共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Lisa J. Faust Doug Cress Kay H. Farmer Stephen R. Ross Benjamin B. Beck 《International journal of primatology》2011,32(4):849-864
Wildlife sanctuaries rescue, rehabilitate, reintroduce, and provide life-long care for orphaned and injured animals. Understanding
a sanctuary’s patterns in arrival, mortality, and projected changes in population size can help managers plan carefully for
future needs, as well as illuminate patterns in source populations of wildlife. We studied these dynamics for chimpanzees
(Pan troglodytes) in 11 sanctuaries of the Pan African Sanctuary Alliance (PASA). We analyzed historic demographic patterns and projected
future population dynamics using an individual-based demographic model. From 2000 to 2006, the population in these sanctuaries
has been growing at a rate of 15% per year. This growth is driven by arrivals of new individuals, with an average of 56 arrivals
per year. The median age of the 760 chimpanzees living in these sanctuaries as of 2007 was 9 yr, with 76% of the population
<15 yr. We found no significant difference in survivorship to age 20 between these chimpanzees and those maintained in North
American accredited zoos. The size of the population in 20 yr is projected to be between 550 and 1800, depending on different
assumptions about arrival and reintroduction rates. Projected shifts in age structure, including increases in the proportions
of adolescent (9–19 yr of age) and older (35+) chimpanzees, may necessitate adjustments to management, veterinary care, and
housing. This research illustrates how data on historic population dynamics can be modeled to inform future sanctuary capacity
and management needs, allowing sanctuaries to plan better for their populations’ long-term care. 相似文献
2.
Coordination of Chimpanzees (<Emphasis Type="Italic">Pan troglodytes</Emphasis>) in a Stag Hunt Game
Anke F. Bullinger Emily Wyman Alicia P. Melis Michael Tomasello 《International journal of primatology》2011,32(6):1296-1310
Group-living animals frequently face situations in which they must coordinate individual and sometimes conflicting goals.
We assessed chimpanzees’ ability to coordinate in a Stag Hunt game. Dyads were confronted with a situation in which each individual
was already foraging on a low-value food (hare) when a high-value food (stag) appeared that required collaboration for retrieval,
with a solo attempt to get the stag resulting in a loss of both options. In one condition visibility between partners was
open whereas in the other it was blocked by a barrier. Regardless of condition, dyads almost always (91%) coordinated to choose
the higher valued collaborative option. Intentional communication or monitoring of the partner’s behavior before decision
making—characteristic of much human coordination—were limited. Instead, all dyads adopted a leader–follower strategy in which
one partner took the risk of going first, presumably predicting that this would induce the other to join in (sometimes communicating
if she was slow to do so). These results show that humans’ closest primate relatives do not use complex communication to coordinate
but most often use a less cognitively complex strategy that achieves the same end. 相似文献
3.
We assessed the health status of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the Kanyawara group in the Kibale National Park in Western Uganda via noninvasive methods. We conducted visual veterinary inspection, parasitological and urine analysis in association with behavioral observations, causing minimal disturbance or stress to individually recognized chimpanzees. We applied multiple parasitological techniques to 252 stool samples to compare their efficacy in detecting parasitic infection and to increase the power of detecting a wide range of parasites at a more sensitive level. We examined 76 urine samples via a quick detection method to evaluate multiple parameters of urine that indicate organ dysfunction. Results of the different analyses are mutually supportive and provided useful information for monitoring bodily condition and diseases. The multifaceted health evaluation system is a beneficial tool for monitoring long-term and short-term changes in health status due to environmental stress, seasonal dietary change, and disease in wild chimpanzee populations. Use of this method to detect changes in health, when employed together with behavioral observations, may also provide important insights into the potential effects of self-medicative behaviors. 相似文献
4.
Conflict management strategies can reduce costs of aggressive competition in group-living animals. Postconflict behaviors such as reconciliation and third-party postconflict affiliation are widely accepted as social skills in primates and have been demonstrated in many species. Although immature primates possess a repertoire of species-specific behaviors, it is thought that they gradually develop appropriate social skills throughout prolonged juvenility to establish and maintain complex social relationships within their group. We examined the occurrence of postconflict skills in five immature chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) over 15 mo, focusing on interactions that were not with the subject’s mother. We observed reconciliation, with conciliatory tendencies comparable to adults, and provide the first evidence that captive immature chimpanzees commonly reconciled using social play. However, immatures were not more likely to reconcile valuable than nonvaluable relationships. We also observed third party postconflict affiliation although at a lower level than reported for adults. Our results provide evidence for postconflict skills in immature chimpanzees but the lack of higher conciliatory tendency with valuable partners and low occurrence of third-party affiliation indicates extended juvenility may be required refine these skills. Further work is needed to investigate whether these behaviors have the same function and effectiveness as those found in adults. 相似文献
5.
