共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Differences in the social organization and dental morphology of Pan paniscus (bonobos) and Pan troglodytes (chimpanzees) have been related to differences in the spatiotemporal availability of food and its exploitation. The presence of abundant terrestrial herbaceous vegetation (THV) in the bonobo's habitat and the apparent greater reliance on herbs for food has been used to explain differences in party size and, by extension, social organization. Using fecal analysis, we assess quantitatively the amount of herbaceous foods consumed by Pan paniscus in the Lomako Forest, Zaire, compared to similar data for Pan troglodytes in the Kibale Forest, Uganda. We examine this data in the context of spatiotemporal patterns of availability of herbaceous foods and fruit, as well as their nutritional content. The results support the suggestion that bonobos consume more herbaceous food than do the Kibale chimpanzees and that these foods are more prevalent in the bonobo's habitat than in the Kibale Forest. However, temporal changes in fruit availability and herb consumption, along with nutritional analyses, suggest that chimpanzees consume herbs as a fallback source of carbohydrates, whereas bonobos consume herbs as a source of protein regardless of season or fruit abundance. Available data suggest that party size while feeding on terrestrial herbs is restricted at both sites, but a determination of the relative strength of this constraint is not possible at this time. Difficulties in methods used for data collection are discussed and areas where more information is needed are highlighted. © 1994 Wiley-Liss, Inc. 相似文献
2.
Randall L. Susman Noel L. Badrian Alison J. Badrian 《American journal of physical anthropology》1980,53(1):69-80
The locomotor behavior of Pan paniscus was studied over a four-week period in Equateur, Republic of Zaire. Bonobos were found to be both arboreal and terrestrial in their daily activities. In the trees adult bonobos are basically quadrupedal, but they also have significant components of armswinging, diving, leaping, and bipedalism in their locomotor repertoire. 相似文献
3.
Takayoshi Kano 《American journal of physical anthropology》1984,63(1):1-11
The wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) of Wamba, Zaire have an extremely high frequency of external abnormal traits. Out of 96 animals in a sample comprising all age and sex classes from two different natural groups, 46 individuals had at least one limb defect. The frequency of abnormalities was greater in males than in females, and increased directly with age. Almost all infants and juveniles had completely normal limbs, but virtually all males and over half the females which had reached their prime were found to have one or more limb defects. These findings indicate that congenital factors contribute little to the frequency of abnormalities in this sample. Survival rate is the same in individuals with defects as in those without defects. It appears that individual social status is far more important in procuring foods than locomotor ability. Thus, the operation of such abnormalities as a selective factor in intraspecific competition is minimized in the network of their social relations. 相似文献
4.
Gen’ichi Idani 《Primates; journal of primatology》1995,36(3):377-383
Peering behavior (prolonged gazing within 30 cm by an animal toward another) in wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba, Zaire, was studied. A total of 230 peering episodes were observed in various social contexts. Peering behavior
was often directed from younger animals toward older ones. In particular, adult females were most frequently involved in peering,
with individuals of all age-sex classes. On the other hand, male bonobos seldom took part in peering behavior. Four types
of behavior patterns followed the peering behavior: (1) the peerer left; (2) the peeree left; (3) both peerer and peeree stayed
but had no further social interaction; and (4) some other social interaction followed. Type (1) was the most frequent. Peering
usually led to tolerance by older (dominant) animals of a younger (subordinate) animal’s subsequent actions directed towards
the former. Peering was thus concluded to be a unilateral action for initiating affinitive interactions by the peerer. 相似文献
5.
