共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Kenji Kawanaka 《Primates; journal of primatology》1973,14(2-3):113-159
Intertroop relationships among Japanese monkeys, which have been paid only scant attention for the past 20 years, are examined from several points of view. Japanese monkey troops are generally distributed in such a way as to concentrate locally, that is, to form a local concentration of troops (LCT). About 20% of the nomadic ranges of the troops within LCT's overlap; but in their natural state, they seldom approach but rather to avoid one another. From observations of intertroop encounters at Takasakiyama, where three troops are provisionized and use the same feeding place, it has been found that there exists a dominant-subordinate relationship among troops, that monkeys of each troop have a clear consciousness of belonging to their troop, and that monkeys of different troops rate one another on the basis of their capability. The frequency of male transfer between troops within LCT's is by far higher than between LCT's. In Japanese monkey society, a troop only is a social unit and a social order higher than a troop is not seen; however, it is not impossible to consider an LCT a consanguineally connected group by reason of the transfer of males among the troops within it. 相似文献
2.
R M Seyfarth 《Journal of theoretical biology》1977,65(4):671-698
Grooming networks among adult female monkeys exhibit two similar features across a number of different species. High-ranking animals receive more grooming than others, and the majority of grooming occurs between females of adjacent rank. A theoretical model which duplicates these features is presented, and the properties of the model are used to explain the possible causation and function of female grooming behaviour. The model illustrates how relatively simple principles governing the behaviour of individuals may be used to explain more complex aspects of the social structure of non-human primate groups. 相似文献
3.
S. A. Mehlenbacher M. M. Thompson 《TAG. Theoretical and applied genetics. Theoretische und angewandte Genetik》1988,76(5):669-672
Summary Pollen-stigma compatibility relationship were studied in 50 cultivars and more than 800 seedlings of the European hazelnut (Corylus avellana L.). A total of 22 unique S-alleles have been identified. Dominance relationships in 75 of the possible 231 pairs of alleles have been determined in both pistil and pollen. In the pistil, all alleles exhibited independent action, whereas in the pollen, alleles exhibited either dominance or codominance. The dominance relationship was linear with 7 levels of dominance.Oregon Agricultural Experiment Station Technical Paper No. 8542 相似文献
4.
Richard Schuster Michael J. Raleigh Michael T. McGuire David Torigoe 《American journal of primatology》1993,31(2):111-127
We examined the influences of dyadic relationships among captive adult male vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus) on behavior directed toward caged “intruder” males placed inside subjects' enclosures. Subjects were all 9 adult male residents from three stable social groups, each of which contained 3 adult males, at least 3 adult females, and their immature offspring. Every male was observed in two 3-hour sessions, each time with one of the 2 other adult males from his group. Observation sessions consisted of six consecutive 30-min stages in which group composition and the presence of the intruder were manipulated. All groups exhibited a stable, linear male dominance hierarchy prior to and throughout the study. In each group, there was one pair of males, when together, in which each member exhibited higher rates of intruder-directed approach and aggressive behaviors than when either animal was paired with the third male of his social group. Such pairs were also distinguished by high levels of within-pair agonistic interactions. The higher-ranking member of each dyad was the most aggressive male toward the intruder in his social group, although only one of these animals was the dominant male of his group. Mutual facilitation of aggression against intruding males is interpreted as cooperative behavior benefitting both males by increasing the likelihood of repelling a potential competitor for resident females. Such cooperation provides further evidence in nonhuman primates for cohesive male-male dyads between animals whose social interactions are characterized by agonism. © 1993 Wiley-Liss, Inc. 相似文献
5.
The concept of dominance has been shown to be useful in describing and predicting social interactions in group-living animals.
However, the dominance relationships among adult hamadryas males (Papio hamadryas) are poorly understood, and systematic data are missing from the literature. This study investigated dominance relationships
among male hamadryas baboons by applying novel statistical techniques to systematic detailed data on agonistic interactions.
