首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 500 毫秒
1.
Faecal samples were taken weekly from a chosen pen on each of four commercial pig farms, two of which used olaquindox as a feed additive. Coliform bacteria were isolated from the samples and the incidence and level of resistance to olaquindox and to four therapeutic antibiotics determined. The coliforms isolated were biotyped to follow the emergence of drug-resistant sub-populations. The results showed that the coliform flora was complex and that a turnover of biotypes was associated with changes in the occupancy of the pen and, possibly, diet. This turnover led to large fluctuations in both incidence and level of resistance to olaquindox and the antibiotics. Olaquindox resistance was not specifically linked with resistance to any therapeutic antibiotic, and the possible origin of olaquindox-resistant strains is discussed in relation to the biotypes.  相似文献   

2.
Faecal samples were taken weekly from a chosen pen on each of four commercial pig farms, two of which used olaquindox as a feed additive. Coliform bacteria were isolated from the samples and the incidence and level of resistance to olaquindox and to four therapeutic antibiotics determined. The coliforms isolated were biotyped to follow the emergence of drug-resistant sub-populations. The results showed that the coliform flora was complex and that a turnover of biotypes was associated with changes in the occupancy of the pen and, possibly, diet. This turnover led to large fluctuations in both incidence and level of resistance to olaquindox and the antibiotics. Olaquindox resistance was not specifically linked with resistance to any therapeutic antibiotic, and the possible origin of olaquindox-resistant strains is discussed in relation to the biotypes.  相似文献   

3.
In the wild, cover is used by animals to seek shelter from the elements, protection from predators and aggressive conspecifics and as a way to reduce inter-animal communication. Rearing environments for captive raised fowl often contain open areas that are lacking in complexity and that are greatly underused. As a result, use of space within pen-type enclosures is often not uniform. This paper investigates the effect of increased environmental complexity on the use of space by broilers. The experiment consisted of three cover treatments (mesh panels, frame panels and no cover) and three group sizes (80, 110 and 140). The vertical panels, constructed of PVC piping, were positioned in pen centers, while the pen centers in the no cover treatment were left empty. It was hypothesized that the provision of vertical panels to pen centers would attract birds into the central space and would lead to a more uniform distribution of birds. Using a grid-coordinate system set up in each pen, scan samples were taken on the use of space in each pen four times per day, 3 days per week from 5 to 44 days of age. Use of space in the central region for the two cover treatments was significantly higher than for the no cover treatment (P<0.0001). The effect of cover treatment on the mean percentage of birds using the central space was influenced by age (P<0.0001) and by group size (P<0.05). Differences in the use of space between the frame and mesh treatment were only found during week 1. In the side, end and middle regions surrounding the panels, use of space was influenced by cover treatment and age (P<0.0001). Over 80% of the use of pen center occurred at the ends and sides of the panels. These results support the hypothesis that the presence of vertical panels results in a more uniform distribution of birds within the pen by increasing the number of birds using the center.  相似文献   

4.
Three feeding trials with different dietary treatments were conducted to evaluate predictions of dry matter intake and average daily gain of Chinese local beef cattle by Cornell Net Carbohydrate and Protein System (CNCPS) version 5.0. In Trial 1, 60 growing Jin Nan yellow steers (local yellow cattle breed originated from Shanxi Province) were assigned to two treatments with five pens in each treatment and six animals in each pen. Trial 2 was conducted with 72 Lu Xi yellow growing steers (local yellow cattle breed originated from Shandong Province) in 12 conventional pens. These animals were assigned to four treatments with six animals in each pen. In Trial 3, 60 Lu Xi yellow steers were allocated to four treatments with five animals in each pen. Average daily dry matter intake (DMI) for each steer was measured as a mean of each pen. All the data about animals, environment, management and feeds required by the CNCPS model were collected, and model predictions were generated for animals of each pen. Model predictions were evaluated by mean bias, mean square prediction error (MSPE) and regression between the observed and predicted values. Animals in 9 of 10 treatments showed a very close model-predicted DMI values to the observed. In another treatment, animals had a mean bias of −0.72 kg DM/day (TRT 10 in Trial 3). With overall data included, the regression equation between observed and predicted DMI was: YOBS = 1.10XCNCPS − 0.78 (R2 = 0.82; P<0.0001), with an intercept not different (P=0.32) from zero, but a slope still different (P<0.0001) from 1. Although the model-predicted ADG values were very close to the observed values in all trials, most of the predicted values were always lower than the observed values with absolute mean bias less than 0.2. When all data were pooled, the regression equation between observed and predicted ADG was: YOBS = 1.07XCNCPS + 0.02 (R2 = 0.73; P<0.0001), with an intercept not different from zero (P=0.88), but a slope that differed from unity (P<0.0001). Results suggest that the CNCPS model is an acceptable model to predict dietary DM intake and average daily gain of Chinese local beef cattle breeds. Further studies are warranted to introduce model adjustment for Chinese conditions.  相似文献   