Monica L. Wakefield 《International journal of primatology》2008,29(4):907-929
In mammals, access to mates is probably the most important influence on male reproductive success, whereas foraging efficiency is probably the most important influence on female reproductive success Emlen and Oring (Science 197:215–223, 1977). Male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are highly gregarious and form cooperative relationships with other males. In contrast, female social relationships vary within and between populations. Females in most East African populations, e.g., Gombe, Mahale, Kibale-Kanyawara, are less gregarious than males and spend most of their time alone or with only their dependent offspring. Researchers have attributed low female gregariousness to the high potential for feeding competition. I provide the first data on association patterns and agonistic interactions of female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) from the unusually large Ngogo community, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Ngogo females were less gregarious than males, but spent a mean of 64% of their time in association with ≥1 other females and as much time in all-female parties as they did alone. Further, female dyads associated nonrandomly and they formed associative cliques. Association levels within cliques were similar to those among the relatively gregarious West African chimpanzee females at Taï (Pan troglodytes verus) and among bonobo (P. paniscus) females. Agonistic interfemale interactions were extremely rare, and monthly mean party size and the numbers of anestrous females per party do not correlate significantly with fruit availability. Thus, Ngogo females maintained relatively high levels of gregariousness, but avoided detrimental feeding competition by preferentially associating with a small subset of other community females. 相似文献
6.
Maureen S. McCarthy Jack D. Lester Craig B. Stanford 《International journal of primatology》2017,38(2):321-337
As habitat loss and fragmentation place growing pressure on endangered nonhuman primate populations, researchers find increasing evidence for novel responses in behavior. In western Uganda between the Budongo and Bugoma Forests, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) inhabit a mosaic landscape comprising forest fragments, human settlements, and agricultural land. We recorded nests and feeding evidence of unhabituated chimpanzees in this region over a 12-mo period. We found extensive evidence of nesting in introduced tree species, including eucalyptus (Eucalyptus grandis), guava (Psidium guajava), cocoa (Theobroma cacao), and Caribbean pine (Pinus caribaea). In addition, we found instances of ground nesting, nest reuse, and composite nests constructed from branches of multiple trees. This evidence may indicate a lack of suitable nesting trees or attempts by chimpanzees to nest in areas of riparian forest that allow them to avoid human detection. We also found new evidence for eucalyptus bark feeding by chimpanzees. Such evidence suggests chimpanzees respond flexibly to mitigate anthropogenic pressures in human-dominated landscapes. The limits of such flexibility remain unknown. Further research is needed to examine systematically the factors influencing the use of such resources and to understand better the extent to which chimpanzees can persist while relying on them. 相似文献
7.
We presented two chimpanzees with a task in which they were required to pull each end of a rope simultaneously to drag blocks supporting food into reach. The chimpanzees did not succeed in initial tests. They did not immediately understand the necessity for cooperation, and they did not adjust their behavior to work with the partner. However, the frequency of success gradually increased as the number of sessions increased and the task was varied. They began to look at the partner frequently, wait if the partner was not holding the rope, and pull the rope in synchrony with the partner. However, they did not use interactive behaviors or eye contact to synchronize their behavior. One chimpanzee was then paired with a human partner in the same situation. After initial failures, the chimpanzee began to solicit the human partner for cooperation: looking up at his face, vocalizing, and taking the partner’s hand. When this chimpanzee was again paired with the chimpanzee partner, no soliciting behavior was observed. Thus, the chimpanzees could learn to coordinate their behavior through trial and error. Communicative behavior emerged during the task, but the communication differed according to the identity of the partner. 相似文献
8.
Patr-AL is a recently described gene found only in the common chimpanzee, but closely related in structure to the highly polymorphic Patr-A and HLA-A genes of the chimpanzee and human MHCs, respectively. Unlike Patr-A and HLA-A, the Patr-AL gene has little polymorphism and is not fixed in the chimpanzee genome. To determine whether Patr-AL is located in the MHC or elsewhere, we compared segregation of the Patr-AL gene with segregation of Patr-A and - B alleles in chimpanzee families. The results demonstrate that Patr-AL is an MHC class I gene present on different MHC haplotypes as defined by their combination of Patr-A and B alleles. 相似文献
9.