We also analyzed the dominance relationship with male age and evaluated the association between dominance and access to mating
partners (i.e. the number of adult females per one-male unit (OMU)), food resources (i.e. monopolization of feeding areas),
and greeting interactions. The derived dominance indexes showed that, in general, leader males were dominant over followers,
and that dominance ability did not correlate with male age. Individual dominance values were very close to each other, suggesting
that dominance relationships among hamadryas males were not very rigid. In addition, dominance values were positively correlated
with number of adult females per OMU but not with feeding priority. Finally, greeting interactions occurred more frequently
between individuals with similar dominance values and with low levels of dominance decidedness. We suggest the need of further
studies, especially in wild populations, to confirm our findings. 相似文献
6.
7.
Margaret R. Clarke 《Primates; journal of primatology》1978,19(3):517-524
This study reports observations on juvenile female bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata) in a field cage environment. Interaction and proximity data collected on different age classes of juvenile females indicate distinct changes in social behavior from the time a female is weaned until her first pregnancy. Behavioral development includes an increase in solitary behavior, an early disinterest in younger animals, and a surprisingly low interaction rate with relatives. Important aspects of ontogeny are lost in analyses that treat all juvenile females as a single age-class. This research was supported partially by US PHS Grant No. RR00169. 相似文献
8.
Nakamichi M 《Primates; journal of primatology》2003,44(3):239-246
The present study investigated the influence of dominance rank in combination with kinship on age-related differences in social
grooming among adult females in a free-ranging group of Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata). Eighty-three adult females were divided into six sub-groups according to age-class (younger: 5–9 years old; middle: 10–14 years
old; older: 15–22 years old) and dominance rank (high and low rank). The ratio of the number of unrelated females that each
female groomed to the total number of available unrelated females and grooming bouts which she gave to unrelated females decreased
with increasing age for both high- and low-ranking females, whereas age did not appear to affect corresponding values for
related females. On the other hand, compared with low-ranking females, high-ranking females of all age-classes received grooming
more often from a larger number of unrelated females. Moreover, older females of low rank received grooming less often from
a smaller number of unrelated females than younger females of low rank. These results indicate that with increasing age females
are more likely to concentrate on related females when they have grooming interactions with other females. This tendency seems
to be more apparent for low-ranking females. Moreover, the present findings also indicate that older high-ranking females
could maintain their social attractiveness as high as younger high-ranking females. 相似文献
9.
Magdalena Janus 《International journal of primatology》1989,10(3):243-261
Play, grooming, and proximity, and the degree to which these were reciprocated between pairs, were studied in immature sibling and nonsibling rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)in four established captive groups over two seasons. “Interaction reciprocity” and “partner reciprocity” were assessed for each dyad for each of the three behaviors. In play, interaction reciprocity was based on the ratio between the play initiations by each dyad member,in grooming on the ratio between the grooming durations by each dyad member, and in proximity on the relative responsibility for proximity maintenance. Two or three most frequent (top) partners for each behavior were found for each individual. If two monkeys were among each other’s top partners, they were said to be reciprocal partners. Monkeys played with nonsiblings as much as with siblings but spent more time grooming and in proximity with siblings than with nonsiblings. Same-age nonsiblings (peers) were more frequent partners than other nonsiblings for each behavior. Siblings’ grooming interactions were more reciprocal than those of nonsiblings. There was no such effect for play and proximity. All-male dyads tended to be more reciprocal in play interactions, and all-female dyads tended to be more reciprocal in grooming interactions. In play, but not in grooming or proximity, the interaction reciprocity of reciprocal partners was higher than that of nonreciprocal dyads. It is argued that the three behaviors have similar roles in infant’s social development but they differ in the expression of this role. Hence the reciprocity patterns vary with the behavior. 相似文献
10.
