首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Retinal explants of mouse embryos were cultured together with explants of different regions in the retinofugal pathway in order to investigate whether ventral temporal (VT) and dorsal nasal (DN) retinal neurites showed differential responses to regional-specific cues in the pathway. In the presence of the chiasm, biased outgrowth of retinal neurites was found in explants of both retinal regions, which was accompanied by a reduction in total neurite growth in the VT but not the DN retina. Such differential responses to the diffusible negative influence were also observed when explants of two retinal origins were cocultured with the ventral diencephalon, but were not found with the dorsal diencephalon that contains targets of the optic axons. Indeed, extensive neurite invasion was found in the dorsal diencephalic explants and this ingrowth was more prominent for VT than DN neurites, showing a difference in axons from a distinct position in the retina to contact-mediated stimulatory activity within the target nuclei. We conclude that neurites from different regions of the retina show differential responses to the regional-specific cues in the diencephalon. These cues exist in both diffusible and contact-mediated forms that may shape the characteristic course and organization of retinal axons in decision regions of the optic pathway and the visual targets.  相似文献   

2.
Konkle T  Oliva A 《Neuron》2012,74(6):1114-1124
While there are selective regions of occipitotemporal cortex that respond to faces, letters, and bodies, the large-scale neural organization of most object categories remains unknown. Here, we find that object representations can be differentiated along the ventral temporal cortex by their real-world size. In a functional neuroimaging experiment, observers were shown pictures of big and small real-world objects (e.g., table, bathtub; paperclip, cup), presented at the same retinal size. We observed a consistent medial-to-lateral organization of big and small object preferences in the ventral temporal cortex, mirrored along the lateral surface. Regions in the lateral-occipital, inferotemporal, and parahippocampal cortices showed strong peaks of differential real-world size selectivity and maintained these preferences over changes in retinal size and in mental imagery. These data demonstrate that the real-world size of objects can provide insight into the spatial topography of object representation.  相似文献   

3.
Anatomical mapping was made of the retinal central pathways from the chiasm to the targets within the tectum in the developing Xenopus tadpoles, after labeling a specific regional population of retinal axons with horseradish peroxidase (HRP). In the tadpoles at stage 50, pathway sorting of retinal axons within the optic tract was clear for the dorsoventral axis of the retina, but not for the nasotemporal axis. Most nasal retinal axons and some dorsal and ventral retinal axons invaded the tectum directly at the diencephalotectal junction, and arrived at their correct sites of innervation after running through ectopic parts of the tectum. These findings indicate that the pathway orientation before targets is not a prerequisite factor for establishment of the orderly map of the retinotectal projection. Rather, a direct interaction between ingrowing retinal axons and tectal cells seems to be a predominant factor for specification of retinal central connections.  相似文献   

4.
BACKGROUND: In anorthoscopic viewing conditions, observers can perceive a moving object through a narrow slit even when only portions of its contour are visible at any time. We used fMRI to examine the contribution of early and later visual cortical areas to dynamic shape integration. Observers' success at integrating the shape of the slit-viewed object was manipulated by varying the degree to which the stimulus was dynamically distorted. Line drawings of common objects were either moderately distorted, strongly distorted, or shown undistorted. Phenomenologically, increasing the stimulus distortion made both object shape and motion more difficult to perceive.RESULTS: We found that bilateral cortical activity in portions of the ventral occipital cortex, corresponding to known object areas within the lateral occipital complex (LOC), was inversely correlated with the degree of stimulus distortion. We found that activity in left MT+, the human cortical area specialized for motion, showed a similar pattern as the ventral occipital region. The LOC also showed greater activity to a fully visible moving object than to the undistorted slit-viewed object. Area MT+, however, showed more equivalent activity to both the slit-viewed and fully visible moving objects.CONCLUSIONS: In early retinotopic cortex, the distorted and undistorted stimuli elicited the same amount of activity. Higher visual areas, however, were correlated with the percept of the coherent object, and this correlation suggests that the shape integration is mediated by later visual cortical areas. Motion information from the dorsal stream may project to the LOC to produce the shape percept.  相似文献   