Call J 《Primates; journal of primatology》2003,44(4):347-357
This study investigated the ability of three chimpanzees and three orangutans to track the position of a reward after a series of displacements. The reward was placed under one of two opaque containers resting on a platform. Experiment 1 investigated rotational displacements in which the platform was rotated 0°, 180°, or 360°. Experiment 2 investigated transpositional displacements in which the platform remained stationary while the containers either remained stationary, or swapped their positions (in a one- two- or three-step sequence). In both experiments, the initial position of the reward was indicated either by directly showing the reward under the containers, or by placing a landmark, which had been previously associated with the reward, on top of the baited container. Subjects successfully tracked the reward through rotations and transpositions when they had seen it, but their performance substantially deteriorated when the landmark indicated the reward's initial position, even though subjects successfully used the landmark to find the reward in the absence of displacements. This decrease was especially pronounced in rotational displacements. A language-trained orangutan outperformed all the other apes and solved all problems. 相似文献
10.
Primate habitats are being transformed by human activities such as agriculture. Many wild primates include cultivated foods (crops) in their diets, calling for an improved understanding of the costs and benefits of crop feeding. We measured the macronutrient and antifeedant content of 44 wild and 21 crop foods eaten by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in a mosaic habitat at Bulindi, Uganda, to evaluate the common assertion that crops offer high nutritional returns compared to wild forage for primates. In addition, we analyzed 13 crops not eaten at Bulindi but that are consumed by chimpanzees elsewhere to assess whether nutritional aspects explain why chimpanzees in Bulindi ignored them. Our analysis of their wild plant diet (fruit, leaves, and pith) corresponds with previous chemical analyses of primate plant foods. Compared to wild food equivalents, crops eaten by the chimpanzees contained higher levels of digestible carbohydrates (mainly sugars) coupled with lower amounts of insoluble fiber and antifeedants. Cultivated fruits were relatively nutritious throughout the ripening process. Our data support the assumption that eating cultivated foods confers energetic advantages for primates, although crops in our sample were low in protein and lipids compared to some wild foods. We found little evidence that crops ignored by the chimpanzees were less nutritious than those that they did eat. Nonnutritional factors, e.g., similarity to wild foods, probably also influence crop selection. Whether cultivated habitats can support threatened but flexible primates such as chimpanzees in the long term hinges on local people’s willingness to share their landscape and resources with them. 相似文献
11.
Synopsis Feeding ecology was compared between sympatric greenling species, Hexagrammos otakii and H. agrammus in the eelgrass beds in Jindong Bay, Korea, from January to December. These two species had similar diets composed of crustaceans,
polychaetes, gastropods and fishes; both species consumed primarily crustaceans throughout study periods. H. otakii, however, fed a greater proportion of polychaetes and fishes than H. agrammus. H. agrammus had a greater proportion of gastropods in their diets. The diet of both species underwent size-related changes; smaller individuals
of H. otakii and H. agrammus consumed amphipods (gammarid amphipods and caprellid amphipods), while larger individuals of H. otakii ate polychaetes and fishes and those of H. agrammus fed mainly on gastropods and crabs. The diet of H. otakii underwent seasonal changes; H. otakii consumed mainly polychaetes and fishes during January and February 2002 but amphipods during March and May 2002. H. agrammus, however, ate mainly gastropods and crabs all seasons. H. otakii underwent also a significant diel changes that could be related to difference of prey availability. Thus the nocturnal emergence
of gammarid amphipods, polychaetes and fishes explained their greater consumption by H. otakii. Dietary breadth of both species was lower in the smallest individuals (<5 cm SL) and in March and April 2002. This was due
to the disproportionate dry mass attributable to the consumption of amphipods by both H. otakii and H. agrammus. Dietary overlap of both species was relatively moderate to high, in particular in <9.9 cm SL (0.62 – 0.71) from May to July
2002 (0.63 – 0.71). This is coincident with higher abundances of crabs, caridean shrimps and polychaetes in the study area,
and it was assumed that these prey species were not limited resources. Higher dietary overlap was correlated with an abundance
of a shared resource and did not indicate the interspecific competition between H. otakii and H. agrammus. 相似文献
12.