Different types of dominance hierarchies reflect different social relationships in primates. In this study, we clarified the hierarchy and social relationships in a one-male unit of captive Rhinopithecus bieti observed between August 1998 and March 1999. Mean frequency of agonistic behaviour among adult females was 0.13 interactions per hour. Adult females exhibited a linear hierarchy with a reversal of 10.9%, indicating an unstable relationship; therefore, R. bieti appears to be a relaxed/tolerant species. The lack of a relationship between the agonistic ratio of the adult male towards adult females and their ranks indicated that males did not show increased aggression towards low-ranking females. Differentiated female affiliative relationships were loosely formed in terms of the male, and to some extent influenced by female estrus, implying that relationships between the male and females is influenced by estrus and not rank alone. A positive correlation between the agonistic ratio of adult females and their ranks showed that the degree to which one female negatively impacted others decreased with reduction in rank. Similarly, a positive correlation between the agonistic ratio of females and differences in rank suggests that a female had fewer negative effects on closely ranked individuals than distantly ranked ones. These data indicate that rank may influence relationships between females. A steeper slope of regression between the agonistic ratio and inter-female rank differences indicated that the extent of the power difference in high-ranking females exerting negative effects on low-ranking ones was larger during the mating season than the birth season, suggesting that rank may influence the mating success of females. 相似文献
11.
Grooming among adult and older juvenile females in a wild group of blue monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni) is analyzed and related to agonistic behavior. Both grooming and agonistic behavior were less frequent than would be expected
in baboon and macaque groups. Fair shares of grooming were given and taken, both within and between interactions, in amost
dyads, and all females were seen to groom with at least half the others. The reciprovity of grooming was its most outstanding
feature. There was no evidence that grooming was preferentially directed at kin. Frequent grooming partnerships fell into
two clusters, one of which ranked higher than the other. We suggest that grooming throughout the group helps to establish
a coherent team of females which effectively, defends its territory in daily encounters at the boundaries with neighboring
groups. 相似文献
12.
13.
With a view to elucidate seasonal variations in testicular spermatogenesis, quantitative analysis of spermatogenic cells was carried out in non-human primate species viz. rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and bonnet (M. radiata) monkeys during breeding (October-December) and non-breeding (May-June) seasons. The results revealed significant inhibition of testicular germ cell population during non-breeding compared with the breeding period in both the species. Quantitative determination of Sertoli cell-germ cell ratio showed a marked decrease in the number of type A-spermatogonia, spermatocytes (non-pachytene and pachytene) and spermatids (in steps 1-12 of spermiogenesis) in rhesus monkey during the non-breeding period. Bonnet monkeys exhibited the significant decline in the number of primary spermatocytes and spermatids during the non-breeding phase. In addition, average diameter of round seminiferous tubules and nuclear diameter of Leydig cells also decreased significantly in rhesus monkeys. However, bonnet monkeys did not show any significant change in nuclear diameter/morphology of Leydig cells, testicular tubular diameter and number of type A-spermatogoniae. Sertoli cell number did not show any significant change during both breeding and non-breeding periods in both the species. The results of this study indicate a prominent seasonal variation in testicular spermatogenic/Leydig cells in rhesus monkeys than those observed in bonnet monkeys. 相似文献
14.
Opazo JC Wildman DE Prychitko T Johnson RM Goodman M 《Molecular phylogenetics and evolution》2006,40(1):274-280
Orthologous sequences of six nuclear genes were obtained for all recognized genera of New World monkeys (Primates: Platyrrhini) and outgroups to evaluate the phylogenetic relationships and to estimate divergence times. Phylogenetic relationships were reconstructed by maximum parsimony, maximum likelihood, and Bayesian approaches. All methods resolved with 100% branch support genus-level relationships, except for the grouping of Aotus as a sister taxa of Cebus and Saimiri, which was supported by low bootstrap percentages and posterior probability. All approaches depict three monophyletic New World monkey families: Atelidae, Cebidae, and Pitheciidae; also within each family, all approaches depict the same branching topology. However, the approaches differ in depicting the relationships of the three families to one another. Maximum parsimony depicts the Atelidae and Cebidae as sister families next joined by the Pitheciidae. Conversely, likelihood and Bayesian phylogenetic trees group families Atelidae and Pitheciidae together to the exclusion of Cebidae. Divergence time estimations using both local molecular clock and Bayesian approaches suggest the families diverged from one another over a short period of geological time in the late Oligocene-early Miocene. 相似文献
15.