5.
To make deliberate decisions, we have to utilize detailed information about the environment and our internal states. The ventral visual pathway provides detailed information on object identity, including color and shape, to the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC). The VLPFC also receives motivational and emotional information from the orbitofrontal cortex and subcortical areas, and computes the behavioral significance of external events; this information can be used for elaborate decision making or design of goal-directed behavior. In this review, we discuss recent advances that are revealing the neural mechanisms that underlie the coding of behavioral significance in the VLPFC, and the functional roles of these mechanisms in decision making and action programming in the brain.  相似文献   

6.
Humans can recognize an object within a fraction of a second, even if there are no clues about what kind of object it might be. Recent findings have identified functional properties of extrastriate regions in the ventral visual pathway that are involved in the representation and perception of objects and faces. The functional properties of these regions, and the correlation between the activation of these regions and visual recognition, indicate that the lateral and ventral occipito-temporal areas are important in perceiving and recognizing objects and faces.  相似文献   

7.
An assay is described that can measure the rates of adhesion of isotopically labeled cell bodies from either the dorsal or ventral regions of the chick embryo retina to dorsal and ventral tectal halves. Immediately after dissociation of the retina, both ventral and dorsal retinal cells adhere preferentially to ventral tectal halves. With increasing time after dissociation, however, the preference of the ventral retinal cells shifts to dorsal tectal halves. The dorsal retinal cells continue to show a specificity for ventral tectal halves regardless of the length of time after their dissociation.When the developmental age of the tectal halves is varied from 8–14 days, there is no change in the specificities shown by retinal cells toward these tecta.When retinal age is varied, ventral retinal cells do not adhere preferentially to dorsal tecta until day 6 and older. Dorsal retinas show their specificity toward ventral tecta as early as day three.Control experiments include the use of nonretinal tissues, noninnervated tectal halves and pigmented retinal cells.  相似文献   

8.
Spiridon M  Kanwisher N 《Neuron》2002,35(6):1157-1165
We used fMRI to study the distribution of object category information in the ventral visual pathway. Extending the findings of, we find that categories of stimuli can be distinguished by the pattern of activation they elicit across this entire pathway, even when the stimuli within a category differ in viewpoint, exemplar, or image format. However, regions within the ventral visual pathway are neither interchangeable nor equipotential. Although the FFA and PPA permit excellent discrimination between preferred versus nonpreferred stimuli (e.g., faces-bottles and houses-bottles, respectively), we find that neither region alone permits accurate discrimination between pairs of nonpreferred stimuli (e.g., bottles-shoes). These findings indicate that the ventral visual pathway is not homogeneous, but contains some regions (including FFA and PPA) that are primarily involved in the analysis of a single class of stimulus.  相似文献   

9.
The dorsal and ventral cortical pathways, driven predominantly by magnocellular (M) and parvocellular (P) inputs, respectively, assume leading roles in models of visual information processing. Although in prior proposals, the dorsal and ventral pathways support non-conscious and conscious vision, respectively, recent modelling and empirical developments indicate that each pathway plays important roles in both non-conscious and conscious vision. In these models, the ventral P-pathway consists of one subpathway processing an object''s contour features, e.g. curvature, the other processing its surface attributes, e.g. colour. Masked priming studies have shown that feed-forward activity in the ventral P-pathway on its own supports non-conscious processing of contour and surface features. The dorsal M-pathway activity contributes directly to conscious vision of motion and indirectly to object vision by projecting to prefrontal cortex, which in turn injects top-down neural activity into the ventral P-pathway and there ‘ignites’ feed-forward–re-entrant loops deemed necessary for conscious vision. Moreover, an object''s shape or contour remains invisible without the prior conscious registration of its surface properties, which for that reason are taken to comprise fundamental visual qualia. Besides suggesting avenues for future research, these developments bear on several recent and past philosophical issues.  相似文献   

10.
Shmuelof L  Zohary E 《Neuron》2005,47(3):457-470
Neuropsychological case studies suggest the existence of two functionally separate visual streams: the ventral pathway, central for object recognition; and the dorsal pathway, engaged in visually guided actions. However, a clear dissociation between the functions of the two streams has not been decisively shown in intact humans. In this study, we demonstrate dissociation between dorsal and ventral fMRI activation patterns during observation of object manipulation video clips. Parietal areas, such as anterior intraparietal sulcus (aIPS) display grasp viewing-dependent adaptation (i.e., fMR adaptation during repeated viewing of the same object-grasping movement) as well as a contralateral preference for the viewed manipulating hand. Ventral regions, such as the fusiform gyrus, show similar characteristics (i.e., adaptation, contralateral preference), but these depend on object identity. Our results support the hypothesized functional specialization in the visual system and suggest that parietal areas (such as aIPS) are engaged in action recognition, as well as in action planning.  相似文献   

