首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Summary Abdominal flexions associated with flight steering were measured in tethered flyingTeleogryllus oceanicus stimulated with a model of conspecific calling song presented at various intensities and from many directions.Flexions increased in size with stimulus intensity until a plateau level was reached. Flexion amplitude was then approximately constant over a range of 20–30 dB, and decreased at still higher intensities (Figs. 2, 3). The shape of this intensity function results from binaural processing; in unilaterally deafened crickets flexion amplitude increased monotonically with stimulus intensity (Fig. 4).Abdominal flexions were graded with respect to sound location; they were larger for laterally placed sound sources and smaller for sound sources near the midline (Figs. 5, 6).A model for the specification of flight steering movements is presented which accounts for our findings (Fig. 7).  相似文献   

2.
Summary Phonotactic responses ofTeleogryllus oceanicus were studied with two methods. Tethered crickets were stimulated with sound while they performed stationary flight, and steering responses were indicated by abdominal movements. Walking crickets tracked a sound source while their translational movements were compensated by a spherical treadmill, and their walking direction and velocity were recorded.During both flight and walking, crickets attempted to locomote towards the sound source when a song model with 5 kHz carrier frequency was broadcast (positive phonotactic response) and away from the source when a song model with 33 kHz carrier frequency was used (negative phonotactic response) (Figs. 2, 4).One-eared crickets attempted, while flying, to steer towards the side of the remaining ear when stimulated with the 5 kHz model, and away from that side in response to the 33 kHz model (Fig. 3). While walking, one-eared crickets circled towards and away from the intact side in response to the 5 kHz and 33 kHz models, respectively (Fig. 6).Positive and negative responses differed in their temporal pattern requirements. Phonotactic responses were not elicited when a non-calling song pattern (2 pulses/s) was played with a carrier frequency appropriate for positive phonotactic responses (5 kHz), but this pattern did elicit negative responses with 33 kHz carrier frequency (Figs. 7–10). When an intermediate carrier frequency, 15 kHz, was used, the response type (positive or negative) depended on the stimulus temporal pattern; the calling song pattern elicited primarily positive responses, while the non-calling song pattern elicited negative responses (Figs. 11, 12, 14, 15). A curious phenomenon was often observed in the flight steering responses; while most responses to 15 kHz song pattern were primarily positive, they often had an initial negative component which was supplanted by the positive component of the response after approximately 2–5 s (Figs. 11, 12).In recent experiments onGryllus campestris, Thorson et al. (1982) described frequency-dependent errors in phonotactic direction (anomalous phonotaxis) and showed how such errors might arise from the frequency-dependent directional properties of the cricket's auditory apparatus. Our findings, particularly the dependence of response type on temporal pattern when 15 kHz carrier frequency was used, argue that frequency-dependent directional properties alone cannot account for positive and negative phonotaxis inT. oceanicus. Rather, these represent qualitatively different attempts to locomote towards and away from the sound source, respectively.We discuss the possibility that central integration of these opposing tendencies might contribute to anomalous phonotaxis.  相似文献   

