首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
An important prediction from game theory is that the value of a resource influences the level to which male-male conflict escalates. An earlier experimental study showed that males of Hypoblemum albovittatum, a common salticid in New Zealand, can discern by sight alone whether a female is in the vicinity and that, having detected a female's presence, males escalate (i.e., adopt behaviour that is likely to put them at greater risk of injury). Seeing a bare moving cork, a conspecific male or a prey item also elicits escalation, but not to the level elicited by seeing a conspecific female. Here the earlier study is extended by using non-conspecific females in control tests and by undertaking experiments with an additional six salticid species. Findings from these experiments suggest that the ability of interacting males to detect by sight alone that a conspecific, instead of a non-conspecific, female is present is widespread in the family Salticidae and that it may be routine for salticid males to use information about the presence of conspecific females when making strategic decisions related to escalation during encounters with other males.  相似文献   

2.
3.
An important prediction from game theory is that the value of a resource influences the level to which conflict escalates. Here we use jumping spiders (Salticidae) to consider this prediction in the context of species adopting different mating systems (‘female mate-choice’ and ‘mutual mate-choice’). Our experiments are designed for determining whether the odour of conspecific females, more than the odour of heterospecific females, primes males to escalate conflict with a potential same-sex rival and also whether the odour of conspecific males, more than the odour of heterospecific males, primes females to escalate conflict with a potential same-sex rival. Four species were studied: Evarcha culicivora, a species in which mutual mate-choice is pronounced, and Portia fimbriata, Portia africana, and Jacksonoides queenslandicus, more conventional salticids in which female mate-choice and male–male competition appear to be dominant. Our hypothesis is that, for all four species, there is strong competition between males for access to females and that, for E. culicivora, but not for the other three species, there is also strong competition between females for access to males. Our findings are consistent with this hypothesis, as we show that, although the odour of conspecific females primes escalation of vision-based male–male conflict for all four species, E. culicivora is the only species for which there is evidence of odour from conspecific males priming the escalation of female–female conflict.  相似文献   

4.
Psilochorus sphaeroides from Queensland, Australia and Smeringopus pallidus from Sri Lanka are long-legged, web-building pholcid spiders with a special defence behaviour, whirling. The efficiency of whirling as a defence against web-invading jumping spiders (Salticidae) was examined in the laboratory. Three salticid species were used in these tests- Euryattus sp., Portia fimbriata and Portia labiata. Euryattus leapt into webs, but Portia fimbriata and Portia labiata walked slowly into webs and practised aggressive mimicry. Portia fimbriata was more consistent at using aggressive mimicry and more efficient at capturing Psilochorus sphaeroides and Smeringopus pallidus than was Portia labiata . Both species of Portia were more efficient at catching pholcids than was Euryattus. Portia , especially Portia fimbriata , was less inclined than Euryattus to stimulate pholcids to whirl. In an experiment in which pholcids were artificially induced to whirl whenever a salticid was near, salticids never captured pholcids, providing additional evidence that whirling is an effective defence of Psilochorus sphaeroides and Smeringopus pallidus against web-invaders. Results from this study are compared to those from a study of another pholcid that whirls, Pholcus phalangioides .  相似文献   

5.
6.
We tested the effect of rearing conditions on the behaviour of jumping spiders, Phidippus audax. Spiders were assigned randomly to either small or large cages that either were empty or contained a painted dowel. Laboratory-reared spiders were raised from second instar to adult in these environments. Field-caught adults also were randomly assigned to these containers and were held for approximately 4 months prior to testing. We presented spiders with three tests designed to examine a range of behaviours. Field-caught spiders were more likely than laboratory-reared spiders to (1) react to videotaped prey, (2) progress further on a detour test, and (3) be less stereotactic and more active in an open field. Larger cage size and the presence of the dowel also improved performance in several tests. Our results suggest that the rearing conditions we used, which are commonly employed by behavioural researchers, may profoundly influence the behaviour of adult spiders. Copyright 2000 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour.  相似文献   