The influences of socionomic sex ratio (SSR; adult males/adult female) and troop size upon male-male, female-female, and male-female
grooming relationships were examined and compared between two wild Japanese macaque troops (Kinkazan A and Yakushima M troops)
in Japan. The Yakushima M troop was smaller and had a higher-SSR than the Kinkazan A troop. Between the troops, (1) the male-male
grooming frequency and number of partners were greater in the Yakushima M troop than in the Kinkazan A troop; (2) the female-female
grooming frequency and number of partners were not different; and (3) the male-female grooming frequency and number of partners
were not different. Based on these features, the patterns of female-female and male-female grooming relationships appear to
be independent of SSR and troop size variations. In contrast, male-male grooming relationships are influenced by both factors,
especially SSR. Frequent grooming interactions among males may be useful for the continued coexistence of relatively many
males especially in a higher-SSR troop. 相似文献
16.
Enrique J. Abordo Russel A. Mittermeier Jerome Lee Paul Mason 《Primates; journal of primatology》1975,16(2):217-221
Young squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) were reported grooming an adult female uakari (Cacajao calvus rubicundus) on four different occasions. Furthermore, the uakari was noted grooming two squirrel monkeys in separate instances. These observations took place in a seminatural rainforest (The Monkey Jungle; Goulds, Florida, U.S.A.) where provisions are provided. Some possible hypostheses tendered to account for this unusual behavior included (a) the unaverted interaction of food-seeking and fur-cleaning behavior, and (b) the compatibility of play-curiosity activities by squirrel monkeys with the uakaris' need for social contact. 相似文献
17.
18.
David F. Raney Virginia Abernethy Peter S. Rodman 《Primates; journal of primatology》1981,22(3):368-378
Female bonnet macaques are ranked according to naturally occurring dyadic aggressive interactions. Several possible correlates of these ranks are examined. Maternal rank and early life history are found to be highly correlated with rank. In addition, a number of social relationships (female/female presentations, female/female mounts, contact among females and aggression) are found to be related to rank. The meaning of rank outside of head to head encounters is discussed. This research was supported in part by the California Primate Research Center, University of California, Davis, Davis, California 95616. 相似文献
19.
Daily sperm production (DSP) rate was estimated in adult male rhesus and bonnet monkeys to evaluate seasonal changes in the gametogenic activity of the testes. Three monkeys of each species were castrated during breeding and non-breeding seasons and DSP rate was estimated by enumerating the homogenization-resistant spermatid nuclei of steps 13 and 14. Results indicated a significant reduction in the DSP rate per testis during the non-breeding season in two species, along with a marked decline in the testis weight. However, the gametogenic capacity of seminiferous tubules did not appear to be markedly affected during non-breeding season, as the DSP rate per gram parenchyma of testis was only marginally reduced. The seasonal changes in DSP were much more pronounced in the rhesus than in the bonnet monkey. The feasibility of circanual rhythm in DSP of sub-human primates to form a baseline for the study of reproductive function in male is discussed. 相似文献
20.
The social relationships of immature male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) were studied during 1977, 1978 and 1981 at the Cayo Santiago colony, Puerto Rico. Three types of social relationship were selected for comparison: the sibling relationship, the strong peer relationship and the weak peer relationship. Sibling relationships were initially distinguished from peer relationships on the basis of individual characteristics (age and relatedness), whereas the two types of peer relationship were initially differentiated on the basis of criteria that concerned their structure. Although the three types of relationship showed certain similarities these were far outweighed by differences in the content, diversity, qualities and the relative frequency and patterning of their interactions, and the degree to which their interactions were characterized as reciprocal or complementary. These findings are discussed in relation to a number of questions concerning the sturucture of relationships and it is concluded that, in broad terms, the three types of relationship can be contrasted in terms of a multidimensional quality, their degree of affiliation. 相似文献