Shigeo Uehara 《Primates; journal of primatology》1990,31(3):311-322
The bonobos of Yalosidi, Ikela zone, near the southeastern limit of the range of this species, make regular visits to a marsh
grassland known locally as Iyoko (or Yoku) within the tropical rain forest. They come to the marsh to feed on the fibrous
vegetative organs of particular species of aquatic or amphibious herbs and grasses, especially those of the families Alismataceae
and Cyperaceae. During fixed point observations at Iyoko between September 1975 and January 1976, seasonal changes were recognized
in the party size, attendance rate, and arrival time of the bonobos, while no conspicuous change was observed in the composition
and phenology of their food plants. The size of the bonobo parties appeared to be an important factor in determining the duration
of stay at the marsh per visit. Throughout the study period with the exception of January, they intensively utilized a particular
portion of Iyoko, in which their preferred food was scattered. Iyoko was also utilized frequently as a stable feeding place
by other large forest herbivores such as elephants, buffalos, bongos, sitatungas, and duikers. In contrast, various species
of cercopithecid monkeys commonly seen in the surrounding forest were never observed to enter Iyoko for foraging. This suggests
a comprehensive use of the habitat by the Yalosidi bonobos compared with the more limited ecological niches of other sympatric
non-human primates. 相似文献
6.
The postnatal ontogenetic patterns and processes that underlie species differences in African ape adult mandibular morphology
are not well understood and there is ongoing debate about whether African ape faces and mandibles develop via divergent or
parallel trajectories of shape change. Using three-dimensional (3D) morphometric data, we first tested when in postnatal development
differences in mandibular shape are initially evident between sister species Pan troglodytes and P. paniscus. Next, we tested whether each species has a distinct and non-parallel trajectory of mandibular development. Mandibles sampled
across a broad developmental range of wildshot bonobos (n = 44) and chimpanzees (n = 59) were radiographed and aged from their dental development. We then collected 3D landmark surface data from all the mandibles.
A geometric morphometric analysis of size-corrected 3D data found that bonobos and chimpanzees had parallel and linear ontogenetic
trajectories of mandibular shape change. In contrast, mandibular shape was statistically different between P. paniscus and P. troglodytes as early as infancy, suggesting that species shape differences are already established near or before birth. A linear and
stable trajectory of shape change suggests that mandibular ontogeny in these apes is unimpacted by non-linear variation in
tooth developmental timing. 相似文献
7.
We collected urine samples from seven male bonobos (Pan paniscus) in the Eyengo community, Lomako Forest, Democratic Republic of Congo, and assayed them for testosterone (T). T levels averaged 525 pmol/mg Cr in adult males, and 309 pmol/mg Cr in subadult males. We collected hormonal and behavioral data during a period of relative social instability following the recent arrival of two immigrant males. In concordance with predictions derived from the challenge hypothesis [Wingfield et al., American Naturalist 136:829-846, 1990], which relates T to levels of reproductive aggression, the alpha male had the highest circulating levels of T. When we removed the two recent immigrant males from the analysis, there was a significant positive correlation between T levels and dominance rank for the long-term resident males (n=5, P=0.001, r2=0.98). These are the first data on T levels in wild bonobos, and the results suggest that further study of the relationship between T levels and social context in this species could inform current models relating hormones and aggression in wild apes. 相似文献
8.
Four types of specific objects: wooden spoons, metal bowls, plastic boxes, and cotton towels were introduced in a similar
setting to two captive groups of different species in the genusPan, the bonobo and the chimpanzee. In total, 582 unique manipulation forms were distinguished by a set of variables: types of
objects, motor patterns, body-parts used, the number of objects manipulated, and types of orienting manipulation. In sum,
chimpanzees and bonobos were not so different in the variety and the complexity of object manipulation forms. However, comparison
of the two species revealed significant differences as follows: (1) chimpanzees preferred to use only one hand during manipulation
of both single- and multiple-objects, whereas in the case of multiple-objects bonobos used both hands significantly more often;
(2) chimpanzees performed more orienting manipulations in single-object manipulations than did bonobos, whereas the reverse
was the case in multiple-object manipulations; and (3) chimpanzees' object manipulations were overall more substrate-oriented
than were bonobos'. The factors producing these differences are discussed in relation to positional behaviors and habitual
tool use in the two species. 相似文献
9.
10.