5.
6.
Some authors have found indications of subgroup formation when domestic fowl are forced to live together in large flocks, while others have not. In this study experiments were carried out to test the hypothesis that hens in large flocks have home ranges in parts of the pen and that they form subgroups. We also studied if this is influenced by males. In a tiered aviary system (density averaged 16 hens/m(2) of floor area) eight flocks of 568+/-59 ISA Brown laying hybrids were kept in pens. Half of the pens contained 1 male per on average 24 females (mixed flocks). At peak production (36-53 weeks of age) four females roosting closely together for about 14 days and four females roosting far apart from each other were taken out from each flock and put together in separate groups in small pens. Their agonistic behaviour was studied for 2 days before they were put back. This was repeated with new birds, resulting in 16 small sample groups being studied. At 70 weeks, three groups of 10 females per flock roosting closely together in different parts of the pen were dyed with different colours and their locations were observed for 2 nights and 2 days.The incidence of aggressive pecks during day 1 among birds that had been roosting close to each other tended to be lower (P=0.05) than among birds that had been roosting far apart. This effect was not significant among birds from all-female flocks, but among birds from mixed flocks (P<0.05). However, this indicates a recognition of roosting partners and possibly also a rebound effect of the males' reduction of female aggressiveness towards strangers. Irrespective of sex composition in the flocks, birds marked while roosting at the ends of the pens were significantly more often observed within these areas than in other areas of the pen during daytime and came back to the same roosting sites at night (P<0.05-P<0.001). This was not the case for birds from the middle of the pens, where the distribution in the pen in most cases did not differ from random. These results show that laying hens in large groups are rather constant in their use of space, which indicate the presence of home ranges. However, environmental features that facilitate localisation may be important. In summary, we think that these findings indicate the existence of subgroup formation.  相似文献   

7.
This study aimed at assessing the effect of the observation method (direct or from video) and the effect of the presence of an observer on the behavioural results in veal calves kept on a commercial farm. To evaluate the effect of the observation method, 20 pens (four to five calves per pen) were observed by an observer for 60 min (two observation sessions of 30 min) and video-recorded at the same time. To evaluate the effect of the presence of the observer in front of the pen, 24 pens were video-recorded on 4 consecutive days and an observer was present in front of each pen for 60 min (two observation sessions of 30 min) on the third day. Behaviour was recorded using instantaneous scan sampling. For the study of the observer's effect, the analysis was limited to the posture, abnormal oral behaviour and manipulation of substrates. The two observation methods gave similar results for the time spent standing, but different results for all other behaviours. The presence of an observer did not affect the behaviour of calves at day level; however, their behaviour was affected when the observer was actually present in front of the pens. A higher percentage of calves were standing and were manipulating substrate in the presence of the observer, but there was no effect on abnormal oral behaviour. In conclusion, direct observations are a more suitable observation method than observations from video recordings for detailed behaviours in veal calves. The presence of an observer has a short-term effect on certain behaviours of calves that will have to be taken into consideration when monitoring these behaviours.  相似文献   

