首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
ABSTRACT. The precision of auditory lateralization was determined behaviourally for the cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus L. A forced-choice Y-maze test was devised in which the cricket, on entering the test arena, could not — in contrast to free phonotactic approaches — change its walking direction until after it had passed through a narrow wire-mesh tunnel. For a sound frequency of 4.7 kHz, matching the species' calling frequency, the minimum audible angle for correct side discrimination was 15°. For stimulus angles smaller than 15° from the longitudinal body axis, the crickets walked randomly to either side; stimulus angles greater than 25° resulted in all crickets turning correctly. These data reveal a sharply tuned lateral sensitivity for the auditory pathway of crickets, with an optimum at the species' calling frequency of 4.7 kHz (when compared with 3.5 and 6.0 kHz). The results for the forced-choice test are compared with the walking pattern during free phonotactic approaches, in order to determine the possible strategy underlying the acoustic orientation behaviour of the cricket.  相似文献   

2.
Periodic auditory click stimulation has been reported to elicit an auditory steady state response (ASSR). The ASSR has been suggested to reflect the efficiency of γ-amino butyric acid (GABA) inhibitory interneuronal activity. Although a potential role for GABAergic dysfunction has been previously proposed, the role of neural synchronization in the ASSR in people with bipolar disorder (BD) has received little attention. In the current study, we investigated ASSRs to 20 Hz, 30 Hz, 40 Hz and 80 Hz click trains in BD patients. A total of 14 (4 males) BD patients and 25 (10 males) healthy controls participated in this study. ASSRs were obtained using whole-head 306-channel magnetoencephalography to calculate, ASSR power values and phase locking factors (PLF). BD patients exhibited significantly reduced mean ASSR power and PLF values bilaterally at frequencies of 30, 40, and 80 Hz (p<0.05 for these frequencies). At 20 Hz, bipolar patients showed no significant reduction in mean ASSR power and PLF values. There was a significant negative correlation between 80 Hz-ASSR-power values obtained from the right hemisphere and scores on the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (rho = −0.86, p = 0.0003). The current study showed reduced low and high gamma band ASSR power and PLF bilaterally with no significant beta band ASSR reduction in BD patients. BD patients are characterized by deficits in gamma band oscillations, which may be associated with GABA inhibitory interneuronal activity dysfunction.  相似文献   

3.
In the absence of sensory stimuli, spontaneous activity in the brain has been shown to exhibit organization at multiple spatiotemporal scales. In the macaque auditory cortex, responses to acoustic stimuli are tonotopically organized within multiple, adjacent frequency maps aligned in a caudorostral direction on the supratemporal plane (STP) of the lateral sulcus. Here, we used chronic microelectrocorticography to investigate the correspondence between sensory maps and spontaneous neural fluctuations in the auditory cortex. We first mapped tonotopic organization across 96 electrodes spanning approximately two centimeters along the primary and higher auditory cortex. In separate sessions, we then observed that spontaneous activity at the same sites exhibited spatial covariation that reflected the tonotopic map of the STP. This observation demonstrates a close relationship between functional organization and spontaneous neural activity in the sensory cortex of the awake monkey.  相似文献   

4.
5.
Both femora of 364 individuals of southern African tribes and South Africans of European origin were analysed for population and side differences. Emphasis was placed on multivariate analyses, although uni-and bivariate statistics were also employed and discussed. While the latter analyses did not provide conclusive results, all multivariate tests were highly significant and demonstrated that differences between samples were predominantly due to shape variability, especially of the proximal part of the femur. Among southern African Negroes a morphological gradient seemed to be apparent between Sothos, Zulus and Xosas following a clinal pattern. Differences between southern African tribes and Europeans were marked, but may, at least in part, be accounted for by different environmental factors and activity levels. Side differences of femora were found to be pronounced in all populations studied and also showed a comparable morphological pattern between the samples. The left femur was generally more robust than the right indicating that it was better suited to withstand axial forces and medio-lateral bending. Thus, the findings lend support to the hypothesis that there exist a universal pattern of lateralization, whereby the left lower limb is commonly used for weight-bearing while the right one is more likely used for motor-tasks.  相似文献   

6.
The past year has seen some important advances in our understanding of central auditory function. Several central auditory neurons that differ from those in the periphery in their selectivity for various spectral and temporal parameters of complex sound have been described. Although central specializations for the processing of complex sound have been known for some time, recent findings suggest that these high-order filter properties are more widespread than previously thought. Significant progress has been made in our understanding of the neural mechanisms by which some high-order filter properties, such as delay-tuning, amplitude-tuning, and complex frequency tuning are performed by the central auditory system. New evidence has clarified the role of high-order auditory filters in auditory learning and perception, and the regions in which they are found.  相似文献   

