首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 375 毫秒
1.
Abstract

The proportion of time spent by South Island robins (Petroica australis australis) of different sex, age, and status giving full song, subsong, and downscales, on a monthly and diurnal basis, is described. Females never gave full song. Males did so throughout the year, but least while moulting. Since bachelors sang significantly more than paired males, song probably functioned largely to attract females. Breeding males sang most during the pre-laying stage. From January to April adult males sang significantly more than immature males, but from May to July the reverse was true. Robins sang mainly in the early morning. Robins generally spent less than 0.5% of time during any stage of the breeding cycle giving sub-song. Outside the breeding season, males spent significantly more time giving sub-song than females, and adult males sang more than immature males. Robins in all categories gave more sub-song during the moult than afterwards. The diurnal pattern of sub-song production was bimodal. About half the downscales were followed by no obvious action from the caller or response from other robins, nor were the calls preceded by an obvious event. About 25% of the downscales were associated with interactions between members of a pair; the remainder were associated with territorial defence. Downscales probably served to maintain contact between members of a pair and to advertise possession of territory. These calls were heard rarely in the breeding season, and most frequently in April. Males gave downscales more than females, but use of downscales by both sexes was evenly distributed through the day.  相似文献   

2.
Variation in singing behaviour between males can involve fixed differences such as the song type composition of repertoires, as well as more flexible effects such as matched counter-singing (Krebs & Kroodsma, 1980; Section III. 4), differences in bout length (the number of songs in a period of song) and changes in strophe length. Short-term strophe length changes seem to be related to the willingness and ability of males to respond strongly to playback. Whether this is because strophe length indicates motivation or the degree of exhaustion of the neuromuscular song-production system, or both, is currently unclear.  相似文献   

3.
Zusammenfassung Die kurze, nur aus drei Lauten bestehende Gesangsstrophe der Bartmeise (Panurus biarmicus), die bis jetzt nur vonKoenig (1951) bei Gefangenschaftsvögeln einige Male gehört worden war, wird von Männchen in verschiedenen Funktionen, z. B. für Paarbildung und Paarbindung, aber auch als Übersprungsverhalten und als funktionsloser sub-song das ganze Jahr über gebracht. An Hand von Sonagrammen wird der Gesang beschrieben. Das dritte Element der Strophe wurde bisher in der Literatur stets also Unmutslaut beschrieben, ohne daß die Autoren einen Zusammenhang mit einer Gesangsfunktion erkannt hätten. Um diesen tschrä-Laut hervorzubringen, müssen die Vögel eine Zwangshaltung einnehmen.
Summary Males of the Bearded Tit(Panurus biarmicus) produce a song consisting of three sounds So far, onlyKoenig (1951) has reported this song from caged birds. The song has different functions for pair formation and pair bond, as displacement activity and as functionless sub-song throughout the year. The song is described by sonagrams. Up to now, the third element of the song had been described as a distress call, because its connection with the song apparently had not been recognised. For producing the sound tschrä, the birds must perform in a constraint attitude.
  相似文献   

4.
R. G. B. Brown 《Ibis》1963,105(1):63-75
Willow Warblers Phylloscopus trochilus at Wytham, Oxford (51° 46' N., 1° 15' W.), start to sing around sunrise, in late April to early June. In the continuous daylight at Neiden, East Finnmark (69° 42' N., 29° 25' E.), they do not sing all the time, but in late June to late July start singing between 22.45 and 01.45 hrs., the start becoming progressively later as the season draws on. At Neiden, nest visits start some time after song (01.00–02.30), and this is probably also true at Wytham.
The influence of environmental variables on the starts of the two cycles is discussed. It is concluded that song starts at Wytham when light intensity has risen above a certain threshold level, and at Neiden, when it has reached a minimum value or has started to rise again. It is suggested that the Neiden birds react to the closest approximation of their light intensity cycle to that at dawn further south. The end of daily song at Neiden may be controlled by internal factors. It is suggested that the start of the Neiden nest-visit cycle is controlled by light intensity, but modified by temperature effects. The possibility that the environment affects the bird indirectly, via its brood or food, can be ruled out. The functional significance of the retention of activity lulls in the continuous daylight of the Arctic summer is discussed.
The daily song-starts of the Garden Warbler Sylvia borin , Brambling Fringilla montifringilla , and Yellowhammer Emberiza citrinella , follow the same pattern as those of the Willow Warbler. The Redwing Turdus musicus often has no daily song lull; its nest-visit lull begins and ends well before midnight. The significance of this is discussed.  相似文献   