8.
The ovulatory and oestrus responses of seasonally anovulatory ewes to the presence of ewes with synchronised oestrus was evaluated. The experiment was carried between 4 June and 1 July when the ewes were in seasonal anoestrus. Two hundred adult Suffolk and Dorset ewes were used. The animals were randomly divided into five groups balanced according to breed: Group I (treated) consisted of 25 ewes induced to cycle by the treatment for 10 days with vaginal sponges containing 40 mg of fluorogestone acetate and an injection of 200 IU of pregnant mares' serum gonadotropin (PMSG) at the time of sponge removal. Group II (mixed) consisted of 25 untreated ewes housed in the same pen as the treated ewes throughout the experiment. Groups III, IV and V each consisted of 50 untreated ewes located in adjacent pens progressively more distant from the pen which contained the treated animals. The ewes in Group III had contact with the treated animals through the fence, while those in Groups IV and V were separated from the treated ewes by one and two pens respectively. Day 0 of the experiment was defined as the day in which the sponges were removed from the treated ewes. Blood samples for progesterone determination were obtained from 25 animals from each group on days 6, 10 and 13. Oestrus was detected twice a day using vasectomised rams introduced to each pen for 15 min in the morning and 15 min in the evening. As expected, the proportion of ewes with luteal activity was higher (P < 0.01) in the treated group than in the other four groups on days 6, 10 and 13. By day 13 progesterone levels were elevated in 87.5%, 52%, 37.5%, 32% and 13% of the ewes sampled in Groups I, II, III, IV and V respectively. There was a direct relationship between the proportion of non-treated ewes with ovarian activity and the intensity of contact with the treated ewes, being maximal in the ewes that remained mixed with the synchronised animals, and lowest in the ewes located in the most distant pen. The proportion of ewes that showed oestrus during the first 14 days after sponge removal was significantly higher in the treated (92%) and mixed (40%) groups than in Groups III (10%), IV (8%) and V (4%). It is concluded that the presence of a large number of ewes in oestrus can stimulate ovarian activity in seasonally anoestrous ewes. This female to female stimulation could be mediated by olfactory, visual and/or auditory stimuli.  相似文献   

9.
Seven different records were used to monitor a 2,000-bird pigeon colony. A nest card was placed on each nest box to record current production. This information was collected monthly and transferred to a breeding pair record which was used to evaluate yearly reproductive performance as well as to maintain a record of the number of offspring produced. An individual record was maintained on each bird to record it life history and pedigree. Weekly, when new offspring were banded, a squab data sheet was taken into the pen to record the offspring's permanent leg band number, hatch date, strain, pen number, and parents' band numbers. An individual pen record was used to record the leg band numbers of each bird occupying each pen. A monthly death record and a health record were maintained to monitor the colony's health status. From examination of the death record, one could determine if the mortality was excessive and if any specific pens were involved. On the health record were recorded diet, monthly morbidity and mortality, results of monitoring programs, and any treatments rendered. The system provided ready accessibility do detailed records of production, pedigrees, pen locations of birds, colony health status, and the number, strain, and age of birds available for research.  相似文献   

10.
Previous research has raised the possibility that levels of aggression between pigs may be influenced by their state of arousal and excitement at the time of mixing. This may, in turn, be affected by factors such as the way in which pigs are handled prior to mixing. We attempted the first systematic test of this idea. Sixty four growing pigs were housed in groups of four for 4 weeks to allow them to become familiar with their group mates. Each pig was then tested in a 10min encounter with another pig in an unfamiliar test pen. Pigs were either handled gently (G) or firmly (F) on their way to the encounter. In addition, pigs either met a familiar (F) or an unfamiliar (U) pig in the test pen. Thus, there were four types of encounter (FF, FG, UF, UG) with N=8 tests of each. Two-way analysis of variance showed no effects of handling quality and no interactions between handling quality and familiarity on behaviour during encounters. Unfamiliar pigs showed higher levels of aggressive behaviour. Any fights were stopped immediately. On a second test day, pigs were handled as previously, but all now encountered a familiar individual. Again, there were no effects of handling quality or interactions between handling quality and familiarity. The results, thus, failed to support the idea that the quality of handling prior to mixing influences the levels of aggression subsequently observed.  相似文献   

11.
The objective of this experiment was to investigate the effect of rearing density on pecking behaviour and plumage during rearing and throughout the laying period in aviaries. Chicks were reared on sand at high (H; 13 m−2) or low (L; 6.5 m−2) density, in four rearing pens of 390 chicks and eight pens of 195 chicks, respectively, each pen measuring 30 m2. Proportions of chicks per pen performing various types of pecking behaviour were recorded by scan sampling during 16 observation bouts in each rearing pen at 6 weeks of age and during 24 observation bouts at 12 weeks. Individual body weights and plumage condition were recorded. Later, these pullets were housed at 17 hens m−2 in Tiered Wire Floor (TWF; 3 H and 3 L pens of 275 hens) and Laco-Volétage (2 H and 2 L pens of 275 hens) aviaries. At 35 weeks, two samples of eight hens from each aviary pen were observed for pecking behaviour in a test pen. Throughout the laying period, additional records were collected on pecking behaviour, body weight, plumage condition, egg production, and mortality. The L birds had better plumage condition at 6 weeks of age and throughout the laying period. These birds also ground pecked more frequently than H birds during rearing and the laying period. At 12 weeks, L birds feather pecked less than H birds, but no relationship was found between rearing density and feather-pecking behaviour during the laying period. Although TWF hens feather pecked more frequently than Volétage hens, there was no interaction between rearing density and type of aviary for the various pecking behaviours.  相似文献   