7.
Fine structure of the eyes of jumping spiders   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
  相似文献   

8.
9.
Studies of aposematism are mostly focused on visual aspects of warning signalization and based on experiments with avian predators. This study presents results of experiments with an arthropod predator, a jumping spider Evarcha arcuata (Araneae: Salticidae) and olfactory (i.e., noncontact chemical) aposematic signals. Spiders were presented with chemically protected firebug Pyrrhocoris apterus (Heteroptera: Pyrrhocoridae). After acquiring the negative experience with the bugs, the spiders were offered a choice between the firebug olfactory signal and a scentless control. Spiders actively avoided the firebug signal, showing that they are able to recognize an unprofitable prey by means of olfactory chemoreception. The results show that olfactory signals alone may function as effective means of aposematic signalization to spiders.  相似文献   

10.
11.
Myrmarachne lupata is an ant-like salticid in which males have very large chelicerae. The display repertoire of this species is unusually large and complex for a salticid spider. Each individual male uses one of three different mating tactics depending on the female's maturity and location. With adult females outside nests type 1 courtship occurs which seems to be a form of visual communication and includes specialized movements and postures of the legs, palps and body. With adult females inside nests, males use type 2 courtship, which seems to be a form of non-visual communication and consists primarily of probing with the legs on the silk; males mate with receptive females inside the nests. With subadult females, males first use type 2 courtship then spin an adjacent silken chamber and cohabit. After she moults and matures, mating occurs inside the nest. Vacant nests of conspecific females, but not those of another sympatric salticid species, elicit courtship behaviour from males. During male-male interactions, embracing occurs with the large chelicerae spread apart. Females and subadults also display, and different displays occur in interactions depending on the sex/age classes of the spiders involved. Despite the unusual morphology of these spiders, their individual displays are similar to those of more typical salticids. During copulation males stand beside the female instead of over or on her as occurs with typical salticids.  相似文献   

12.
13.
Animals emit visual signals that involve simultaneous, sequential movements of appendages that unfold with varying dynamics in time and space. Algorithms have been recently reported (e.g. Peters et al. in Anim Behav 64:131–146, 2002) that enable quantitative characterization of movements as optical flow patterns. For decades, acoustical signals have been rendered by techniques that decompose sound into amplitude, time, and spectral components. Using an optic-flow algorithm we examined visual courtship behaviours of jumping spiders and depict their complex visual signals as “speed waveform”, “speed surface”, and “speed waterfall” plots analogous to acoustic waveforms, spectrograms, and waterfall plots, respectively. In addition, these “speed profiles” are compatible with analytical techniques developed for auditory analysis. Using examples from the jumping spider Habronattus pugillis we show that we can statistically differentiate displays of different “sky island” populations supporting previous work on diversification. We also examined visual displays from the jumping spider Habronattus dossenus and show that distinct seismic components of vibratory displays are produced concurrently with statistically distinct motion signals. Given that dynamic visual signals are common, from insects to birds to mammals, we propose that optical-flow algorithms and the analyses described here will be useful for many researchers.Damian O. Elias and Bruce R. Land contributed equallyAn erratum to this article can be found at  相似文献   

14.
As a case study of how insects use masks as a defence against vision-guided predators, an experimental study was carried out using Acanthaspis petax , a reduviid bug ('ant bug') that covers itself with a 'mask', or 'backpack', made from carcasses of its preferred prey (ants), and three salticid spider species, Hyllus sp., Plexippus sp. and Thyene sp., salticids being predators with exceptionally acute vision. The ant bugs and the salticids were from the Lake Victoria region of East Africa. In each test, a salticid was presented with a live bug or a lure made from a dead bug, with the mask removed ('naked') or intact ('masked'). Salticids made predatory responses to naked bugs significantly more often than to masked bugs. These findings suggest that salticids readily identify naked bugs as prey, but fail to identify masked bugs as prey.  相似文献   

15.
Spectral sensitivities of cells in principal eyes of the jumping spider Phidippus reqius were measured using techniques of intracellular recording. Three types of cells were found. UV cells had peak sensitivities at 370 nm and were over 4 log units less sensitive at wavelengths longer than 460 nm. Green-sensitive cells had spectral sensitivities which were well fit by nomogram curves peaking at 532 nm. UV-green cells had dual peaks of sensitivity at about 370 and 525 nm, but the ratios of UV-to-green sensitivities varied over a 40: 1 range from cell to cell. Moreover, responses of UV-green cells to flashes of UV light were slower than to flashes of green light. Segregation of receptor types into the known layers of receptors in these eyes could not be shown. It is concluded that jumping spiders have the potential for dichromatic color vision.  相似文献   