3.
The steering responses of three species of field crickets, Teleogryllus oceanicus, T. commodus, and Gryllus bimaculatus, were characterized during tethered flight using single tone-pulses (rather than model calling song) presented at carrier frequencies from 3-100 kHz. This range of frequencies encompasses the natural songs of crickets (4-20 kHz, Fig. 1) as well as the echolocation cries of insectivorous bats (12-100 kHz). The single-pulse stimulus paradigm was necessary to assess the aversive nature of high carrier frequencies without introducing complications due to the attractive properties of repeated pulse stimuli such as model calling songs. Unlike the natural calling song, single tone-pulses were not attractive and did not elicit positive phonotactic steering even when presented at the calling song carrier frequency (Figs. 2, 3, and 9). In addition to temporal pattern, phonotactic steering was sensitive to carrier frequency as well as sound intensity. Three discrete flight steering behaviors positive phonotaxis, negative phonotaxis and evasion, were elicited by appropriate combinations of frequency, temporal pattern and sound intensity (Fig. 12). Positive phonotactic steering required a model calling song temporal pattern, was tuned to 5 kHz and was restricted to frequencies below 9 kHz. Negative phonotactic steering, similar to the 'early warning' bat-avoidance behavior of moths, was produced by low intensity (55 dB SPL) tone-pulses at frequencies between 12 and 100 kHz (Figs. 2, 3, and 9). In contrast to model calling song, single tone-pulses of high intensity 5-10 kHz elicited negative phonotactic steering; low intensity ultrasound (20-100 kHz) produced only negative phonotactic steering, regardless of pulse repetition pattern. 'Evasive', side-to-side steering, similar to the 'last-chance' bat-evasion behavior of moths was produced in response to high intensity (greater than 90 dB) ultrasound (20-100 kHz). Since the demonstration of negative phonotactic steering did not require the use of a calling song temporal pattern, avoidance of ultrasound cannot be the result of systematic errors in localizing an inherently attractive stimulus when presented at high carrier frequencies. Unlike attraction to model calling song, the ultrasound-mediated steering responses were of short latency (25-35 ms) and were produced in an open loop manner (Fig. 4), both properties of escape behaviors.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)  相似文献   

4.
Summary Recordings were made from an identified auditory neuron, the omega neuron, in the cricketTeleogryllus oceanicus. Models of the conspecific calling song and of the song of another species were presented either singly or simultaneously, and the degree to which the temporal pattern of the conspecific model was encoded in the neuron's spike train was determined. When a single stimulus was presented alone, its temporal pattern was faithfully reflected by the cells's spiking activity, no matter what the azimuth of the broadcasting loudspeaker (Fig. 3). When two stimuli were presented simultaneously from opposite sides, encoding of the pattern ipsilateral to the recorded neuron was interfered with only slightly by the contralateral pattern, as long as the two loudspeakers were sufficiently separated (Figs. 2, 3, 4). When the loudspeakers were each 15° from the cricket's midline, however, the encoding of the temporal pattern of the ipsilateral song model was severely disrupted (Figs. 3, 4). Bilateral interactions are important in determining the response level of the neuron, but do not appear to contribute to the direction-selective encoding of the stimulus temporal pattern (Figs. 5, 6).Phonotactic steering movements of tethered, flying crickets were recorded under stimulus conditions similar to those used in the neurophysiological tests. Under one-stimulus conditions, crickets attempted to turn towards the conspecific model for all tested speaker locations. The heterospecific model elicited reliable steering behavior when it was broadcast from azimuths of 90° and 60°, but often failed to elicit consistent responses when the speaker was positioned closer to the cricket's midline (Figs. 7, 8A and 8B). Responses to the heterospecific pattern were smaller in amplitude than those to the conspecific song model (Figs. 7, 8B). Under two-stimulus conditions, the conspecific model was consistently preferred over the heterospecific song for all tested speaker locations in half the tested crickets. In the remaining animals, preference for the conspecific pattern was only evident for the larger loudspeaker azimuths (Figs. 7, 8C).These results demonstrate that simultaneouslypresented stimuli can be represented separately in the nervous system as a consequence of auditory directionality. It is postulated that the cricket's ability to choose between these stimuli may result from the interactions between two bilaterallypaired song recognizers, each of which may be driven primarily by sound stimuli from one side.  相似文献   

