共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
Lilly Alecia A. Mehlman Patrick T. Doran Diane 《International journal of primatology》2002,23(3):555-573
We report prevalences and eggs/protozoa per g (EPG; PPG) of helminths and protozoa in gorillas, chimpanzees, agile mangabeys, indigenous Ba'Aka and Bantu, and western researchers at a remote field site in the Central African Republic. We examined fecal samples for eggs, larvae, proglottids, cysts, amoeba, trophozoites, and flagellates. For helminths, strongylates were most prevalent, infecting 82–94% of nonhuman primates (NH) and 30–93% of human (H) groups, followed by ascaroids (14–88% NH; 0–15% H), and threadworms (0–22% NH; 0–29% H). For protozoa, Entamoeba histolytica (2–100% NH; 33–52% H) and trichomonads (11–88% NH; 0–54% H) were most prevalent. Among gorilla samples (n = 156) there were significant age/sex differences in EPG/PPG for strongylates, threadworms, Entamoeba histolytica, and trichomonads, with infants exhibiting the highest mean EPG/PPG for all parasites except trichomonads. Between group analyses revealed that the Ba'Aka had significantly higher mean EPG of strongylates, ascaroids and threadworms than all other primate groups, except the mangabeys. For Entamoeba histolytica, E. coli, Balantidium coli, and Iodamoeba butschlii, the agile mangabeys had significantly higher mean PPG than other groups; for trichomonads, the chimpanzees, and mangabeys had the highest mean PPG. Relative to other African ape sites, the gorillas and chimpanzees at Mondika appear to have high prevalences of intestinal parasites. This may be the result of the high proportion of swamp and seasonally flooded areas, which provide optimal viability for parasite eggs and ova. At Mondika, the significantly higher parasite levels of Ba'Aka probably result from more traditional methods of hygiene and lack of available medical treatment. All workers at research sites should be monitored and treated to minimize cross-transmission between humans and local fauna. 相似文献
3.
Though many modern techniques are available for studying brains, they are difficult to use in evolutionary contexts that require examination of large numbers of specimens and species, and all major parts of the brain. Thus, evolutionary studies of many species and of whole brains still tend to be based upon simpler data such as sizes of brains and brain components. Such investigations, carried out over many decades, have usually employed univariate and bivariate analyses, though a few investigators used early multivariate methods. In mammals, these studies generally show the primacy of the relationship between brain and brain-part sizes with overall body size. More recent multivariate applications have confirmed this (Finlay, B. L., and Darlington, R. B. (1995). Science 268: 1578–1584) and some have also separated the highest level phylogenetic groups: strepsirrhines and haplorrhines (Barton, R. A., and Harvey, P. H. (2000). Nature 405: 1055–1058). Both findings were, in fact, evident in earlier multivariate studies (Holloway, R. L. (1979). In Hahn, M. E., Jensen C., and Dudek, B. C. (eds.), Development and Evolution of Brain Size: Behavioral Implications, Academic Press, New York, pp. 59–88; Sacher, G. A. (1970). In Noback, C. R., and Montagna, W. (eds.), The Primate Brain: Advances in Primatology. Vol. 1, Appleton-Century-Crofts, Educational Division, Meredith Corporation, New York, pp. 245–287). However, new studies employing proportional data aimed at conveying input/output relationships between brain components show further groupings of species that share convergences in lifestyles (de Winter, W., and Oxnard, C. E. (2001). Nature 409: 710–714). The convergences are brought about by combinations of brain variables that seem to be associated with brain functions implied by the specific lifestyles. Our most recent results demonstrate that chimpanzees and humans are especially different from one another, and the difference is not due to size alone. Part of this difference is merely a continuation, from chimpanzees towards humans, of a trend already present across all other primates that relates mainly to neocortical increase. But several other large and independent differences are not in the direction of the overall primate trend, but are differences of humans from all other mammals examined including all nonhuman primates. The combinations of brain variables associated with the latter differences are not related simply to enhancement of the neocortex, but seem to reflect other internal relationships. The overall separation of humans and chimpanzees is so large that it goes far beyond the conventional 98.6% commonality in their DNAs. It fits better with more recent molecular, developmental and evolutionary studies implying a considerably greater difference between chimpanzees and humans than usually recognized. 相似文献
4.
