首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
It was found in healthy adult subjects (n = 90) that under conditions of complication of cognitive tasks (increasing number of relevant stimuli), the stability (rigidity) of the unconscious cognitive sets markedly rises. These changes in both verbal and nonverbal sets depend on the working memory loading. The revealed dependence of the motor reaction time to the probe stimulus on the sequence of the set stimuli in a context and on the set stage suggests that unconscious cognitive sets can regulate selective attention.  相似文献   

2.
Monkeys were taught to differentiate stimuli with different types of information and to perform a spatial choice. Development of Alzheimer's disease in experimental monkeys entailed a deficit of operative memory, a considerable enhancement of entropy related to correct decisions. In control monkeys, no significant changes of these characteristics occurred. In monkeys, following a bilateral removal of the sulcus principalis, the 7th field, or both, the operative memory deficit was also determined by two components. The specifics of disorders in the operative memory due to Alzheimer's disease suggest that cholinergic mechanisms determining the sensory processing differ from those involved in decision-making. The structural-functional organization of interaction between sensory and cognitive processes controlled by the motivation and attention system, is discussed.  相似文献   

3.
4.
In order to reveal the character of motivation influence on the stability of the unconscious visual set, young adult healthy subjects were promised to be rewarded for correct solution of the cognitive task. A substantial increase in set stability was observed under conditions of augmented motivation of a subject to the result of his performance. Positive and negative aspects of the impact of the increased motivation of a subject on his cognitive activity at the stages of set formation and actualization are discussed.  相似文献   

5.
6.
It has been just under a decade since contemporary neuroimaging tools, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging, were first applied to developmental questions. These tools provide invaluable information on how brain anatomy, function and connectivity change during development. Studies using these methods with children and adolescents show that brain regions that support motor and sensory function mature earliest, whereas higher-order association areas, such as the prefrontal cortex, which integrate these functions, mature later.  相似文献   

7.
Functional characteristics of responses of tectal neurons in the frog and the primary visual cortex of the cat obtained under indentical experimental conditions during changes in the brightness and duration of flashes within the range of 6 log units were compared. In frogs most units have short summation times and relatively lower thresholds; the response latency is 5–7 times longer than in cats. The overwhelming majority of tectal units can respond only to a narrow range of photic energy that differs for different cells. Most cells in cats respond to changes in brightness of between 4 and 5 log units; they have long summation times, short latent periods, and relatively higher thresholds. The differences found on comparison of the various functional characteristics of the cells in the visual center show that frogs have fixed mechanisms of temporal and spatial interaction responsibile for detection of stimulus brightness. In cats this interaction between individual cell populations and this mutual inhibition between adjacent cells are not prominent. The increased complexity and overlapping of interneuronal connections leads to convergence of day and twilight vision stimuli on the same neuron and to the ability of single units to respond to the whole "working" range of light brightness for the cat visual system.  相似文献   

8.
Summary Intracellular recording and Lucifer yellow dye filling of male fleshflies, Sarcophaga bullata, have revealed male-specific neurons in the lobula, the axons of which project to the origin of premotor channels supplying flight motor neurons. Dendrites of male-specific neurons visit areas of the retinotopic mosaic supplied by the retina's acute zone, which is used by males to keep the image of a conspecific female centered during aerial pursuit. Only males engage in high-speed acrobatic chases, and male-specific neurons are suspected to under-lie this behavior. Physiological determination of receptive fields of male-specific neurons substantiates the fields predicted from anatomical studies and demonstrates that they subtend the acute zone. Male-specific neurons respond in a manner predicted on theoretical grounds from observations of tracking behavior. Such properties include directional selectivity to visual motion and higher sensitivity to motion of small images than to wide-field motion. The present account substantiates and extends neuroanatomical evidence that predicts that male-specific lobula neurons comprise a distinct circuit mediating conspecific tracking.  相似文献   

9.
视皮层分区及其fMRI 研究进展   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3       下载免费PDF全文
血氧水平依赖功能磁共振成像(BOLD—fMRI)作为一种无创、可精确定位的脑功能研究技术,已广泛应用于视觉系统的研究中,并取得了许多重要成果,本文就fMRI研究进展及其在大脑视觉皮层功能分区中的应用做一综述。  相似文献   