5.
Male crickets Teleogryllus oceanicus (Le Guillou) produce a complex species‐specific calling song with phrases combining groups of single pulses (chirps) and groups of double‐pulses (trills) to attract females, which fly or walk towards singing males. In open‐loop trackball experiments, phonotactic steering responses to normal calling song phrases consisting of chirps and trills are strongest, suggesting that both components are necessary for maximal attractiveness. Sequences of just chirps or trills are less effective in eliciting phonotactic walking and steering. Split‐song paradigms are used to analyze the steering behaviour underlying orientation in more detail. The females' phonotactic steering reflects the alternating acoustic pattern of the split‐song paradigm. Analysis with high temporal resolution demonstrate, that even when the calling song is presented only from one side, the steering velocity and lateral deviation towards the song is modulated by steering events to single‐sound pulses. Therefore, pattern recognition, which integrates the structure of the song, appears not to be directly involved in the rapid steering response. This organization of phonotactic behaviour with a parallel processing of pattern recognition and steering is similar to other cricket species and may allow T. oceanicus females to steer transiently towards distorted song patterns as they occur in natural habitats.  相似文献   

6.
Phonotaxis in flying crickets   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0  
The effects of two-tone stimuli on the high frequency bat-avoidance steering behavior of flying crickets (Teleogryllus oceanicus) were studied during tethered flight. Similarly, the effects of two-tone stimuli on the ultrasound sensitive auditory interneuron, Int-1, which elicits this behavior, were studied using intracellular staining and recording techniques. When a low frequency tone (3-8 kHz) was presented simultaneously with an aversive high frequency tone (in a two-tone stimulus paradigm), the high frequency avoidance steering behavior was suppressed. Suppression was optimal when the low frequency tone was between 4 and 5 kHz and about 10-15 dB louder than the high frequency tone (Figs. 2, 3). Best suppression occurred when the low frequency tone-pulse just preceded or overlapped the high frequency tone-pulse, indicating that the suppressive effects of 5 kHz could last for up to 70 ms (Fig. 4). The threshold for avoidance of the bat-like stimulus was elevated when model bat biosonar (30 kHz) was presented while the animal was performing positive phonotaxis toward 5 kHz model calling song, but only if the calling song intensity was relatively high (greater than 70-80 dB SPL) (Fig. 1). However, avoidance steering could always be elicited as long as the calling song was not more than 10 dB louder than the ultrasound (Fig. 1). This suppressive effect did not require performance of positive phonotaxis to the calling song (Fig. 2) and was probably due to the persistence of the suppressive effects of the 5 kHz model calling song (Fig. 4). The requirement for relatively high intensities of calling song suggest that the suppression of bat-avoidance by the calling song is not likely to be of great significance in nature. The high frequency harmonics of the male cricket's natural calling song overlap the lower frequency range used by insectivorous bats (10-20 kHz) and are loud enough to elicit avoidance behavior in a flying female as she closely approaches a singing male (Fig. 5). The high frequency 'harmonics' of a model calling song were aversive even if presented with a normally attractive temporal pattern (pulse repetition rate of 16 pps) (Fig. 6A). When the 5 kHz 'fundamental' was added to one of the high frequency 'harmonics', in a two-tone stimulus paradigm, this complex model calling song was attractive; the high frequency 'harmonic' no longer elicited the avoidance behavior (Fig. 6) and the animals steered toward the model CS. Thus, addition of 5 kHz to a high frequency harmonic of the calling song 'masked' the aversive nature of this stimulus.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)  相似文献   

7.
  1. The courtship behavior of male field crickets, Gryllus bimaculatus (De Geer) and Teleogryllus oceanicus, is a complex, multimodal behavioral act that involves acoustic signals (a courtship song; Fig. 1A,B). The dominant frequency is 4.5 kHz for T. oceanicus song (Fig. 1A) and 13.5 kHz for G. bimaculatus (Fig. IB).
  2. When courting males are deprived of their courtship song by wing amputation, their courtship success declines markedly but is restored when courting is accompanied by tape-recordings of their courtship songs or a synthetic courtship song with only the dominant frequency of the natural song; other naturally occurring frequency components are ineffective for restoring mating success (Figs. 4, 5).
  3. It has been suggested that an identified auditory interneuron, AN2, plays a critical role in courtship success. Chronic recordings of AN2 in an intact, tethered female show that AN2's response to the natural courtship song and synthesized songs at 4.5 and 13.5 kHz is similar in T. oceanicus. By contrast, in G. bimaculatus, AN2's response to the natural courtship song and synthesized song at 13.5 kHz, but not at 4.5 kHz, is similar (Figs. 2,3).
  4. In behavioral experiments, playback of a 30 kHz synthetic courtship song in G. bimaculatus does not restore courtship success, yet this same stimulus elicits as strong a response from AN2 as does the normal courtship song (Fig. 6). Thus, contrary to earlier work by others, we conclude AN2 is not, by itself, a critical neural link in the courtship behavior of these two species of crickets.
  相似文献   