We compared sex differences in behaviors leading to copulation of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in the Kalinzu Forest, Uganda with those of bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba, D.R. Congo, using the same definition. Female chimpanzees were more likely to initiate copulation than female bonobos. While most of copulations (96%) were initiated by males in bonobos, among chimpanzees only 63% of copulations were initiated by males. Female bonobos initiated an interaction leading to copulation when males approached them within a short distance. On the other hand, both male and female chimpanzees initiated behavior at a longer distance. Higher proceptivity and a higher copulation rate during the maximal swelling period of female chimpanzees might suggest that they gain greater benefits from a high frequency of copulations than do female bonobos. 相似文献
5.
Hiroshi Ihobe 《Primates; journal of primatology》1997,38(4):351-357
Interspecific relations between wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) and two species of guenons (Cercopithecus wolfi andC. ascanius) were studied at Wamba in the Central Zaire Basin from September 1989 to January 1990. Data on the guenons were collected
while following parties of bonobos or when searching for them. The guenons were observed directly 59 times during the study
period. In about half of these observations, the guenons were found within 20 m from the bonobo parties. The encounters between
the bonobos and the guenons sometimes lasted over an hour. The guenons mainly initiated the encounters by approaching the
bonobos. During the encounters, no aggressive interactions were observed between the bonobos and the guenons. Evidence of
hunting by wild bonobos has been restricted to small mammals, and there has been no evidence of hunting of primates by wild
bonobos. These findings and the results of the present study strongly suggest that wild bonobos do not hunt sympatric primates. 相似文献
6.
Traditional posturographic analysis and four statistical mechanics techniques were applied to center-of-pressure (COP) trajectories of young, older "low-fall-risk" and older "high-fall-risk" individuals. Low-fall-risk older adults were active 3 days per week in a cardiac rehabilitation program, while high-fall-risk older adults were diagnosed with perilymph fistula. Subjects diagnosed with perilymph fistula must have experienced two of the following vestibular findings: constant disequilibrium, positional vertigo and/or a positive fistula test. Non-parametric statistical tests were used to determine whether the posturographic measures could detect differences between the young and older "low-fall-risk" groups (age comparison) and between the older "low-" and "high-risk" groups (risk of falling comparison). The statistical mechanics techniques were more sensitive than the traditional measures: detecting significant differences between the young and older "low-risk" groups, while none of the traditional measures were significantly different. In addition, interpretation of the statistical mechanics techniques may offer more insight into the nature of the process controlling the COP trajectories. However, the methods offered slightly different explanations. For instance, the Hurst rescaled range analysis suggests that the movement of the COP is governed solely by anti-persistent behavior, whereas the stabilogram diffusion analysis suggests a short-term persistence balanced by a long-term anti-persistence. These discrepancies highlight the need for a model that incorporates the biological systems responsible for maintaining balance and experimental methods to directly quantify their status and roles. Until such a model exists, however, the statistical mechanics techniques appear to have some advantages over traditional posturographic measures for studying balance control. 相似文献
7.
Neural signatures of economic preferences for risk and ambiguity 总被引:9,自引:0,他引:9
People often prefer the known over the unknown, sometimes sacrificing potential rewards for the sake of surety. Overcoming impulsive preferences for certainty in order to exploit uncertain but potentially lucrative options may require specialized neural mechanisms. Here, we demonstrate by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) that individuals' preferences for risk (uncertainty with known probabilities) and ambiguity (uncertainty with unknown probabilities) predict brain activation associated with decision making. Activation within the lateral prefrontal cortex was predicted by ambiguity preference and was also negatively correlated with an independent clinical measure of behavioral impulsiveness, suggesting that this region implements contextual analysis and inhibits impulsive responses. In contrast, activation of the posterior parietal cortex was predicted by risk preference. Together, this novel double dissociation indicates that decision making under ambiguity does not represent a special, more complex case of risky decision making; instead, these two forms of uncertainty are supported by distinct mechanisms. 相似文献
8.
Old World monkeys are more similar to humans than New World monkeys when playing a coordination game
Brosnan SF Wilson BJ Beran MJ 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2012,279(1733):1522-1530
There is much debate about how humans' decision-making compares with that of other primates. One way to explore this is to compare species' performance using identical methodologies in games with strategical interactions. We presented a computerized Assurance Game, which was either functionally simultaneous or sequential, to investigate how humans, rhesus monkeys and capuchin monkeys used information in decision-making. All species coordinated via sequential play on the payoff-dominant Nash equilibrium, indicating that information about the partner's choice improved decisions. Furthermore, some humans and rhesus monkeys found the payoff-dominant Nash equilibrium in the simultaneous game, even when it was the first condition presented. Thus, Old World primates solved the task without any external cues to their partner's choice. Finally, when not explicitly prohibited, humans spontaneously used language to coordinate on the payoff-dominant Nash equilibrium, indicating an alternative mechanism for converting a simultaneous move game into a sequential move game. This phylogenetic distribution implies that no single mechanism drives coordination decisions across the primates, while humans' ability to spontaneously use language to change the structure of the game emphasizes that multiple mechanisms may be used even within the same species. These results provide insight into the evolution of decision-making strategies across the primates. 相似文献
9.
