共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Davydov DV Mikhaĭlova ES Logunova NN Nikitaeva ES 《Zhurnal vysshe? nervno? deiatelnosti imeni I P Pavlova》2002,52(6):665-672
Visual evoked potentials (VEP) in standard 16 EEG derivations were recorded in 26 young men and 20 women during recognition of facial emotional expressions and geometric figures. The stimuli were presented on a computer screen in the center of the visual field or randomly in the right or left vision hemifields. Peak VEP latency and mean amplitude in 50-ms epochs were measured; spatiotemporal VEP dynamics was analyzed in a series of topographic maps. The right hemisphere was shown to be more important in processing emotional faces. The character of the asymmetry was dynamic: at earlier stages of emotion processing the electrical activity was higher in the right inferior temporal region compared to the left symmetrical site. Later on the activity was higher in the right frontal and central areas. The dynamic mapping of "face-selective" component N180 of VEPs revealed the onset of activation over the right frontal areas that was followed by the fast activation of symmetrical left zones. Notably, this dynamics didn't correlate with the hemifield of stimuli exposition. The degree of asymmetry was lower during presentation of figures, especially in the inferior temporal and frontal regions. The prominent asymmetry of information processes in the inferior temporal and frontal areas was suggested to be specific for recognition of facial expression. 相似文献
2.
Ch A Izma?lov S G Korshunova E N Sokolov 《Zhurnal vysshe? nervno? deiatelnosti imeni I P Pavlova》1999,49(2):186-199
The four-dimensional spherical emotional space was constructed by multidimensional scaling of visually perceived differences between emotional expressions of schematic faces. In this spherical model Euclidean distances between the points representing the schematic faces are directly proportional to perceived differences of emotional expressions. Three angles of the four-dimensional sphere correspond to specific characteristics of emotions, such as emotional modality (joy, fear, anger, etc.), intensity of emotions, and emotional fullness (saturation). At the same time Cartesian coordinates represent excitations in the neuronal channels encoding line orientations. It was shown that the structure of the emotional space is similar to the structure of color space, i.e., emotional modality corresponds to color hue, emotional intensity to brightness, and emotional fullness to color saturation. The obtained evidence suggests the common mechanisms of information coding in the visual system. 相似文献
3.
4.
Mikhaĭlov ES Rozenberg ES 《Zhurnal vysshe? nervno? deiatelnosti imeni I P Pavlova》2006,56(4):481-490
A correlation between some characteristics of the visual evoked potentials and individual personality traits (by the Kettell scale) was revealed in 40 healthy subjects when they recognized facial expressions of anger and fear. As compared to emotionally stable subjects, emotionally unstable subjects had shorter latencies of evoked potentials and suppressed late negativity in the occipital and temporal areas. In contrast, amplitude of these waves in the frontal areas was increased. In emotionally stable group of subjects differences in the evoked potentials related to emotional expressions were evident throughout the whole signal processing beginning from the early sensory stage (P1 wave). In emotionally unstable group differences in the evoked potentials related to recognized emotional expressions developed later. Sensitivity of the evoked potentials to emotional salience of faces was also more pronounced in the emotionally stable group. The involvement of the frontal cortex, amygdala, and the anterior cingulate cortex in the development of individual features of recognition of facial expressions of anger and fear is discussed. 相似文献
5.
6.
7.
A simple method is presented which by subtraction of amplitudes of averaged evoked potentials (AEP) within regular time-intervals enables us to estimate differences in the course of the potential. First results demonstrate that all parts of the potential may be influenced and that most striking differences must not necessarily occur in the peak region of the AEP. The late negative complex of the AEP is likely to be a result of summation of some subcomponents, which may be altered differently. The method presented allows exact measurements of each of them. 相似文献
8.
Kharauzov AK Pronin SV Sobolev AF Koskin SA Boĭko EV Shelepin IuE 《Rossi?skii fiziologicheski? zhurnal imeni I.M. Sechenova / Rossi?skaia akademiia nauk》2005,91(8):956-969
Electrophysiological measurements of the threshold spatial frequency were conducted in 26 healthy subjects by using visual evoked potentials with the purpose of objective determination of the visual acuity. For that we proposed a universal method of the visual stimulation and EEG processing (using ICA decomposition in particular) to minimize errors arising on account of individual differences in the visual system functioning. As a result, a correlation of 0.74 and a logarithmic dependence were obtained between spatial resolution measured by electrophysiological and psychophysical methods. The proposed methods of objective measurement of visual resolution has a high effectiveness, does not depend on specificity of individual EEG and domination of different channels in the visual system. Therefore it is possible to determinate objectively the visual acuity in humans independently of their responses. 相似文献
9.