10.
11.
Receptive fields of neurons in Area 17 of the visual cortex were investigated in cats. Concentrically shaped fields, fields responding selectively to orientation of a strip or edge, and fields which can be regarded as intermediate between the first two types are described. The boundary between zones of summation and of lateral inhibition coincides in some receptive fields with the boundary between central and peripheral zones with opposite forms of response, while in other fields they do not coincide. For some cells there is no peripheral zone or it may disappear with worsening of the state of function. Cells were observed for which an increase in area of the stimulus in the central zone inhibits the response reaction. Analysis of these data suggests that several cells of the geniculate ganglion converge on some cortical neurons, and several cortical cells on others. An effect of adaptive inhibition was found in which constant illumination of an area in the center of the receptive field inhibits the response in another part. It is shown that this effect is unconnected with the action of scattered light. Constant illumination of the peripheral part of the receptive field deinhibits adaptive inhibition. The boundary between the zones of summation and of lateral inhibition coincides with the boundary between the zones of adaptive inhibition and deinhibition.I. V. Pavlov Institute of Physiology, Academy of Sciences of the USSR, Leningrad. Translated from Neirofiziologiya, Vol. 1, No. 1, pp. 90–100, July–August, 1969.  相似文献   

12.
13.
Marvin M. Chun and Yuhong Jiang (1998) investigated the role of spatial context on visual search. They used two display conditions. In the Old Display condition, the spatial arrangement of items in the search display was kept constant throughout the experiment. In the New Display condition, the spatial arrangement of items was always novel from trial to trial. The results showed better performance with Old Displays than with New Displays. The authors proposed that repeated spatial context help guiding attention to the target location, thus they termed this effect Contextual Cueing. We present three attempts to reproduce this effect. Experiments 1 and 2 were near exact replications of experiments in Chun and Jiang's report, where we failed to obtain Contextual Cueing. Post-experimental interviews revealed that participants used different search strategies when performing the task: an 'active' strategy (an active effort to find the target), or a 'passive' strategy (intuitive search). In Experiment 3, we manipulated task instructions to bias participants into using active or passive strategies. A robust Contextual Cueing Effect was obtained only in the passive instruction condition.  相似文献   

14.
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) is used widely to determine the spatial layout of brain activation associated with specific cognitive tasks at a spatial scale of millimeters. Recent methodological improvements have made it possible to determine the latency and temporal structure of the activation at a temporal scale of few hundreds of milliseconds. Despite the sluggishness of the hemodynamic response, fMRI can detect a cascade of neural activations - the signature of a sequence of cognitive processes. Decomposing the processing into stages is greatly aided by measuring intermediate responses. By combining event-related fMRI and behavioral measurement in experiment and analysis, trial-by-trial temporal links can be established between cognition and its neural substrate.  相似文献   

15.
We have recorded from single neurons in the medial bank of the middle suprasylvian sulcus (PMLS) of anaesthetized and paralysed cats aged between nine days and eight weeks. Visual responses were assessed qualitatively, by using conventional projected stimuli, and quantitatively for drifting, high-contrast gratings of optimum spatial and temporal frequencies, but varying in orientation and direction of drift. At 9 days of age, some cells in the PMLS were spontaneously active but in three long penetrations only one visually responsive neuron was isolated. Between 9 and 15 days there was a rapid increase in the proportion of responsive units, which first appeared in small clusters in the lower layers (IV, V, VI). During the second and third postnatal weeks, spontaneous activity and the strength of visual responses increased to adult levels, and the proportion of cells showing rapid habituation to visual stimulation decreased. Even before two weeks of age, at least 85% of responsive cells in the PMLS were selective, by quantitative criteria, for image motion along one particular axis, and a majority of these were clearly direction-selective (responding to movement in one direction significantly more strongly than to that in the opposite). By the end of the third postnatal week the proportion of units with strong direction preference reached adult levels. The selective cells were initially more broadly 'tuned', on average, for the direction of motion of a grating (mean half-width in animals of 10-12 days was 32.6 degrees), but the sharpness of tuning improved to reach the adult level (ca. 23 degrees) during the third postnatal week. In animals younger than three weeks a slightly smaller proportion of cells than in adults (but always more than one third of all visually responsive cells) responded to stationary, contrast-modulated gratings. The majority of these cells showed clear selectivity for the orientation of a flashed grating. A few 'non-selective' cells were found in the youngest animals but by the end of the third postnatal week virtually all cells responsive to stationary gratings displayed orientation selectivity. There was always good agreement between the preferred orientations for stationary and drifting gratings. Even before two weeks of age, when responsive cells occurred only in small clusters, there was a clear tendency for neighbouring neurons to have similar or opposite preferred directions, just as in adult cats. By 2-3 weeks of age there were clear progressive shifts in stimulus preference along oblique or tangential tracks.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)  相似文献   