Sacha Bourgeois-Gironde Elsa Addessi Thomas Boraud 《Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences》2021,376(1819)
Do we have any valid reasons to affirm that non-human primates display economic behaviour in a sufficiently rich and precise sense of the phrase? To address this question, we have to develop a set of criteria to assess the vast array of experimental studies and field observations on individual cognitive and behavioural competences as well as the collective organization of non-human primates. We review a sample of these studies and assess how they answer to the following four main challenges. (i) Do we see any economic organization or institutions emerge among groups of non-human primates? (ii) Are the cognitive abilities, and often biases, that have been evidenced as underlying typical economic decision-making among humans, also present among non-human primates? (iii) Can we draw positive lessons from performance comparisons among primate species, humans and non-humans but also across non-human primate species, as elicited by canonical game-theoretical experimental paradigms, especially as far as economic cooperation and coordination are concerned? And (iv) in which way should we improve models and paradigms to obtain more ecological data and conclusions? Articles discussed in this paper most often bring about positive answers and promising perspectives to support the existence and prevalence of economic behaviours among non-human primates.This article is part of the theme issue ‘Existence and prevalence of economic behaviours among non-human primates’. 相似文献
10.
Gottfried Hohmann 《Primates; journal of primatology》2001,42(1):91-99
This study reports on close spatial association and repeated behavioural interactions between two strange adult male bonobos with residents of another community. Over a period of 12 months one of the two males developed friendly social relations to some of the females and other residents, which were indistinguishable from those existing between co-residents. Aggression by resident males against the strangers decreased but the former remained intolerant. The strange males appeared at a time when the number of adult resident males was lower as in the years before and when the adult sex ratio (number of adult females per male) was higher as in the years before. Using definitions from studies on dispersal patterns of male gorillas (Harcourt, 1978) and female bonobos (Furuichi, 1989) the spatial association between the two strange males and residents could be described as male transfer. 相似文献
11.
This paper aims at quantifying ontogenetic differences between bonobo (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) endocrania, using dental development as a timeline. We utilize a methodology based on smooth and invertible deformations combined with a metric of “currents” that defines a distance between endocranial surfaces and does not rely on correspondence between landmarks. This allows us to perform a temporal surface regression that estimates typical endocranial ontogenetic trajectories separately for bonobos and chimpanzees. We highlight non-linear patterns of endocranial ontogenetic change and significant differences between species at local anatomical levels rather than considering the endocranium as a uniform entity. A spatiotemporal registration permits the quantification of inter-species differences decomposed into a morphological deformation (accounting for size and shape differences independently of age) and a time warp (accounting for changes in the dynamics of development). Our statistical simulations suggest that patterns of endocranial volume (EV) increase may differ significantly between bonobos and chimpanzees, with an earlier phase of a relatively rapid increase (preferentially at some endocranial subdivisions) in the former and a much later phase of relatively rapid increase in the latter. As a consequence, the chimpanzee endocranium appears to reach its adult size later. Moreover, the time warp indicates that juvenile bonobos develop much slower than juvenile chimpanzees, suggesting that inter-specific ontogenetic shifts do not only concern EV increase, but also the rate of shape changes over time. Our method provides, for the first time, a quantitative estimation of inter-specific ontogenetic shifts that appear to differentiate non-linearly. 相似文献
12.
13.
Doxiadis GG Rouweler AJ de Groot NG Louwerse A Otting N Verschoor EJ Bontrop RE 《Immunogenetics》2006,58(4):259-268
In contrast to rhesus monkeys, substantial knowledge on cynomolgus monkey major histocompatibility complex (MHC) class II haplotypes is lacking. Therefore, 17 animals, including one pedigreed family, were thoroughly characterized for polymorphic Mhc class II region genes as well as their mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) sequences. Different cynomolgus macaque populations appear to exhibit unique mtDNA profiles reflecting their geographic origin. Within the present panel, 10 Mafa-DPB1, 14 Mafa-DQA1, 12 Mafa-DQB1, and 35 Mafa-DRB exon 2 sequences were identified. All of these alleles cluster into lineages that were previously described for rhesus macaques. Moreover, about half of the Mafa-DPB1, Mafa-DQA1, and Mafa-DQB1 alleles and one third of the Mafa-DRB exon 2 sequences are identical to rhesus macaque orthologues. Such a high level of Mhc class II allele sharing has not been reported for primate species. Pedigree analysis allowed the characterization of nine distinct Mafa class II haplotypes, and seven additional ones could be deduced. Two of these haplotypes harbor a duplication of the Mafa-DQB1 locus. Despite extensive allele sharing, rhesus and cynomolgus monkeys do not appear to possess identical Mhc class II haplotypes, thus illustrating that new haplotypes were generated after speciation by recombination-like processes. 相似文献
14.