Chie Hashimoto 《International journal of primatology》1997,18(1):1-21
I studied sexual behavior of immature bonobos (Pan paniscus) in a wild group living at Wamba, Zaire, with special reference to its development. Even immature individuals under 1 year old performed sexual behavior. Sexual behavior occurred in almost all age–sex combinations, except between immature and mature females. Based on analyses of behavioral pattern and context, I classified sexual behavior involving immature individuals into three categories. (1) Genital contact between immature individuals was observed during play, and was performed by males more frequently than by females. This sexual behavior shared many traits with that of other great apes. (2) Copulation-like genital contact was observed between immature males and mature females. Its frequency increased with the immature male's age; it developed into copulation in adulthood. (3) Genital contact used to regulate interindividual relationships. This behavior, which is unique to bonobos, was absent among infants. It developed between late juvenile and early adolescent periods in association with changes in social circumstances. 相似文献
11.
Eastern black-and-white colobus (Colobus guereza), or guerezas, have long been considered to be one of the most folivorous primates. I conducted a study of the feeding ecology of two guereza groups (T and O) over an annual cycle in the Kakamega Forest of western Kenya. I found that the annual diets of both groups comprised mostly of leaves (T: 48%, O: 57%) though fruit (T: 44%, O: 33%) also accounted for a substantial portion of the diet. In the six months when fruit was most abundant, fruit consumption constituted an average of 58% of T-group's monthly diet and 42% of O-group's monthly diet. In contrast to most previous studies of colobines, in which seeds were the primary fruit item consumed, almost all of the fruit eaten by guerezas at Kakamega consisted of whole fruits. At least 72% of the whole fruits consumed by T-and O-groups were whole fruits from trees in the Moraceae family, which dominates the tree family biomass at Kakamega. Unlike at sites where guerezas consumed fruit primarily when young leaves were scarce, at Kakamega guerezas ate fruit in accordance with its availability and irrespective of the availability of young leaves. My findings demonstrate that guerezas are more dietarily flexible than was previously known, which may help to explain why the species can survive in such a wide variety of forested habitats across equatorial Africa. 相似文献
12.
Urinary and fecal hormones were analyzed on average every other day in 17 female bonobos kept at four US zoos (San Diego Zoo
and Wild Animal Park, Milwaukee, Columbus, and Cincinnati). Ovarian cycle activity was monitored throughout the 15-month study
period using estrogen and progesterone profiles and swelling charts. Behavioral data on sexual activity were also collected
on a daily basis. Fecal and urinary samples were analyzed using high pressure liquid chromatography (HPLC), gas chromatography-mass
spectrometry (CG-MS), and nanoelectrospray. Preliminary results indicate that in urine, both conjugated progestin and estrogen
metabolites were abundant, while in fecal samples, free progestin metabolites from the 5a-pregnane series were found. Although
traces of estrogen metabolites were detected in fecal samples, long-term monitoring of ovarian activity in our study yielded
no meaningful estrogen profiles. In contrast, fecal progestin profiles, after adjusting for a one-day delay in excretion,
closely matched the corresponding urinary progestin profiles. Using the identical antibody and tracer for both, fecal and
urinary progestins, fecal samples yielded approximately ten times the relative amount of progestins compared to urinary progestins.
Thus, when converted using a regression formula, fecal progestins may complete the picture obtained from urinary progestins,
particularly in cases where the urine sample record is unavailable or incomplete. Evidence of the usefulness of urinary cortisol
as a measure of stress is presented. 相似文献
13.
Hilde Vervafcke Han de Vries Linda van Elsacker 《Primates; journal of primatology》2000,41(3):249-265
We examined the distribution of support behaviour within a captive group of bonobos. Most support was evoked by inter-sexual
conflicts with the two highest ranking females. Within a dyad, the usual winner was most often supported. Individuals that
challenged the rank order by aggressions and pestering were aggressed more often by their targets in the company of an ally.