7.
8.
Two experiments examined the trade-off between concentration and stimulus duration in nasal lateralization of n-ethyl alcohol. In nasal lateralization, a common measure of irritation threshold, subjects receive chemical vapor in one nostril and clean air in the other. Subjects try to determine which nostril received the chemical. Within experimental runs, subjects received fixed concentrations (1650-5000 ppm) of ethanol, and duration was varied to find the shortest, lateralizable stimulus. In Experiment 1, a small group of subjects was tested intensively to obtain stable individual data. In Experiment 2, a larger group was studied using more rapid methods. In both cases, subjects could lateralize increasingly weaker concentrations with longer stimulus presentations. Hence integration occurred. However, more than a twofold increase in duration was required to compensate for a twofold decrease in concentration to maintain threshold lateralization. These results suggest that an imperfect, mass-integrator model can describe short-term integration of nasal lateralization of ethanol.  相似文献   

9.
Functional asymmetries, for example, the preferential involvement of 1 brain hemisphere to process stimuli, may increase brain efficiency and the capacity to carry out tasks simultaneously. We investigated which hemisphere was primarily invoIved in processing acoustic stimuli in goats using a headorienting paradigm. Three playbacks using goat vocalizations recorded in different contexts: food anticipation (positive), isolation (negative), food frustration (negative), as well as 1 playback involving dog barks (negative) were presented on the left and right sides of the test subjects simultaneously. The head-orienting response (left or right) and latency to resume feeding were recorded. The direction of the head-orienting response did not differ between the various playbacks. However, when the head-orienting response was tested against chanee level, goats showed a right bias regardless of the stimuli presented. Goats responded more to dog barks than to food frustration calls, whereas responses to food anticipation and isolation calls were intermediate. In addition, the latency to resume feeding, an indicator of fear reaction, was not affected by the kind of vocalization presented. These results provide evidence for asymmetries in goat vocal perceptio n of emotional-li nked con specific and heterospecific calls. They also suggest involvement of the left brain hemisphere for processing acoustic stimuli, which might have been perceived as familiar and non -threate ning.  相似文献   

10.
Two parts of a geometrical figure are consecutively presented to healthy adult subjects in the left and right visual fields; the subjects have to compare them mentally and to decide whether these parts form a standard figure or not. Correctness of the reaction is controlled by a computer which lights up on the screen the words "good" or "error". The number of correct decisions of this visual-spatial task does not depend on the hemisphere to which information is addressed. The reaction time is substantially shorter if the information comes "directly" to the right hemisphere. Due to better training in the left hemisphere interhemispheric difference in reaction time gradually disappears in repeated tests. Training to mental "constructing" takes place only in the tests following positive feedback stimulus. Analysis of amplitude-temporal parameters of P300 wave shows that at correct decision of the visual-spatial task the level of activation in the right hemisphere is higher than in the left one.  相似文献   

11.
Lateralized behavior is widespread among vertebrate animals and is determined primarily by structural-functional brain asymmetry as well as by the presence of somatic and visceral asymmetry. Some kinds of asymmetric reactions are suggested to be due to the presence of asymmetry at the level of sense organs, in particular, of otolith organs. This review presents data on values and characters of otolith asymmetry (OA) in animals of various species and classes and on the effect of weightlessness and hypergravity on OA; the issue of the effect of OA on vestibular and auditory functions also is considered. In symmetric vertebrates, OA was shown to be fluctuating, and its coefficient χ ranges from ?0.2 to +0.2; in the overwhelming majority of individuals, |χ| < 0.06. The low OA level enables the paired otolith organs to work in coordination; this is why the OA level is equally low regardless of the individual taxonomic and ecological position, size, age, and otolith growth rate. Individuals with the abnormally high OA level can experience difficulties in analyzing auditory and vestibular stimuli; therefore, most of such individuals are eliminated by natural selection. Unlike symmetric vertebrates, labyrinths of many Pleuronectiformes have pronounced OA-otoliths in the lower labyrinth, on average, are significantly heavier than those in the upper labyrinth. The organs of flatfish represent the only example when OA, being directional, seem to play an essential role in lateralized behavior and are suggested to be used in the spatial localization of the source of sound. The short-term weightlessness and relatively weak hypergravity (≤ 2g) do not affect OA. However, it cannot be ruled out that the long-term weightlessness and hypergravity ≥ 3g as well as some diseases and age-related changes can enhance OA and cause some functional disturbances.  相似文献   