Katherine von Stackelberg Maura Nelson Barbara Southworth Todd Bridges 《人类与生态风险评估》2007,13(5):1053-1077
We evaluate risk drivers at selected U.S. Army installations by developing a database containing contaminant-pathway-receptor combinations that exceed regulatory thresholds for ecological (toxicity quotient greater than one), human health cancer risk (predicted incremental lifetime cancer risk greater than one in ten thousand), and noncancer human health (hazard index greater than one). We compare the risk drivers from the database to reported corrective action objectives from available decision documents. For noncancer hazards, explosives (particularly in ground water) dominate the reported exceedances of regulatory thresholds in the database. PAHs in home-grown produce show the highest number of exceedances of regulatory thresholds for cancer risk. For ecological risks, PAHs in both terrestrial and aquatic environments dominate the exceedances of regulatory thresholds. All available cleanup levels were derived based on human health exposures rather than ecological exposures, except for one site. In general, ecological risks were considered to be “more uncertain,” and that was used as a basis for not relying on backcalculated target levels on the basis of ecological risk. The reverse was true for human health risks: the “conservative” assumptions incorporated into the modeling provided the justification for backcalculating health-protective target levels. 相似文献
15.
The effects of benextramine and nifedipine were examined on the dose-diastolic pressure response to methoxamine in pithed normotensive rats. Benextramine (3, 6 and 12 mg/Kg) displaced the dose-response curve to methoxamine to the right. Maximum response was reduced after the administration of 12 mg/Kg benextramine. Nifedipine (0.1 and 0.3 mg/Kg) also caused the dose-response curve to methoxamine to be displaced to the right with reduction in maximum response. Nifedipine effects were additive with an increase in the EC50 values as well as reduction in the maximum response after pretreatment with benextramine (3 and 6 mg/Kg). However, at the highest dose of benextramine the effects of nifedipine were diminished and no longer apparent. It is concluded that benextramine may have alkylated a nifedipine sensitive site on the alpha 1-adrenoceptors. 相似文献
16.
The geographic ranges of rhesus ( Macaca mulatta ) and cynomolgus ( M. fascicularis ) macaques adjoin in Indochina where they appear to hybridize. We used published and newly generated DNA sequences from 19 loci spanning ~20 kb to test whether introgression has occurred between these macaque species. We studied introgression at the level of nuclear DNA and distinguished between incomplete lineage sorting of ancestral polymorphisms or interspecific gene flow. We implemented a divergence population genetics approach by fitting our data to an isolation model implemented in the software IMa. The model that posits no gene flow from the rhesus into the cynomolgus macaque was rejected ( P = 1.99 × 10−8 ). Gene flow in this direction was estimated as 2 Nm ~1.2, while gene flow in the reverse direction was nonsignificantly different from zero ( P = 0.16). The divergence time between species was estimated as ~1.3 million years. Balancing selection, a special case of incomplete sorting, was taken into consideration, as well as potential crossbreeding in captivity. Parameter estimates varied between analyses of subsets of data, although we still rejected isolation models. Geographic sampling of the data, where samples of cynomolgus macaques derived from Indochina were excluded, revealed a lost signature of gene flow, indicating that interspecific gene flow is restricted to mainland Indochina. Our results, in conjunction with those by others, justify future detailed analyses into the genetics of reproductive barriers and reticulate evolution in these two genome-enabled primates. Future studies of the natural hybridization between rhesus and cynomolgus macaques would expand the repertoire of systems available for speciation studies in primates. 相似文献
17.