5.
Fernando  Nottebohm 《Ibis》1968,110(4):549-568
The present paper describes the results of experiments designed to evaluate the importance of auditory-motor experience in the development of song in the Chaffinch. For this purpose a number of Chaffinches collected as nestlings or adults were deafened by extirpating the cochlea at different ages. Recordings of their song made subsequently provide the material on which the paper is based. Since the results of the experiments can only be interpreted in the light of knowledge of normal song development, the latter is described from the observation of previous workers and from some new material. Of the 20 males deafened, only three came into full song of their own accord; song was induced in the rest by the administration of testosterone phenylacetate or testosterone propionate. Songs of abnormal length were common among first-year males though males deafened when two or more years old produced song of the normal length. The song of male Chaffinches deafened as adults, that is after they had produced full song for one or more breeding seasons, was infistinguishable from that produced by intact adult males. They even went through a “plastic” song stage in which some “intermediate” themes occurred. A first-year male deafened when it had almost stabilized its song produced song that gradually deviated more and more from normal song. This indicates that the bird had not yet laid down a long-term memory of its song independent of auditory feedback. First-year male Chaffinches deafened during early spring, mid-winter and late sumer, respectively, show that the more they were deprived of the normal period of auditory-motor experience, the simpler was the song they produced in their first spring. Thus, at the time of deafening they had acquired a particular auditory-motor experience, and it is the type of element established up to that time that is incorporated into full song. Male chaffinches brought into the laboratory as nestlings and deafened when three months old produced a virtually sfyctureless song, in which the only recognizable element was a sound reminiscent of the juvenile “chirrup”. The subsong and other calls of these birds were also highly abnormal. Song development in the Chaffinch clearly starts before song as such has made its appearance. The available evidence suggests that the kind of song a male Chaffinch produces depends directly on its auditory-motor experience before deafening. There is no indication that deafening achieves its effects on the resultant song by interfering with the hormonal substrate. Relatively stable song end patterns develop in the absence of auditory feedback, so their occurrence does not necessitate the postulation of templates or preconceived patterns. Song development differs markedly from species to species.  相似文献   

6.
How does an animal know the appropriate context in which to use its different vocalizations? I studied this problem with a songbird, the blue-winged warbler (Vermivora pinus). Mated males in nature tend to sing one song form (II) at dawn, under low light levels, and at a median rate of 15 songs/min. Their other song (I, the more familiar bee-buzz) is used later in the day, under higher light levels, and at a median rate of 6 songs/min. In the laboratory I tutored different groups of hand-reared males under normal circumstances (group 1: song II early in the morning, under low light, at a rapid rate; song I later, under high light, at a slow rate), under reversed circumstances (group 2: song I early, low light, rapid rate; etc.), and with no contextual cues (group 3: songs I and II both early and late, under both light levels, and at an intermediate rate). Few males developed entirely normal blue-wing song, but males of group 2 clearly reversed the use of song forms and components. The relationship between signal form and function in the songs of this warbler appears flexible and to some extent learned by the males.  相似文献   

7.
白头鹎繁殖期鸣声行为的研究   总被引:28,自引:1,他引:28  
姜仕仁  丁平 《动物学报》1996,42(3):253-259
利用计算机鸣声分析技术对白头鹎繁殖期的求偶炫耀鸣唱、晨鸣、遇险告警惊叫等鸣声的特征进行分析,以探讨鸣声特征与行为的关系。白头鹎繁殖前期待有的求偶炫耀鸣唱婉转多变,频率一般变化在1.9-3.5kHz之间,另一类出现在繁殖前、中期的炫耀鸣唱急促而响亮,音节变化快且频率高(变化范围约3.4-4.4kHz);最鸣和繁殖期特有的其它鸣声,音节清晰,响度较大,能量主要集中在2-3kHz之间,可能具有宣告领域的  相似文献   

8.
In songbirds, the ability to learn and render the species-specific song is influenced by the development of both the song nuclei in the brain and the syrinx (bird''s vocal apparatus) early in the bird''s life. In black-capped chickadees (Poecille atricapillus), habitat quality is known to affect song structure, with birds in high-quality habitat (mature forest) having a higher song consistency than birds in low-quality habitat (young forest). Although this difference is suspected to stem from differences in development, the developmental status of juvenile birds in either habitat remains unexplored. In this study, we used ptilochronology and feather corticosterone to compare the conditional state of juvenile chickadees in young and mature forest during two distinct periods of song learning - the sensory phase, which occurs prior to settlement, and the sensorimotor phase, which occurs post-settlement. A sample of juvenile males was captured and euthanized several weeks prior to their first breeding season to compare the development of song center nuclei and syrinx in both habitats. The corticosterone levels of natally-grown feathers were greater among birds that settled in mature than young forests - as these feathers were grown pre-settlement, they reflect differences in physiology during the sensory phase. This difference in conditional state is reflected by differences in syrinx and song center nuclei development later during the sensorimotor phase - birds in young forest have smaller syrinx, and moderately-larger RA, than birds in mature forest. Those differences could be responsible for the difference in consistency in song structure observed across habitats. The difference in physiological state across habitats, combined with potential compounding effect of differences in winter resources between habitats, could influence the difference in syrinx and neural development seen in juvenile males during the early spring, and influence the male''s ability to learn and render their species-specific song.  相似文献   