8.
Summary Two ventral-cord neurons in the auditory system ofGryllus bimaculatus were studied electrophysiologically by stimulation with pulses of sound at a single frequency (sine-wave pulses), stridulatory songs, and artificial sounds constructed to imitate the conspecific songs. The sine-wave pulses were varied in frequency, sound intensity, duration, and repetition rate. The stridulatory songs were the conspecific calling, aggressive, and courtship songs and the calling songs of 8 sympatric gryllids (played back at different sound intensities). The artificial songs were varied in carrier frequency, pulse rate, chirp rate, and sound intensity.The LF1 neuron precisely duplicates the temporal structure of the conspecific calling (and aggressive) song over the whole intensity range (Figs. 7, 8, 10). It is sharply tuned to the carrier frequency of the song (5 kHz) and shows little or no response above 10 kHz and below 3 kHz (Figs. 1, 2). By variation of the calling song's temporal structure it can be demonstrated that the LF1 neuron is particularly suited to respond to the pulse duration and the pulse and chirp repetition rates of this song pattern (Figs. 6, 9).On the other hand, the HF1 neuron is a broad-band neuron with a maximal sensitivity at 16 kHz (Figs. 1, 4); it is tuned to the conspecific courtship song with respect to carrier frequency, the short pulse duration, and the very low pulse repetition rate (Figs. 6, 7, 8).The results demonstrate that the two ventral-cord neurons represent highly evolved channels of the auditory pathway in gryllids, each of which transmits important features of the corresponding conspecific songs to several areas of the brain (Fig. 11). But they are not ideal filters for these conspecific songs, since they also respond to many other sound signals (Fig. 10).Supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft as part of the program Sonderforschungsbereich 114 (Bionach), BochumUnder the auspices of the scientist exchange program of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and the Academy of Sciences, USSRWe thank Prof. Dr. Schwartzkopff for his help and support; it was due to his initiative and organization that this work could be done in collaboration between the Sechenov Institute, Leningrad, and the Lehrstuhl für Allgemeine Zoologie, Ruhr University, Bochum. We are grateful to Mrs. I. Klotz and Mrs. B. Brücher for technical assistance.  相似文献   

9.
Summary Phonotactic behavior was studied in male crickets,Teleogryllus oceanicus. Tethered flying males were presented with electronically synthesized calling song models in a two-choice phonotaxis assay, and their song preferences were determined and compared with previous findings for females.Males are poorer at discriminating between songs than females; they do not display choice behavior as frequently as females, and the choices they do make are not as consistent as those of females (Figs. 3, 4). T. oceanicus calling song is composed of rhythmically different chirp and trill sections. The selectivity of males for these two components differs from that of females. Females prefer chirp to trill, but the opposite is true for males (Fig. 5B-F). Males are similar to females in that they prefer either a conspecific song model or its separate components to a heterospecific model (Fig. 5A, G, H).Behavioral and neural implications of these findings are discussed.  相似文献   