Immunogenicity of a DNA prime and recombinant adenovirus boost regime significantly varies between rhesus macaques of Chinese and Indian origins 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Stahl-Hennig C Suh YS Park KS Sauermann U Kim KS Ahn S Franz M Schulte R Stolte-Leeb N Hunsmann G Sung YC 《Journal of medical primatology》2007,36(4-5):195-205
BACKGROUND: Due to an ever increasing shortage of rhesus macaques of Indian origin (InR) that have been generally used for preclinical AIDS vaccine trials in non-human primates, demand is rising for Chinese rhesus macaques (ChR). However, the immunogenicity of an AIDS vaccine candidate has not been compared in parallel in both rhesus macaque subspecies. METHODS: ChR and InR were immunized with SIV/HIV DNA and adenovirus vaccine and their immune responses to SIV and HIV evaluated. RESULTS: SIV Gag- and Env-specific T-cell responses and SIV-specific lymphoproliferative responses measured in ChR were significantly weaker than those in InR (P < 0.05). By contrast, antibody responses to SIV Env, Tat, and Nef in ChR were stronger than those in InR (P < 0.05). CONCLUSIONS: Immunogenicity of an AIDS vaccine can vary significantly depending on the geographic origin implying genetic differences of macaques. This must be considered when describing and interpreting results of such vaccine studies. 相似文献
18.
Takeshi Furuichi 《Primates; journal of primatology》2009,50(3):197-209
Differences in party size and cohesiveness among females have been primary topics in socio-ecological comparisons of chimpanzees
(Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus). This paper aims to review previous studies that attempted to explain these differences and propose some hypotheses to be
tested in future studies. Comparisons of recent data show that relative party size (expressed as a percentage of total group
size) is significantly larger for bonobos than chimpanzees. Although the prolonged estrus of females, close association between
mother and adult sons, female social relationships including unique homosexual behavior, and high female social status might
be related to the increased party size and female cohesiveness of bonobos, these social and behavioral factors alone do not
appear to explain the differences between the two species. Differences in ecological factors, including fruit-patch size,
density of terrestrial herbs, and the availability of scattered foods that animals forage as they travel between large fruit
patches could also contribute to the differences between chimpanzees and bonobos. However, these factors cannot fully account
for the increased party size and female cohesiveness of bonobos. The higher female cohesiveness in bonobos may be explained
by socio-ecological systems that reduce the cost in feeding efficiency incurred by attending mixed-sex parties. These systems
may include female initiatives for party ranging movements as well as the factors mentioned above. Because of their geographical
isolation, the two species probably evolved different social systems. Chimpanzees, whose habitats became very dry during some
periods in the Pleistocene, likely evolved more flexible fission–fusion social systems to cope with seasonal and annual variation
in food availability. On the other hand, bonobos had a large refugia forest in the middle of their range even during the driest
periods in the Pleistocene. Therefore bonobos, whose habitats had more abundant food and smaller variation in food availability,
probably evolved systems that help females stay in mixed parties without incurring large costs from contest and scramble competition. 相似文献
19.
Vereecke E D'Août K De Clercq D Van Elsacker L Aerts P 《Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology》2004,75(4):266-278
The aim of this paper is twofold. Firstly, we investigate whether contact times, as recorded by pedobarographic systems during quadrupedal and bipedal walking of bonobos, can be used to reliably calculate actual velocities, by applying formulae based on lateral-view video recordings. Secondly, we investigate the effect of speed on peak plantar pressures during bipedal and quadrupedal walking of the bonobo. Data were obtained from 4 individuals from a group of bonobos at the Animal Park Planckendael. From our study, we can conclude that both walking speeds calculated from contact times and lower leg length or simply from recorded contact times are good estimators for walking speed, when direct observation of the latter is impossible. Further, it was found that effects of speed on peak plantar pressures and vertical forces are absent or at least subtle in comparison to a large variation in pressure patterns. In bonobos, the same pressure patterns are used at all walking speeds, and, in consequence, we do not expect major changes in foot function. 相似文献
20.
Michael V. Hurley Kim E. Lowell David C. Cook Shuang Liu Abu-Baker Siddique Art Diggle 《人类与生态风险评估》2010,16(6):1379-1394
A decision framework called Deliberative Multi-Criteria Evaluation (DMCE) was developed and deployed to prioritize biosecurity risks using a variety of subjective and objective information. To aid stakeholders in the prioritization of Emergency Plant Pest (EPP) species risk, we presented them with outputs from a Stella-based bio-economic pest risk model, and probable ecological and socioeconomic impacts. The stakeholder participants weighted the consequence criteria they deemed to have the highest expected impact. The methodology featured an uncertain set of parameters, multiple iterations of criteria weighting along with real-time sensitivity analysis. Of the five criteria, economic cost was weighted the highest at 26% while landscape amenity was weighted the lowest at the 10–12% range. The increased understanding and support gained by stakeholders through the DMCE process provided a greater likelihood of the sanctioning of the policies concerned and progress toward desired outcomes. 相似文献