9.
白眉长臂猿鸣叫的时间特征   总被引:4,自引:1,他引:4  
滇西白眉长臂猿(Hylobateshoolock)鸣叫主要发生在上午,最早开始于黎明时分,最晚则在下午16:30以后。平均开始时间为09:05,SD=1095min(N=70,范围07:12~16:30),持续时间为197min,SD=934min(N=55,R=4~50)。多数鸣叫发生在07:00~10:00之间(80%)。不同季节鸣叫发生时间有显著差异,可能与黎明时间(光亮度)不同有关,但持续时间无差异。同一季节异地间鸣叫持续时间差异显著。气候、猿群密度、栖息地状态对鸣叫有一定影响,但未见明显相关性。与黑长臂猿的种间比较表明,白眉长臂猿的鸣叫声在时间分布上有较大的散开度,持续时间也较长,二者有显著差异。  相似文献   

10.
11.
磺化铝酞菁为新一代的光敏剂,为了研究其平均磺化程度对光敏活性的影响,本文比较测定了平均磺化度为三的铝酞菁Alpcs(3)及平均磺化度为二的铝酞菁A1pcs(2)的单态氧(~1O)产率,细胞的吸收及光敏杀伤。结果表明,磺化度的不同并不影响~1O_2的生成及细胞的吸收动力学规律,但人体肝癌细胞对Alpcs(2)的摄入量比Alpcs(3)高出5倍左右,且Alpcs(2)比Alpcs(3)对癌细胞有更强的光敏杀伤效应。提示,可通过调节降低磺化程度,增进细胞吸收,获得更佳的光敏效率。  相似文献   

12.
白头鹎方言的初步研究   总被引:18,自引:0,他引:18  
姜仕仁  施青松 《动物学报》1996,42(4):361-367
对浙江省乘泗岛,普陀岛,温岭,长兴,杭州和龙游等6个地区白头鹎的鸣声进行了研究,发现它们鸣声主句的语调,音节数,持续时间,频谱特征和频率范围等均有差异,说明白头鹎鸣声中普遍存在“方言”,但它们作为同一个种在鸣声主句上有其共同特征:主要是鸣声多在1.5-3kHz的低中频段内变化,单音节的鸣叫声等都极为相似。  相似文献   

13.
Tibor  Farkas 《Ibis》1969,111(3):281-292
A preference for bushy evergreen undergrowth determines both the local and the general distribution of Cossypha natalensis. The lack of such undergrowth makes equatorial rain-forests unsuitable. Tropical and subtropical evergreen forests provide excellent conditions from this point of view, and they are thus the main habitat type from which C. natalensis has secondarily extended into tropical and subtropical deciduous forests with evergreen undergrowth.
Adult birds remain in or close to their territories throughout the year. First-year birds undertake longer movements during the winter. Breeding territories are relatively small, from 2,500 up to (exceptionally) 10,000 square yards (2,100-8,400 sq. m).
Breeding begins in October. Both sexes build the nest, but only the female incubates. In southern Africa there is one brood a year, and the care of the offspring is protracted. The juvenile moult begins in the third and ends in the sixth month; adults have only one (post-breeding) moult a year.
The begging behaviour of the young is somewhat different from that of most other turdine birds, and is considered to be more primitive. The ontogeny of the adult displays is described as observed in young birds kept in captivity.
The development of vocal behaviour is described. Special attention is given to the different functional forms of the song, and to two of its special features, imitation and a tonal quality termed vox humana.  相似文献   

14.
The song of male Kentucky warblers consists of several repetitions of syllables containing 2 to 7 elements within a frequency range from 1.8 to 6.0 kHz. Each male has a single, individually distinctive song type that spans part or all of this frequency range. Song responses to playbacks of songs artificially elevated or lowered in frequency by 200 Hz show that a bird is able to alter its song in two ways. It may lower or raise the frequency range of the song or put more energy into the lower or higher frequencies within the song's elements to more closely match the energy distribution of the playback song. The syllable or element structure of the song is kept constant with only the song's energy distribution among frequencies changing. Thus, Kentucky warblers are able to alter their single song type to “match countersing” in a way similar to that of species with song repertoires who switch song types to match those of rivals. The Kentucky warbler may have a single songtype because of its ability to change its song's energy distribution. This ability may perform the same functions as song matching does in species with larger repertoires. This finding is discussed in relation to ideas on the evolution of song repertoires in intrasexual competition.  相似文献   