12.
The present study was conducted to determine if the social status of Australian Cashmere goats affects their response to the male effect in terms of LH secretion, ovulation and expression of estrus. Australian Cashmere goats were kept isolated from the males during 5 months. The index of success (SI) of each goat was calculated to establish their social rank. In the first experiment, the ten most dominant and the 10 most subordinate goats were separated from the original herd and housed in two pens (5 dominant and 5 subordinate animals in each pen). An androgenized wether was then introduced into each pen. Luteinizing hormone (LH) was measured every 20 min from 2h before to 4h after introduction of the male in the goats of first pen and from 4 to 8h after male introduction in the second pen. In the second experiment, the remaining 50 goats were exposed in their original pen to two androgenized wethers. Their association index with the males (AI) was calculated for each of these 50 goats, and the intervals from exposure to the males to the onset of estrus and to ovulation were determined. During the first 4h after male introduction, the dominant goats had more LH pulses (0.65+/-0.06 compared with 0.3+/-0.09; P<0.05) and greater LH mean concentrations (1.79+/-0.14 ng/ml compared with 1.30+/-0.15 ng/ml, P=0.05) than the subordinate animals. Although not significantly different, the AI was 35% greater for high and medium ranking goats than for low ranking animals (0.031+/-0.004, 0.032+/-0.005 and 0.023+/-0.005, respectively, P>0.05). Although the number of goats ovulating in response to male exposure was similar between dominance groups (high: 100%, medium: 94% and low ranking: 92%), the high and medium dominance goats showed a greater incidence of expression of estrus than low-dominance goats (94.4%, 89.5% and 53.8%, respectively, P<0.05). It is concluded that the social rank of the Australian Cashmere goat influences their response to the male effect in terms of early LH secretion and expression of estrus.  相似文献   

13.
Muscidifurax zaraptor Kogan and Legner (Hymenoptera: Pteromalidae) was released at three rates in nine beef cattle feedlots in eastern Nebraska to measure dosage response. Dosage response was measured by the percentage parasitism of sentinel house fly pupae. Releases were made from a single location near the center of the pen at three feedlots, from two locations within the pen at three feedlots, and from four corners of the pen at three feedlots. One-time releases initiated held propagation of M. zaraptor using freeze-killed house fly pupae as hosts. Three treatment rates, averaging 4480, 20,300, and 37,100 parasitoids, were released weekly over a 15-week period with each of the three release methods receiving a low, medium, or high treatment rate. All nine release sites produced significantly higher levels of parasitoid emergence and sentinel host mortality than sentinel hosts at two control facilities. The three sites receiving the high treatment rate averaged 38% host mortality, compared with 26% for the medium treatment rate and 17% for the low treatment rate. The two control sites averaged 2% host mortality. No significant differences could be detected in the number of release stations except at the four-station method using the low treatment rate. High temperatures during at least two of the weekly periods were detrimental to the released parasitoids.  相似文献   

14.
With the aim of testing the hypothesis that regrouping decreases milk production in French Alpine goats that were lactating, a study was done using two groups (n = 8, 7). During their third month after parturition, four goats from each group were exchanged (first regrouping) between pens and left for 2 weeks, then the same two subgroups of four goats were taken back to their original pen for another 2 weeks (second regrouping). In the third regrouping, the two groups were all placed in the one pen. Milk production and social behaviour were measured daily before and after each regrouping. All regroupings led to an increase in aggressive behaviours that last by 1–2 days. Mean daily milk production decreased after first (2.82 ± 0.2 kg versus 2.53 ± 0.2 kg; P < 0.05) but not after second and third regrouping. It is concluded that aggressive behaviour increases after all regroupings, whereas milk production decreases only after the first regrouping, suggesting an important capacity of adaptation to a novel and stressful managements in the French Alpine goat. The study highlights the importance of considering effects of common practices in herd managements on social behaviour and production.  相似文献   

