the phase when given in the later subjective low tide, whereas it had only a slight effect in the subjective high tide. We conclude that contact with water functions as a Zeitgeber for the circatidal rhythm.  相似文献   

20.
The aim of this paper is to show the activity cage as a viable method for tracking functional nerve recovery. The activity cage measures spontaneous coordinate activity, meaning movement in either the horizontal or vertical plane, of experimental animals within a specified amount of time. This uses a minimum of researcher time conducting functional testing to determine functional recovery of the nerve. Using microsurgical forceps, a crush injury was inflicted unilaterally, on the left side, upon the 4-month-old C3H mice creating a very high degree of pressure for 6 s upon the exposed sciatic nerve. The locomotion function of the mice was evaluated using the activity cage preoperatively, 1, 7, 14, 21, and 28 days after the surgical procedure. We found that using the activity cage functional recovery occurred by 14 days after nerve crush injury. It was also shown that, coinciding with functional recovery, immunohistochemistry changes for GD1a and nNOS appeared at the level of L4, where the sciatic nerve joins the spinal column. GD1a and nNOS have both been linked to regenerative processes in mammalian nervous systems.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号