10.
Summary Intact and one-eared crickets,Gryllus bimaculatus, were tested for phonotactic behavior in a closed-loop and an open-loop situation and for related physiological characteristics of an identified auditory neuron pair, the left and the right AN2.Intact animals that performed phonotaxis in the closed-loop condition showed intended turning tendencies in the open-loop condition that correlated with the directional characteristics of their AN2s (Figs. 1–3).Animals in which one foreleg had been amputated during postembryonic development (one-eared regenerates) were classified according to their phonotactic performance as tracking or unoriented animals. The ability of one-eared regenerates to track a sound source was closely correlated with the direction of turning tendencies in the open-loop behavior and to specific features of their AN2 pair (Figs. 4–6).Some animals with one foreleg amputated as adults (one-eared amputees) perform stable phonotactic walking. Their open-loop behavior, however, is different from that of the tracking one- eared regenerates (Fig. 7).One-eared amputees showed stable phonotactic walking when calling song was presented from above and the sound intensity was varied according to the actual walking angle of the animal. The only orientational cue under this condition is the difference of sound intensity at different walking directions (Figs. 8–11).Different mechanisms are discussed for sound localization in one-eared regenerates and one- eared amputees based on turning tendencies which depend on the instantaneous stimulus intensity or on the intensity change between successive stimuli.  相似文献   

11.
Summary InGryllus bimaculatus females one foreleg was amputated at the coxa-trochanter joint in the 2nd, 4th or 8th/9th larval instar. A leg of up to normal length is regenerated (Fig. 1) but it lacks a functional ear. In spite of the, usually shorter, regenerated foreleg, the adult one-eared crickets show no impairments in walking when tested on a locomotion compensator. Without sound they walk erratically and most of them weakly circle towards the intact side (Fig. 2).With calling song presentation three response types can be distinguished:tracking (Fig. 3A), hanging on (Fig. 3B) or continuouscircling towards the intact side (Fig. 3C, D). Turning tendencies in monaurals increase with song intensity and exceed those of intact and bilaterally operated animals (Fig. 4). Course deviations towards the intact side also slightly increase with intensity (Fig. 5). Course stability is reduced compared to that of intact animals but exceeds that of bilaterally operated crickets (Figs. 5, 6). It is best at 60 dB and deteriorates at higher sound intensities (Fig. 6). The percentage of monaurals tracking or hanging on decreases with increasing intensity (Fig. 7B). Tracking is established in most animals but it is limited to a narrow intensity range (Fig. 7A, C). Apart from an increased percentage of tracking after early operations (Fig. 7D), there are no prominent changes in orientational parameters with the date of foreleg amputation.Reamputation of the regenerated leg in the adult monaurals does not significantly impair acoustic orientation (Figs. 8, 9), but occlusion of the ipsilateral prothoracic spiracle does (Figs. 10, 11).An attempt is made to correlate the behavioral performance with the activity of auditory interneurons which have undergone morphological and physiological changes (Fig. 12).  相似文献   

12.
Summary The activity of auditory receptor cells and prothoracic auditory neurons of the cricket,Gryllus bimaculatus, was recorded intracellularly while the animal walked on a sphere or while passive movement was imposed on a foreleg.During walking the responses to simulated calling song is altered since (i) the auditory sensory cells and interneurons discharged impulses in the absence of sound stimuli (Figs. 1, 3) and (ii) the number of action potentials in response to sound is reduced in interneurons (Figs. 2, 3).These two effects occurred in different phases of the leg movement during walking and therefore masked, suppressed or did not affect the responses to auditory stimuli (Figs. 3, 4). Hence there is a time window within which the calling song can be detected during walking (Fig. 5).The extra excitation of receptors and interneurons is probably produced by vibration of the tympanum because (i) the excitation occurred at the same time as the leg placement (Fig. 4), (ii) during walking on only middle and hindlegs, no extra action potentials were observed (Fig. 6), (iii) in certain phases of passive movements receptor cells and interneurons were excited as long as the ipsilateral ear was not blocked (Figs. 8, 9).Suppression of auditory responses seems to be peripheral as well as central in origin because (i) it occurred at particular phases during active and passive leg movements in receptor cells and interneurons (Figs. 1, 4, 9), (ii) it disappeared if the ear was blocked during passive leg movements (Fig. 9) and (iii) it persisted if the animal walked only on the middle and hind legs (Fig. 6).  相似文献   