The two lowest ranking males served as scapegoats, receiving 80% of the contra-support. In coalitions, inviduals did not aggress
victims they would not dare to attack without supporters. However, the victims of coalitions reacted more strongly with fear
and rarely counteraggressed than when being attacked alone, indicative of the high impact of aggression in support. The alpha
female showed some control behaviour when intervening in conflicts. The data fitted with several functional hypotheses: coalitions
functioned to maintain existing ranks, to acquire ranks, to reduce tension, and to test or strengthen the bond. We suggest
that support behaviour fulfilled a crucial role in the maintenance of the power of the two highest ranking females over the
males. Among the females themselves the dominance relationships were not based on coalitions, but on individual attributes. 相似文献
14.
Scott McGraw 《American journal of primatology》1994,34(4):295-307
This paper reports preliminary data on a number of poorly known cercopithecids inhabiting the Lomako Forest, Zaire. Data include those on density, biomass, group size, the relationship between group type (mono vs. polyspecific) and forest type (mixed primary, secondary, monodominant primary, and swamp), and vertical stratification. Species examined are Colobus angolensis, Cercocebus aterrimus, Cercopithecus wolfi, Cercopithecus ascanius, Cercopithecus neglectus, and Allenopithecus nigroviridis. The density and biomass for the Lomako Forest anthropoids (excluding Cercopithecus neglectus and A. nigroviridis) are 165 ind/km2 and 1,034 kg/km2, respectively. Cercopithecus wolfi and Colobus angolensis are found in group sizes similar to those reported from other sites while Cercopithecus ascanius, Cercocebus aterrimus, and A. nigroviridis display group sizes unique to the Lomako. While Colobus angolensis, Cercocebus aterrimus, Cercopithecus ascanius and Cercopithecus wolfi were found, to varying extents, in all four forest types, Cercopithecus neglectus and A. nigroviridis inhabited only swamp forest. The different species sort out along vertical continua both within and between the four forest types. © 1994 Wiley-Liss, Inc. 相似文献
15.
Takeshi Furuichi 《International journal of primatology》1989,10(3):173-197
The unit-group of Pan paniscustends to form one large mixed party consisting of most of its members. Females usually stay in the party irrespective of their
estrous state. They aggregate in the center of the party; and, older females stay in the most central part. Adult and adolescent
sons of the old adult females stay in the central part more than males without mothers in the unit-group do. Females leave
their natal unit-groups as older juveniles or in early adolescence and. settle in another unit-group after visiting several.
Newly immigrated young females are eager to have social interactions with senior females to improve their social positions.
Females become less eager to interact socially with other females when they have their own offspring. The strong bond between
mother and son continues into his adulthood; and, females in old age become important members of the unit-group, both as the
targets of association for younger females and as the mothers of highranking males. High social status of females seems related
to their cohesive grouping tendency. The consistency of the multimale/multifemale party and the existence of prominent mother-offspring
subunits are unique characteristics of P. paniscusamong the Pongidae. This social structure may provide a feasible model of the basic society from which human society evolved. 相似文献
16.
Martin Surbeck Andrew Fowler Caroline Deimel Gottfried Hohmann 《American journal of primatology》2009,71(2):171-174
We present evidence for the consumption of a diurnal, arboreal, group living primate by bonobos. The digit of an immature black mangabey (Lophocebus aterrimus) was found in the fresh feces of a bonobo (Pan paniscus) at the Lui Kotale study site, Democratic Republic of Congo. In close proximity to the fecal sample containing the remains of the digit, we also found a large part of the pelt of a black mangabey. Evidence suggests that the Lui Kotale bonobos consume more meat than other bonobo populations and have greater variation in the mammalian species exploited than previously thought [Hohmann & Fruth, Folia primatologica 79:103–110]. The current finding supports Stanford's argument [Current Anthropology 39:399–420] that some differences in the diet and behavior between chimpanzees (P. troglodytes) and bonobos are an artefact of the limited number of bonobo study populations. If bonobos did obtain the monkey by active hunting, this would challenge current evolutionary models relating the intra‐specific aggression and violence seen in chimpanzees and humans to hunting and meat consumption [Wrangham, Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 42:1–30]. Am. J. Primatol. 71:171–174, 2009. © 2008 Wiley‐Liss, Inc. 相似文献
17.