V V Loginov I P Leshchinskaia L P Latash 《Zhurnal vysshe? nervno? deiatelnosti imeni I P Pavlova》1987,37(3):414-421
"Fast" and "slow" habituation of N1 and N1-P2 components of auditory evoked potentials was studied in healthy subjects and in depressed patients. In patients, initially more low amplitudes of N1 and N1-P2 were revealed, as well as slowing down of habituation in the beginning of stimuli series and acceleration to its end (in healthy people--the greatest habituation in the initial part of the series and amplitude increase at its end), the absence of power effect in the component N1 at reaching, in the process of habituation, of the same minimum parameters as in healthy subjects. This points to weakening of dishabituation process parallel with well expressed "slow" habituation in patients and allows to suggest at expressed negative emotions a deficit of attention processes as a result of "internal abstraction". 相似文献
10.
11.
12.
《Electronic Notes in Theoretical Computer Science》1984,59(6):441-453
Fifteen right-handed women evaluated the similarity of two faces presented to them either in the left or in the right visual field. The subjects' task was to decide whether the faces were the same or different and accordingly to press a button or abstain from doing so. Errors made by the subjects and the visual evoked potentials (VEPs) from points O1 and O2 referred to point Cz were recorded. Behavioural and electrophysiological results demonstrated the superiority of the right hemisphere in the perception of faces. The subjects made fewer errors when faces were presented in the left visual field. The number of errors also decreased when the presented faces were different from each other. Analysis of the VEPs indicates the existence of hemispheric asymmetry as early as 100 msec after the first stimulus disappeared. The asymmetry is larger in response to the second stimulus. On the basis of these results we conclude that differentiation in hemispheric functions takes place in the phase of short-term memory. The smaller number of errors made when the presented faces were different we explain by the strategy applied by the subjects. 相似文献
13.
In a complex choice reaction time experiment, patterned stimuli without luminance change were presented, and pattern-specific visual evoked potentials to lower half-field stimulation were recorded. Two experimental conditions were used. The first was the between-field selection, where square patterns were presented in either the lower or the upper half of the visual field. In a given stimulus run one of the half-fields was task-relevant, and the subjects' task was to press a microswitch to stimuli of higher duration value (GO stimuli), while they had to ignore shorter ones, i. e. stimuli of lower apparent spatial contrast (NOGO stimuli). They had to ignore the stimuli appearing in the irrelevant half-field (IRR stimuli). In order to ensure proper fixation, the subjects had to press another microswitch at the onset of a dim light at the fixation point (CRT stimuli). Our second experimental condition was the within-field selection, where the GO, NOGO, and IRR stimuli appeared in the lower half of the visual field. GO and NOGO were square patterns while IRR stimuli were constructed of circles, or vice versa. (The CRT stimuli were the same as in the previous condition.) Three pattern-specific visual evoked potential components were identified, i. e. CI (70 ms latency), CII (100 ms latency), and CIII (170 ms latency). There were marked selective attention effects on both the CI-CII and CII-CIII peak-to-peak amplitudes. In both experimental conditions, responses with the highest amplitude were evoked by the GO type of stimuli, while the IRR stimuli evoked the smallest responses. According to these results, attention effects on the pattern-specific visual evoked potentials in the first 200 ms cannot be attributed to a simple stimulus set kind of selection. 相似文献
14.
15.
Experiments on cats showed that complete operative exclusion of the reticular formation by precollicular section prevents the development of habituation of evoked potentials in the primary visual projection area and lateral geniculate body. Similar results were obtained after postcollicular section of the mesencephalon. The phenomenon of habituation of visual evoked potentials is found in posttrigeminal preparations. It is postulated that the tonic inhibitory division of the reticular formation plays an important role in the development of the habituation phenomenon.Odessa State Medical Institute. Translated from Neirofiziologiya, Vol. 4, No. 5, pp. 540–544, September–October, 1972. 相似文献
16.
The independent component analysis was applied to multichannel transient visual evoked potentials elicited by a high contrast pattern-reversal and motion-onset (motion velocity of 7 and 23 deg/s). Three overlapping independent components with different topographical distribution over the scalp were described. The first component displayed similar timing in response to all three stimuli (40-200 ms) but was a different in shape and scalp projection. This activation component is considered to reflect the stimulus properties. The second component (100-227 ms), related to negativity at about 160 ms, can be referred to visual processing of motion. The last component, attributed to positivity at 230 ms dominates in the fronto-central area and might represent a cognitive process. 相似文献
17.
18.
19.
20.
Electrophysiological correlates of the perception of emotional stimuli were studied by means of recording the visual evoked
potentials (EP) in 20 derivations (Fz, Cz, Pz, Oz, Fp
1/2,F
3/4,F
7/8,C
3/4,P
3/4,T
3/4,T
5/6, andO
1/2) during the emotional test performance. The performance of a special task by subjects was assessed positively or negatively
(by administering emotionally positive or negative stimuli, respectively). Factor analysis revealed seven factors, which described
the EP component structure. Analysis of variance demonstrated the influence of the emotional stimuli sign on the factorsP
100,P
140,N
160,P
220,P
340, and “slow wave.” Hemispheric difference in reactions to the stimuli of a different emotional sign were recorded. During
presentation of the positive and negative assessments, the amplitudes of the factorsP
100,P
340, and “slow wave” were maximally different in the left hemisphere, while the factorsN
160 andP
220 were maximally different in the right hemisphere. 相似文献