13.
We compare the temporal and directional processing properties of an identified auditory interneuron, ON1, between species with calling songs containing relatively low and high pulse rates (Teleogryllus oceanicus and Gryllus texensis, respectively). Using information theory, we find that the ON1 of G. texensis encodes higher amplitude-modulation frequencies than that of T. oceanicus. Bilateral differences in ON1 responses are also more pronounced in G. texensis, particularly for rapid, G. texensis-like stimuli. We show that brief silent intervals in a pulse train, such as those that occur in the natural calling song of G. texensis, enhance the representation of the stimulus pulse pattern as well as bilateral differences in activity. Our results suggest that the characteristics of an identified neuron vary, across cricket species, in accordance with the temporal structures of their communication signals.  相似文献   

14.
Summary In females of the acridid grasshopperChorthippus biguttulus, thoracic auditory interneurons were investigated with respect to their selectivity for temporal parameters of the conspecific song. Special attention was given to the detection of small gaps in the syllables of the song, since behavioural experiments have shown that the presence or absence of gaps is critical for the female's Innate Releasing Mechanism (cf. Fig. 1).The spiking response of one ascending interneuron, the AN4, shows filtering properties which closely resemble the behavioural reactions (cf. Figs. 1, 3 and 5b). The difference in the AN4's reaction to stimuli with gaps and uninterrupted stimuli is maintained over the behaviourally relevant intensity range (Fig. 4). This reaction is reliable enough that the stimulus type could be inferred by higher centres even from single stimulus presentations. Hence, this neuron is likely to participate in the task of gap detection and probably is a part of the neuronal filter network which determines the characteristics of the Innate Releasing Mechanism of this species. However, this interneuron is not species-specific: A homologue exists in other acridids as well and, inLocusta migratoria, has similar response characteristics (Fig. 6). The inferences of this observation for the evolution of an Innate Releasing Mechanism are discussed.Abbreviations CNS central nervous system - PST-histogram post-stimulus-time-histogram - SPL sound pressure level - IRM Innate Releasing Mechanism  相似文献   

15.
EPSP waveforms were recorded from the omega neuron of Teleogryllus oceanicus for 5 kHz and ultrasonic sound stimuli. EPSPs in response to 5 kHz stimuli were smooth in shape and increased in amplitude with increasing stimulus intensity, while responses to ultrasound consisted of series' of large, discrete, unitary EPSPs, which increased in frequency with stimulus intensity.The hypothesis that a few, synaptically potent receptors might account for ultrasound sensitivity was tested by examining temporal coupling between ultrasound responses of the omega neuron and of another ultrasound-sensitive neuron, INT-1. INT-1 spikes were temporally correlated both to omega neuron spikes and to the large EPSPs recorded in the omega neuron. Coupling was not apparent for 5 kHz stimuli.The omega neuron encodes the intensity of 5 kHz and ultrasonic stimuli with similar resolution. Response latencies are markedly shorter for ultrasonic stimuli.These findings suggest that 5 kHz information is carried by a relatively large number of receptors, each of which has only a small effect on central neurons, while ultrasound information is carried by a few, synaptically potent, receptors.  相似文献   