18.
Hiroshi Ihobe 《Primates; journal of primatology》1992,33(2):247-250
Meat-eating behavior of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) was witnessed on two occasions at Wamba, Republic of Zaire. Only flying squirrels were observed to be eaten by the bonobos.
Several bonobos gathered around the possessor of the meat and showed interest in the meat on all occasions. Begging behavior
was noted on one of the two occasions, but the possessor of the meat ignored it. No sharing of meat was seen on either occasion.
The exclusive targets of hunting by bonobos are apparently small mammals, such as flying squirrels and infant duikers, since
evidence of meat eating by wild bonobos, which have been studied for more than fifteen years, has been restricted to these
mammals. The bonobos at Wamba may have a specialized “prey image”, as in the case of the chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) of the Tai forest, and certain medium-sized or small mammals may not conform to this image. 相似文献
19.
We investigated the existence of a social dominance hierarchy in the captive group of six adult bonobos at the Planckendael Zoo. We quantified the pattern of dyadic exchange of a number of behaviors to examine to what extent each behavior fits a linear rank order model. Following de Waal (1989), we distinguish three types of dominance: agonistic dominance, competitive ability and formal dominance. Fleeing upon aggression is a good measure of agonistic dominance. The agonistic dominance hierarchy in the study group shows significant and strong linearity. The rank order was: 1. female (22 yr), 2. female (15 yr)., 3. male (23 yr.), 4. female (15 yr.), 5. male (9 yr.), 6. male (10 yr.). As in the wild, the females occupy high ranks. There is prominent but nonexclusive female agonistic dominance. Teeth-baring does not fulfil the criteria of a formal submission signal. Peering is a request for tolerance of proximity. Since its direction within dyads is consistent with that of fleeing interactions, it is a useful additional measure to determine agonistic ranks in bonobos. In competitive situations, the females acquire more food than other group members do. The rank obtained from access to food resources differs from the agonistic rank due to female intrasexual social tolerance, expressed in food sharing. We typify the dominance styles in the group as female intrasexual tolerance and male challenging of rank differences. The agonistic rank order correlates significantly with age and has a strong predictive value for other social behaviors. 相似文献
20.
We examined the spatial and temporal distribution of the foods of ursine colobus (Colobus vellerosus) at Boabeng-Fiema, Ghana as a means to predict the monopolizablity and usurpability of their food resources. Recent evidence
suggests that food may not be limiting for folivorous primates, and that male sexual coercion may be a more important influence
on folivore social organization. To address the question, we collected focal data on the feeding behavior of adult females
and males over 11 mo (September 2000-August 2001) on 2 groups: WW (n = 31–33 individuals) and B (n = 8–16 individuals). We also conducted phenological monitoring and a tree survey of the two-group home ranges to establish
food availability and distribution. We used 2 behavioral or organism-defined indicators of feeding behavior to assess potential
resource contestability: food site residence time and distance moved between food sites. The colobus fed on a high diversity
of species, most of their food trees were not clumped in distribution, within-tree interfood distances were short, and food
trees were large. The only condition associated with the potential for monopolization was low food tree density. However,
low food tree density may be offset by the colobus’ use of large trees. Taken together, the ecological and behavioral indicators
suggest the food resources of Colobus vellerosus had a low potential for monopolization. Our results also indicate mature leaves had the longest food site residence time,
which may suggest they should be the most usurpable plant part, though their presumed low quality and high abundance probably
counteracted the effect. The pattern implied the potential for direct feeding competition among Colobus vellerosus at Boabeng-Fiema was low and agonistic interactions over food are not expected. Instead, a group size effect on feeding efficiency
should be a more predominant influence on feeding efficiency, if food is limiting for the species. 相似文献