11.
Growing evidence indicates a moderate but significant relationship between processing speed in visuo-cognitive tasks and general intelligence. On the other hand, findings from neuroscience proposed that the primate visual system consists of two major pathways, the ventral pathway for objects recognition and the dorsal pathway for spatial processing and attentive analysis. Previous studies seeking for visuo-cognitive factors of human intelligence indicated a significant correlation between fluid intelligence and the inspection time (IT), an index for a speed of object recognition performed in the ventral pathway. We thus presently examined a possibility that neural processing speed in the dorsal pathway also represented a factor of intelligence. Specifically, we used the mental rotation (MR) task, a popular psychometric measure for mental speed of spatial processing in the dorsal pathway. We found that the speed of MR was significantly correlated with intelligence scores, while it had no correlation with one’s IT (recognition speed of visual objects). Our results support the new possibility that intelligence could be explained by two types of mental speed, one related to object recognition (IT) and another for manipulation of mental images (MR).  相似文献   

12.
Richards (1985) showed that veridical three-dimensional shape may be recovered from the integration of binocular disparity and retinal motion information, but proposed that this integration may only occur for horizontal retinal motion. Psychophysical evidence supporting the combination of stereo and motion information is limited to the case of horizontal motion (Johnston et al., 1994), and has been criticised on the grounds of potential object boundary cues to shape present in the stimuli. We investigated whether veridical shape can be recovered under more general conditions. Observers viewed cylinders that were defined by binocular disparity, two-frame motion or a combination of disparity and motion, presented at simulated distances of 30 cm, 90 cm or 150 cm. Horizontally and vertically oriented cylinders were rotated about vertical and horizontal axes. When rotation was about the cylinder's own axis, no boundary cues to shape were introduced. Settings were biased for the disparity and two-frame motion stimuli, while more veridical shape judgements were made under all conditions for combined cue stimuli. These results demonstrate that the improved perception of three-dimensional shape in these stimuli is not a consequence of the presence of object boundary cues, and that the combination of disparity and motion is not restricted to horizontal image motion.  相似文献   

13.
Neural crest cells migrate along defined pathways in the trunk of avian embryos. Previous studies have demonstrated that crest-derived pigment cells migrated ventrally after injection onto the ventral neural crest pathway (M. E. Bronner-Fraser and A. M. Cohen, 1980, Develop. Biol.77, 130–141). In the present study, latex polystyrene beads and retinal pigment epithelial (RPE) cells were injected onto the ventral pathway in order to probe the environment along this migratory route. Although the RPE cells are nonmotile and not derived from the neural crest, they also translocated ventrally. Thus, factors independent of active migration may affect the localization of RPE cells (and endogenous crest cells). To test this possibility, latex polystyrene beads were injected onto the ventral pathway. Three types of beads were used: (a) uncoated latex beads; (b) latex beads coated with bovine serum albumin (BSA-beads); and (c) latex beads coated with fibronectin (FN-beads). Uncoated and BSA-beads distributed along the ventral pathway similarly to RPE cells and endogenous crest cells. However, FN-beads remained near the site of implantation and did not move ventrally. The results suggest (1) that molecules like fibronectin on the cell surface might serve as a recognition mechanism that prevents entrance onto the ventral pathway; and (2) that crest cell localization may, in part, be influenced by a driving force imparted by the embryonic environment.  相似文献   

14.
By means of calculation and instrumental modeling it has been shown that a change of the form and slurring of the object moving in a slit of an arbitrary shape results from a distortion of the retinal projection appearing due to decelerated following.  相似文献   

15.
Summary The projections of horseradish peroxidase-filled axons from each quadrant of the retina were studied to determine whether retinal projections of goldfish are topographically organized in diencephalic target nuclei. A distinct topography of the dorsal, nasal, ventral and temporal retina exists in the lateral geniculate nucleus and in the dorsolateral optic nucleus of the thalamus. The projections of retinal quadrants show minimal spatial overlap in each of these nuclei. The suprachiasmatic nucleus of the hypothalamus is extensively innervated by ventral retinal fibers, whereas the nucleus is sparsely innervated by fibers from the other three retinal quadrants. A rudimentary topography also exists in the pretectum where the dorsal pretectal area receives projections primarily from the ventral retina and the ventral pretectal area receives projections mostly from the dorsal retina. These data show that retinal projections to some diencephalic nuclei are topographically organized.This work was supported by Research Grant EY-01426 to S.C.S.  相似文献   