16.
Power spectra of cortical potentials of baseline activity during interstimuli intervals (4 s; Fourier transform in the frequency band of 1-60 Hz) and short-term (0.8 s) induced responses to facial stimuli (wavelet transform in the 15-60 Hz band) were assessed in the study of the visual cognitive set to facial expression. Significant differences between groups of subjects with different set rigidity were observed only at the set-testing stage. Estimation of the short-term (0.8 s) induced responses of the wavelet spectra in the group with plastic set revealed an increase in the power (in comparison with the power of background activity) of the gamma2 band (41-60 Hz) in the left hemisphere of the temporal, central and occipital areas, whereas in the group with rigid set these power spectra decreased. At the same time the power in the gamma1 band (21-40 Hz) was significantly lower (at the same level with the rigid form), indicating a discrete nature and functional selectivity in the gamma frequency band.  相似文献   

17.
Dalley JW  Everitt BJ  Robbins TW 《Neuron》2011,69(4):680-694
Impulsivity is the tendency to act prematurely without foresight. Behavioral and neurobiological analysis of this construct, with evidence from both animal and human studies, defines several dissociable forms depending on distinct cortico-striatal substrates. One form of impulsivity depends on the temporal discounting of reward, another on motor or response disinhibition. Impulsivity is commonly associated with addiction to drugs from different pharmacological classes, but its causal role in human addiction is unclear. We characterize in neurobehavioral and neurochemical terms a rodent model of impulsivity based on premature responding in an attentional task. Evidence is surveyed that high impulsivity on this task precedes the escalation subsequently of cocaine self-administration behavior, and also a tendency toward compulsive cocaine-seeking and to relapse. These results indicate that the vulnerability to stimulant addiction may depend on an impulsivity endophenotype. Implications of these findings for the etiology, development, and treatment of drug addiction are considered.  相似文献   

18.
Attentional selection plays a critical role in conscious perception. When attention is diverted, even salient stimuli fail to reach visual awareness. Attention can be voluntarily directed to a spatial location or a visual feature for facilitating the processing of information relevant to current goals. In everyday situations, attention and awareness are tightly coupled. This has led some to suggest that attention and awareness might be based on a common neural foundation, whereas others argue that they are mediated by distinct mechanisms. A body of evidence shows that visual stimuli can be processed at multiple stages of the visual-processing streams without evoking visual awareness. To illuminate the relationship between visual attention and conscious perception, we investigated whether top-down attention can target and modulate the neural representations of unconsciously processed visual stimuli. Our experiments show that spatial attention can target only consciously perceived stimuli, whereas feature-based attention can modulate the processing of invisible stimuli. The attentional modulation of unconscious signals implies that attention and awareness can be dissociated, challenging a simplistic view of the boundary between conscious and unconscious visual processing.  相似文献   

19.
Results of multidisciplinary studies, including neuromorphological, neurophysiological, neuropsychological, and psychphysiological studies, are reviewed. They allow the brain mechanisms of cognition formation and development during maturation to be identified. The role of regulatory (modulatory) brain systems in forming the cognitive function in the child is demonstrated. Data on considerable changes in the brain systems responsible for the development of cognitive functions in children between the ages of five to six and seven to eight years are presented. At this age, the morphological and functional maturations of the frontal cortical areas and their descending connections with other cerebral structures increase the efficiencies of arbitrary selective attention, learning the activity program, inhibition of spontaneous responses, and regulation and organization of activity, i.e., the functions that are important for successful schooling.  相似文献   

20.
To recognize a previously seen object, the visual system must overcome the variability in the object''s appearance caused by factors such as illumination and pose. Developments in computer vision suggest that it may be possible to counter the influence of these factors, by learning to interpolate between stored views of the target object, taken under representative combinations of viewing conditions. Daily life situations, however, typically require categorization, rather than recognition, of objects. Due to the open-ended character of both natural and artificial categories, categorization cannot rely on interpolation between stored examples. Nonetheless, knowledge of several representative members, or prototypes, of each of the categories of interest can still provide the necessary computational substrate for the categorization of new instances. The resulting representational scheme based on similarities to prototypes appears to be computationally viable, and is readily mapped onto the mechanisms of biological vision revealed by recent psychophysical and physiological studies.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号