15.
羊草不同叶龄叶片光-光合特性的初步研究   总被引:9,自引:0,他引:9  
在6月至7月,羊草叶片在充分展开后的数天之内,即最上展开叶,其净光合速率及其光强系数,以及光饱和点,近饱和点和半饱和点最高;幼龄叶和老龄叶的上述各项指标均较低,但随着光强的下降,不同叶龄叶片的净光合速率或光合的光强系数的差别均逐渐减小,光补偿点以半展开幼叶较低,而后随叶龄增长逐渐升高,羊草的壮龄叶对光的利用能力较强,在其光合生产中起主要作用,幼龄叶仅对弱光的利用能力高,老龄叶相对地较为耐阴。  相似文献   

16.
The songs of the six different species of Darwin's ground finches (Geospiza) on the Galápagos Islands are difficult to distinguish unambiguously because of high levels of intraspecific variation and interspecific similarity in some cases. We recorded the responses of males on five islands to playback of (a) the two main conspecific song types, A and B, (b) local conspecific and heterospecific song, and (c) local and foreign dialects. Males reacted equally strongly to different conspecific song types (A and B), but responded significantly more strongly to local conspecific song than to either heterospecific song or foreign dialect. These results are inconsistent with earlier suggestions that song types subdivide Geospiza populations and that Geospiza song lacks species-distinctness because of loss-of-contrast or character convergence. The apparent paradox of low song specificity and well-developed acoustic discrimination is discussed in the light of other data showing that close-range species recognition also depends on visual cues.  相似文献   

17.
In seasonally breeding songbirds, brain nuclei of the song control system that act in song perception change in size between seasons. It has been hypothesized that seasonal regression of song nuclei may impair song discrimination. We tested this hypothesis in song sparrows (Melospiza melodia), a species in which males share song types with neighbors and must discriminate between similar songs in territorial interactions. We predicted that song sparrows with regressed song systems would have greater difficulty in discriminating between similar songs. Sparrows were housed either on short days (SD) and had regressed song circuits, or were exposed to long days and implanted with testosterone (LD+T) to induce full growth of the song circuits. We conducted two experiments using a GO/NO-GO operant conditioning paradigm to measure song discrimination ability of each group. Birds learned four (experiment 1) or three (experiment 2) pairs of song types sequentially, with each pair more similar in the number of shared song elements and thus more difficult to discriminate. Circulating T levels differed between the SD and LD+T groups. The telencephalic song nuclei HVc, RA, and area X were larger in the LD+T birds. The two groups of sparrows did not differ, however, in their ability to learn to discriminate between shared song types, regardless of the songs' similarity. These results suggest that seasonal changes in the song control system do not affect birds' ability to make difficult song discriminations.  相似文献   

18.
光照强度调控4种亚热带森林植物叶片的抗氧化能力   总被引:13,自引:1,他引:12       下载免费PDF全文
 以亚热带林木荷树(Schima superba)和黧蒴(Castanopsis fissa)(乔木)、九节(Psycotria robra)和罗伞(Ardisia- quinguegona)(灌木)为材料,盆栽苗给予100%、36%及16%自然光照处理,同时在自然林中选择两种低光强区并采用疏伐措施使部分幼树暴露于全自然光下。在夏、冬两季采样分析叶片的抗氧化剂(AsA、GSH)含量和抗氧化酶(APX、SOD)活性,研究光强对4种亚热带树种抗氧化能力的影响。结果表明:同在自然光下,灌木的抗氧化能力较强,其AsA含量,APX.SOD活性高于乔木;随生长光强的减弱,4种幼树叶片的AsA.GSH含量和APX、SOD活性均下降,而光强改变对灌木的影响大于乔木。夏、冬季的分析结果有一定差异,说明光是自然条件下影响植物叶片抗氧化能力因子,但温度等亦有一定的作用。  相似文献   

19.
We documented temporal patterns of natural selection on beak and body traits in a song sparrow population. We looked for evidence of selection in association with reproduction and overwinter survival in order to identify the conditions under which size in beak and body traits is adaptive. We also attempted to identify the specific traits most closely associated with fitness under these conditions. Selection was observed in association with both survival and reproduction. Patterns of selection differed between the sexes. Selection on males was weak and stabilizing in association with overwinter survival. Selection on females was strong, was both stabilizing and directional, and was associated with both survival and reproduction. In females, traits that enhanced juvenile survival also reduced reproductive success; i.e., there was a trade-off between survival and reproduction. Patterns of selection in the song sparrow parallel those reported for the Galápagos finch, Geospiza fortis. However, in song sparrows, selection occurred mainly on tarsus length and beak length, and not on beak depth or width as in G. fortis. This difference may occur because most North American sparrows partition food resources by habitat, while most Galápagos ground finches partition food by seed size.  相似文献   

20.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号