16.
In a new mouse mutant, circletail (Crc), failure of neural tube closure (embryonic day [E] 8-9) is associated with errors in retinal axon projection at the optic chiasm (E12-18), such that many axons normally projecting contralaterally instead grow to ipsilateral targets. Although the architecture of the chiasmatic region is altered, neurons and glia containing putative cues for axon guidance are present. The aberrant ipsilateral-projecting cells originate from a nonrandom expansion of the wild-type uncrossed retinal region. These axon pathway defects are found in two other mutants with cephalic neural tube defects (NTD), loop-tail (Lp) and Pax3 (splotch; Sp(2H)). Crc is phenotypically similar to Lp, exhibiting an open neural tube from midbrain to tail (craniorachischisis), while splotch has spina bifida with or without a cranial NTD. The retinal axon abnormalities occur only in the presence of NTD and not in homozygous mutants lacking cranial NTD. Thus, failure of neural tube closure is associated with failure of many retinal axons to cross the ventral midline. This study therefore reveals an unexpected connection between closure of the neural tube at the dorsal midline and development of ventral axon tracts. genesis 27:32-47, 2000.  相似文献   

17.
18.
Relative binocular disparity cannot tell us the absolute 3D shape of an object, nor the 3D trajectory of its motion, unless the visual system has independent access to how far away the object is at any moment. Indeed, as the viewing distance is changed, the same disparate retinal motions will correspond to very different real 3D trajectories. In this paper we were interested in whether binocular 3D motion detection is affected by viewing distance. A visual search task was used, in which the observer is asked to detect a target dot, moving in 3D, amidst 3D stationary distractor dots. We found that distance does not affect detection performance. Motion-in-depth is consistently harder to detect than the equivalent lateral motion, for all viewing distances. For a constant retinal motion with both lateral and motion-in-depth components, detection performance is constant despite variations in viewing distance that produce large changes in the direction of the 3D trajectory. We conclude that binocular 3D motion detection relies on retinal, not absolute, visual signals.  相似文献   

19.
Bone morphogenetic proteins (BMP) direct dorsal–ventral patterning in both invertebrate and vertebrate embryos, with strong evolutionary conservation of molecular components of the pathway. Dorsal–ventral patterning of the early Drosophila embryo is a powerful experimental system to probe mechanisms of BMP gradient formation and interpretation. Recent studies have found that spatial patterns of activated BMP signal transducers in Drosophila go through an unexpected transition: a shallow gradient of weak responses at mid-cellularization changes to a step gradient of stronger responses in cellularized embryos. The transition between two gradients of different shape yields new insights into the progression of Drosophila dorsal–ventral patterning and raises new issues about the mechanisms of gradient formation.  相似文献   

20.
Transplantation of neural stem cells for replacing neurons after neurodegeneration requires that the transplanted stem cells accurately reestablish the lost neural circuits in order to restore function. Retinal ganglion cell axons project to visual centers of the brain forming circuits in precise topographic order. In chick, dorsal retinal neurons project to ventral optic tectum, ventral neurons to dorsal tectum, anterior neurons to posterior tectum and posterior neurons to anterior tectum; forming a continuous point-to-point map of retinal cell position in the tectal projection. We found that when stem cells derived from ventral retina were implanted in dorsal host retina, the stem cells that became ganglion cells projected to dorsal tectum, appropriate for their site of origin in retina but not appropriate for their site of implant in retina. This led us to ask if retinal progenitors exhibit topographic markers of cell position in retina. Indeed, retinal neural progenitors express topographic markers: dorsal stem cells expressed more Ephrin B2 than ventral stem cells and, conversely, ventral stem cells expressed more Pax-2 and Ventroptin than dorsal stem cells. The fact that neural progenitors express topographic markers has pertinent implications in using neural stem cells in cell replacement therapy for replacing projecting neurons that express topographic order, e.g., analogous neurons of the visual, auditory, somatosensory and motor systems.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号