15.
It is known that pigs can acquire flavour preferences by brief social interactions with conspecifics that previously consumed a flavoured solid feed. However, there is no information about whether a flavoured solution could support flavour preferences through social transmission. Ninety-six pigs (49 days old) were housed in 12 pens (8 pigs/pen). Four animals per pen were randomly selected to act as observers and four as demonstrators. Demonstrator animals were temporarily moved to an empty pen where a protein solution was offered (porcine digestive peptides (PDPs), 4% weight/volume) with the addition of 0.075% aniseed (six pens) or garlic (six pens) powdered artificial flavours for 30 min. Afterwards, demonstrators were returned to interact with observer animals for 30 min. A choice test (30 min) between aniseed and garlic PDP was performed for each observer group after the interaction. Observers showed a higher intake of solutions previously consumed by their demonstrator conspecifics (648 v. 468 ml; SEM 61.36, P < 0.05). As with flavoured solid feeds, protein solutions containing artificial flavours can create preferences in pigs for those flavours through social transmission from conspecifics.  相似文献   

16.
Statolith shape and microstructure were studied in 151 specimens of the common arctic squid Gonatus fabricii (7.3–322 mm pen length) collected in the southern Norwegian Sea. Statolith development and growth both comprised two main periods, which corresponded with the epipelagic and meso-bathypelagic ontogenetic periods of G. fabricii. During the epipelagic period (pen length range from 3 to 50–60 mm), statoliths quickly developed from the droplet-like form in early paralarvae to the pre-definite stage in juveniles (30–50 mm pen length). Paralarval and juvenile statoliths grew with high growth rates, and their microstructure contained narrow first-order growth increments. Three main growth zones (Z1, Z2, Z3) developed during this period, being well distinguished from each other by specific patterns of microstructure and separated from each other by distinct checks. During the meso-bathypelagic period (from 50–60 to 322 mm pen length), statoliths hardly changed their shape and grew very slowly. Only one growth zone (Z4) was formed within the statolith microstructure, characterized by disappearance of the first growth increments and formation of specific second-order bands. Each second-order band consisted of approximately seven first-order increments. If the assumption “one increment-one day” is true for G. fabricii, the squid would then be a slow-growing animal with a life span for both sexes not exceeding 2 years. Accepted: 25 May 1999  相似文献   

17.
Restriction of behavioral opportunities and uneven use of space are considerable welfare concerns in modern broiler production, particularly when birds are kept at high densities. We hypothesized that increased environmental complexity by provision of barrier perches would help address these issues by encouraging perching and enhancing use of the pen space across a range of stocking densities. 2,088 day-old broiler chicks were randomly assigned to one of the following barrier and density treatment combinations over four replications: simple barrier, complex barrier, or control (no barrier) and low (8 birds/m(2)), moderate (13 birds/m(2)), or high (18 birds/m(2)) density. Data were collected on focal birds via instantaneous scan sampling from 2 to 6 weeks of age. Mean estimates per pen for percent of observations seen performing each behavior, as well as percent of observations in the pen periphery vs. center, were quantified and submitted to an analysis of variance with week as the repeated measure. Barrier perches, density and age affected the behavioral time budget of broilers. Both simple and complex barrier perches effectively stimulated high perching rates. Aggression and disturbances were lower in both barrier treatments compared to controls (P<0.05). Increasing density to 18 birds/m(2) compared to the lower densities suppressed activity levels, with lower foraging (P<0.005), decreased perching (P<0.0001) and increased sitting (P = 0.001) earlier in the rearing period. Disturbances also increased at higher densities (P<0.05). Use of the central pen area was higher in simple barrier pens compared to controls (P<0.001), while increasing density above 8 birds/m(2) suppressed use of the central space (P<0.05). This work confirms some negative effects of increasing density and suggests that barrier perches have the potential to improve broiler welfare by encouraging activity (notably by providing accessible opportunities to perch), decreasing aggression and disturbances, and promoting more even distribution of birds throughout the pen space.  相似文献   