16.
A range of web-invading jumping spiders with different predatory strategies was tested with A. appensa in the laboratory: Mimetus maculosus (Mimetidae), Pholcus phalangioides (Pholcidae), Taieria erebus (Gnaphosidae), and 11 species of salticids. Spiders that are known to specialize at web-invading, either by leaping into webs or by walking slowly into webs and practising aggressive mimicry, captured A. appensa ; three salticid species not known to be web-invaders never did. Web-invaders that practised aggressive mimicry were more efficient than were species that only leapt into webs. Portia fimbriata from Queensland was the most consistent at using aggressive mimicry and was also the most efficient at catching A. appensa . Web-invaders that were more efficient at catching A. appensa were also better able to avoid setting off pumping, a special defence behaviour used by A. appensa . Portia fimbriata from Queensland was especially efficient at avoiding setting off pumping: P. fimbriata more consistently than other Portia made its final approach toward A. appensa by coming down from above the web on a dragline and making minimal contact with the web. An experiment, in which A. appensa was artificially induced to pump whenever the predator was near, provided additional evidence that pumping is effective in defending A. appensa against web-invaders.  相似文献   

17.
Three species of web-invading salticid spiders, with different predatory strategies, were tested with Holocnemus pluchei in the laboratory: Portia fimbriata and Portia labiata , which practise aggressive mimicry, and Euyattus sp., which leaps from outside on to spiders in webs but does not practise aggressive mimicry. Portia was shown to be more efficient than Euryattus at catching H. pluchei. Portia fimbriata from Queensland was more consistent at using aggressive mimicry than was Portia labiata and was also more efficient at catching H. pluchei . The web-invaders that were more efficient at catching H. pluchei were also better able to avoid setting off bouncing, a special defence behaviour used by H. pluchei . An experiment, in which H. pluchei was artificially induced to bounce whenever the predator was near, provided additional evidence that bouncing is effective in defending H. pluchei against web-invaders.  相似文献   

18.
Portia is a genus of specialized web-invading salticids that use aggressive mimicry. Some other salticids leap into webs to catch spiders but do not use aggressive mimicry. Pholcus phalangioides is a web-building spider with a special defensive behaviour—called whirling—in which it swings its body around in a circle while keeping its long legs on the silk. Pholcus phalangioides is preyed on by Portia and probably other salticid spiders in nature. Interactions between P. phalangioides and 13 species of salticids were studied in the laboratory to compare how effective salticids with different styles of predation were at catching the pholcids. Four species of Portia were studied and each was more efficient at catching P. phalangioides than were the other nine salticids tested. For one species—Portia fimbriata—individuals from three different populations were studied. The Queensland P. fimbriata used aggressive mimicry more consistently and were more efficient at catching P. phalangioides than were the other species of Portia and the other populations of P. fimbriata . The salticids that were the most efficient at catching pholcids were also better able to avoid setting off whirling by the pholcids. An experiment in which pholcids were artificially induced to whirl whenever the predator was near provided additional evidence that whirling is an effective defence of pholcids against predation by salticids.  相似文献   

19.
Spectral sensitivity in jumping spiders (Araneae,Salticidae)   总被引:2,自引:2,他引:0  
1. We report here a psychophysical technique for studying the spectral sensitivity of jumping spiders (family Salticidae), based on a newly discovered oculomotor reflex. 2. Our results, obtained from Maevia inclemens (Salticidae), are compatible with electrophysiological findings of retinal cells maximally sensitive in the green and ultraviolet regions of the spectrum. 3. Sensitivity to longer wavelengths (greater than 650 nm) has been controversial. In our study jumping spiders are shown to have a broad spectral sensitivity function extending from the ultraviolet (330 nm) to the deep red (700 nm).  相似文献   

20.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号