12.
The past year has seen significant advances in our understanding of the structural (circuitry and chemistry of synaptic connections) and functional characteristics of the auditory brainstem. Some of the findings that shed light on the mechanisms underlying complex auditory information processing are highlighted.  相似文献   

13.
We developed a rapid-scanning, light-scattering densitometer by which extensive measurements of band migration speeds and band profiles of chemotactic bands of Escherichia coli in motility buffer both with and without serine have been made. The purpose is to test the applicability of the phenomenological model proposed by Keller and Segel (J. Theor. Biol. 1971. 30:235) and to determine the motility (mu) and chemotactic (delta) coefficients of the bacteria. We extend the previous analytical solution of the simplified Keller-Segel model by taking into account the substrate diffusion which turns out to be significant in the case of oxygen. We demonstrate that unique sets of values of mu and delta can be obtained for various samples at different stages of migration by comparing the numerical solution of the model equation and the experimental data. The rapid-scanning technique also reveals a hitherto unobserved time-dependent fine structure in the bacterial band. We give a qualitative argument to show that the fine structure is an example of the dissipative structure that arises from a nonlinear coupling between the bacterial density and the oxygen concentration gradient. Implications for a further study of the dissipative structure in testing the Keller-Segel model of chemotaxis are briefly discussed.  相似文献   

14.
Frequency resolution and spectral filtering in the cat primary auditory cortex (AI) were mapped by extracellular recordings of tone responses in white noise of various bandwidths. Single-tone excitatory tuning curves, critical bandwidths, and critical ratios were determined as a function of neuronal characteristic frequency and tone level. Single-tone excitatory tuning curves are inadequate measures of frequency resolution and spectral filtering in the AI, because their shapes (in most neurons) deviated substantially from the shapes of “tuning curves for complex sound analysis”, the curves determined by the band limits of the critical bandwidths. Perceptual characteristics of spectral filtering (intensity independence and frequency dependence) were found in average critical bandwidths of neurons from the central and ventral AI. The highest frequency resolution (smallest critical bandwidths) reached by neurons in the central and ventral AI equaled the psychophysical frequency resolution. The dorsal AI is special, since most neurons there had response properties incompatible with psychophysical features of frequency resolution. Perceptual characteristics of critical ratios were not found in the average neuronal responses in any area of the AI. It seems that spectral integration in the way proposed to be the basis for the perception of tones in noise is not present at the level of the AI. Accepted: 21 July 1997  相似文献   

15.
16.
17.
Analysis of the lateralization phenomena of a fused auditory image (FAI) was performed on the basis of the previously developed model of the binaural directional hearing. It was found earlier that, under conditions of auditory stimulation without interaural differences, the FAI was localized at the head midline only in about a quarter of subjects. In a greater part of the listeners, the FAI was lateralized within the range of -4.6 ... +11.2 degrees from the midline. It was shown that FAI localization with reference to the head midline may be determined by the extent of asymmetry and spatial contrast between the "active" neural zones in the left and right halves of the subjective auditory space. In turn, the asymmetry (or its absence) of these "active" zones fully depends on a distribution of neurons by characteristic time delays in the left and right halves of the subjective auditory field. The model also explains the fact of a decrease in localization precision with the FAI position just at the midline.  相似文献   

18.
Brown C  Magat M 《Biology letters》2011,7(4):496-498
Individual preference for the use of one limb over the other to explore the environment or manipulate objects is common trait among vertebrates. Here, we explore the hypothesis that limb preference is determined by the engagement of a particular cerebral hemisphere to analyse certain stimuli. We recorded the eye and foot preferences of 322 individuals from 16 species of Australian parrots while investigating potential food items. Across all species, eye preferences explained 99 per cent of the variation in foot use in Australian parrots. The vast majority of species showed significant relationships between eye and foot preferences at the population level.  相似文献   

19.
Eight white New Zealand rabbits were submitted to auditory stimulation in order to obtain normative BAEP parameters. A monaural alternating 0.1 ms click stimulation at 20 Hz, 90 dB was used. Two series of 1000 responses were averaged (10 ms time-base, 160-3000 Hz band-pass) and highly reproducible peaks were obtained. Peaks P1, P2, P3, P4 were obtained in all ipsilateral recordings, whereas peak P5 was detectable in only 6 animals. In contralateral recordings P1 was absent and the following peaks were similar to those of ipsilateral recordings. Normative values of absolute and interpeak latencies, peak amplitudes and amplitude ratios were obtained. The procedure was repeated 24 hours after basal recordings and measures of test-retest variability were obtained.  相似文献   

20.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号