16.
The effect of ultrasound on the attractiveness of acoustic mating signals   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Abstract. Previous laboratory studies ( Nolen & Hoy 1986b ) have shown that the phonotactic responses of flying crickets are influenced by the relative intensities of attractive (mating signal) and repulsive (predator) stimuli. At the functional level, these results suggest that predator cues (ultrasound) can change the attractiveness of a calling song. Using extracellular recordings from cervical connectives it was shown that, like other field crickets, Gryllus rubens (south-eastern field cricket) is sensitive to ultrasound. This ultrasonic sensitivity has probably evolved in response to predation pressure from echolocating bats. Using acoustic playback under field conditions, it was tested whether the relative attractiveness of two male calling songs was influenced by the simultaneous broadcast of ultrasound. A simulated male calling song of G. rubens was broadcast at two different intensities (109 and 103 dB) from two sound traps that caught flying crickets attracted to the songs. Simulated bat ultrasound was broadcast simultaneously with the high-intensity calling song (109 dB) and the relative catch in each of the two traps was measured. The intensity of the ultrasound was varied on different nights. The relative attractiveness of the high-intensity sound trap decreased significantly as the intensity of the ultrasound broadcast with it was increased. For the lowest of the ultrasound broadcast levels, the relative attractiveness did not differ from that expected for two calling songs broadcast without ultrasound. Thus, increased levels of simulated predation risk decreased the attractiveness of the calling song associated with it. These are the first field experiments to show that predation risk in the form of simulated bat ultrasound influences the phonotactic behaviour of flying crickets.  相似文献   

17.
Female crickets lacking experience with phonotaxis to conspecific calling song respond to trains of continuously repeated sound pulses (trill), whereas experienced females do not. In the present study such inexperienced crickets were tested for their responsiveness to trills of pulse repetition periods from 30 to 70 ms on a Y- shaped maze. Stimulation with a repetition period of 30 ms led to unexpectedly low phonotactic and exploratory activity. Initial stimulation with trills of 30- ms repetition period drastically reduced the responsiveness of inexperienced animals to conspecific calling song and other attractive stimuli. Effects of visual stimulation on the phonotactic behavior of female crickets are demonstrated. Threatening visual stimuli changed the behavior of experienced animals to a state that resembles that of inexperienced animals. The relevance of these observations is discussed with respect to the development of the auditory pattern recognition mechanism in crickets.  相似文献   

18.
Acoustically-signaling animals such as crickets may experience interference from environmental noise, a particular concern given the rise in anthropogenic or other novel sources of sound. We examined the potential for acoustic interference of female phonotaxis to calling song in the Pacific field cricket (Teleogryllus oceanicus) by invasive coqui frogs (Eleutherodactylus coqui) in Hawaii. The frogs were introduced to Hawaii from Puerto Rico in the 1980s. When female crickets were exposed to male calling songs with and without simultaneous broadcast of a coqui chorus, they were equally likely to move toward the cricket song, regardless of the location of the frog sound (ground level or above ground). Unlike some species of frogs and birds, T. oceanicus do not appear to experience acoustic interference from an introduced signaler, even though the introduced species’ calls subjectively seem to be masking the crickets’ songs.  相似文献   

19.
The field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus has been introduced to Hawaii, where it is parasitized by an acoustically orienting parasitoid fly, Ormia ochracea. Previous work showed that call parameters from parasitized populations differ from those in unparasitized populations in a direction expected if selection by flies is occurring. Here we examined songs of males collected in the field and compare calling song characters of crickets later found to harbor parasitoid larvae with those of males free of parasitoids. The two groups differ significantly in several song characteristics, particularly the trill-like long chirp given at the beginning of each song. Males with longer long chirps containing shorter interpulse intervals are more likely to be parasitized, suggesting that the flies find such males more attractive. Depending on the traits females prefer, sexual selection may oppose natural selection in altering T. oceanicus song in parasitized populations.  相似文献   

20.
Nearest neighbor analyses of the field crickets Gryllus integer, G. veletis, and Teleogryllus oceanicus demonstrated that calling ♂♂ were aggregated. Broadcasts of conspecific song to calling ♂♂ indicated that attraction of neighboring ♂♂ maintained inter-male distances. Broadcasts of G. integer song through aggregated and isolated loudspeakers showed that the total number of crickets and parasitoid flies, Euphasiopteryx ochracea, attracted to aggregated loudspeakers was greater than that to an isolate. The average number of attracted crickets and flies in an aggregation was comparable to the isolated total.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号