18.
The aim of this study was to evaluate the effects of age of separation on the behavioural responses of the dairy calf and cow. Calves were separated from their dams 6 h, 1 day, or 4 days after birth (n=9 cow-calf pairs in each of the 3 treatment groups) and behaviour was video and audio taped from 1 h before separation to 21 h after separation. In the hour immediately before separation, we found that the younger calves tended to call and move more in the pen, and spent more time standing than the older calves, but after separation these trends reversed. Calves separated at older ages made significantly more movements in the pen (P<0.05), spent more time standing (P<0.05) and spent more time with the head out of the pen (P<0.01) than calves separated soon after birth. We observed a similar pattern for the cows. Before separation, cows with younger calves moved more frequently about the pen (P<0.05), and called at much higher rates (a mean of 40.7 calls during 40 min for cows on the 6-h treatment, vs. 0.2 calls for cows in the 4-day group; P<0.001). After separation, cows in the 4-day group called at approximately four times the rate of those separated at 6 h or 1 day (P<0.01). Moreover, the calls produced by cows separated later had a significantly higher fundamental frequency (P<0.001) and a lower emphasized harmonic (P<0.02) than the calls of cows separated from calves soon after birth. There was no difference between treatment groups in the other behavioural measures, either before or after separation. Calves separated at older ages tended to require fewer days of treatment for scouring, but calf weight gain and cow milk production did not differ among treatment groups. In conclusion, behavioural responses of both the cow and calf increase in relation to calf age at separation. However, there may be health advantages associated with delayed separation that compensate for the increased behavioural response.  相似文献   

19.
The studies reported here demonstrate that increased resistance of Neisseria gonorrhoeae to penicillin, tetracycline, and chloramphenicol results from the combined effect of two resistance loci. As shown by experiments with deoxyribonucleic acid from transformants carrying only a single resistance locus, transformants with an incresed level of resistance to penicillin result from the combination of a penicillin-specific locus, pen, and a multiple resistance locus, mtr. Similarly, transformants with an increased level of resistance to tetracycline result from the combination of mtr and a tetracycline-specific locus, tet. Transformants with an increased level of resistance to chloramphenicol result from the combination of mtr and a chloramphenicol-specific locus, cml. Deoxyribonucleic acid dilution experiments established that only a single dose of each of the two required resistance loci is necessary to give higher-level resistance. Higher-level-resistant transformants were not obtained when a double dose of one resistance locus or a combination of loci pairs other than mtr and pen, mtr and tet, or mtr and cml was introduced into a recipient. Combinations of the mtr and tet genes resulted in increased resistance to semisynthetic tetracyclines. The presence of the mtr and pen genes resulted in increased resistance to penicillinase-stable penicillins.  相似文献   

20.
To investigate the effect of repeated regrouping and relocation (R&R) on behaviour of steers, 72 Holstein–Friesian (14-month-old; 441 ± 3.2 kg) steers were assigned to either control (n = 30; C) or regrouped (n = 42; R) treatments and housed six per pen in 12 pens. The R steers were exposed to six R&R over 84 days. New pen cohorts were allowed to stabilise for 14 days and none of the R steers were allowed to share the same pen or pen mates, where or with whom, they were previously housed. Control steers were housed in the same pen with the same pen mates. Each steer was marked on its back with an individual identification code. Twelve cameras were used to observe and record behaviour for each pen allowing observation of all individual steers continuously for a week following each R&R. The following behaviours were recorded for each steer: lying, standing, eating, drinking, head-to-head contact with another animal while not eating, head contact with the body of another animal and bodily contact with none, one, two or three steers. Behaviour was observed by instantaneous scan sampling after each R&R, at 2 min intervals for 2 h on day 1; at further 20 min intervals on days 1 and 2; and at 120 min intervals from day 3 to 7. Where appropriate, the % of time spent in each behaviour was calculated from the data on total counts in each behavioural category. The total count data were analysed by χ2-statistics for all behavioural categories. Steers were weighed before each R&R. Average daily gain from day 0 to 84 was analysed by ANOVA. During the first 2 h observation period following mixing, R steers displayed a greater (P < 0.05) % of time standing (following the first to sixth R&R), eating (first to fourth and sixth R&R) and drinking (first, third and fourth R&R) than control steers. In the 20 min observational period, a greater % (P < 0.05) of time was spent standing, eating and drinking in R than in C steers following each R&R. In the 120 min observation period, R steers spent a greater (P < 0.05) % of time lying with less body contact behaviours than C steers, and these findings increased in the fourth, fifth and sixth R&R. These data suggest that there was partial adaptation to repeated R&R at the first two R&R followed by complete adaptation at the third and subsequent R&R, with no detrimental effect on animal performance.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号