A study on the distribution and population density of the western lowland gorilla (Gorilla g. gorilla) and the central chimpanzee (Pan t. troglodytes) was undertaken between December 1997 and August 2000 in the Campo and Maan Forests in southwestern Cameroon. The aim of this survey was to estimate the densities of the apes in different parts of the area, to assess the importance of the region for the conservation of these endangered species and to determine the influence of human activities such as logging and hunting. The survey was based on night nest counts on a total of 665.5 km of line transects. The overall density in the Campo Forest was estimated at 0.2 gorillas/km2 and at 0.63–0.78 chimpanzees/km2. The overall density of chimpanzees in the Maan Forest was estimated at 0.8–1 individuals/km2. Gorilla density in this area was too low to allow an estimation. The highest gorilla nest density was found in secondary forest. The gorilla density in unlogged forest was significantly lower. Chimpanzees showed a clear preference for less disturbed areas. In unlogged forest, old secondary forests (logging more than 23 years ago) and areas of recent logging with large remaining patches of primary forest, significantly higher densities were calculated than inside the more heavily exploited logging concession. In areas with both logging and high hunting pressure both species were rare or even absent. The Campo Maan area is considered a very important area for the conservation of gorillas and chimpanzees. Conservation measures are urgently required to reduce the impact of logging and hunting. The creation of the Campo Maan National Park in January 2000 is an important measure to preserve the unique biodiversity in this so far hardly protected area. 相似文献
13.
Goossens B Setchell JM Vidal C Dilambaka E Jamart A 《Primates; journal of primatology》2003,44(1):67-69
We report a case of successful reproduction in wild-released orphan chimpanzees. Using non-invasive genetic analysis, we
determined the paternity of an infant born to a female chimpanzee released by HELP (Habitat Ecologique et Liberté des Primates)
Congo into the Conkouati-Douli National Park, Republic of Congo. The sire was a released male, thus demonstrating successful
reproduction in both male and female released chimpanzees. These results provide evidence that release into the wild may be
a viable response to the plight of orphan chimpanzees in Africa, and we discuss further applications of non-invasive genetic
tagging to release programs.
Electronic Publication 相似文献
14.
A Comparison of Female Mating Strategies in <Emphasis Type="Italic">Pan troglodytes</Emphasis> and <Emphasis Type="Italic">Pongo</Emphasis> spp. 总被引:1,自引:1,他引:1
Orangutans and chimpanzees differ in many aspects of their mating and social systems. Nevertheless, because both great apes
require enormous maternal investment in offspring and because female reproductive potential is limited, female orangutans
and chimpanzees should be selective of their mates, yet expected to exhibit anti-infanticide strategies such as mating with
multiple males. We review and compare mating patterns in orangutans and chimpanzees to understand how these critical pressures
are filtered through the different demands of the socioecology of each species. We highlight the variation in female mating
behavior as a function of the proximity of ovulation. We conclude that both genera pursue tactics for paternity confusion
by mating with multiple males and by mating cooperatively or even proceptively with nonpreferred partners when conception
is unlikely. Mating selectivity is suggested by variation in proceptive behavior toward particular partners and by increased
resistance of nonpreferred partners during the periovulatory period. Thus, data for both species support a mixed mating strategy
whereby females shift their mating behavior in accordance with ovulatory status to accommodate the competing demands of mate
selectivity and paternity confusion. 相似文献
15.
Asami Kabasawa Rosa M. Garriga Bala Amarasekaran 《International journal of primatology》2008,29(6):1671-1685
In April 2006, a group of chimpanzees escaped from the Tacugama Chimpanzee Sanctuary in Sierra Leone, resulting in the death
of 1 local citizen and injury of another. The chimpanzees caused no further damage after the initial incident. Of the 31 escaped
chimpanzees, 27 returned to the sanctuary by the end of 2006, 21 of them voluntarily. The sanctuary is in a forest reserve,
a habitat of wild chimpanzees. Ironically, the tragic incident provided an opportunity to observe the behavior of the escaped
chimpanzees and their adaptability in the forest. As a result of the incident, local communities could have come to fear the
chimpanzees or develop negative feelings toward the sanctuary and its activities, which include keeping a colony of orphaned
chimpanzees for rehabilitation and promoting the protection of wild populations. However, collaboration and understanding
among the sanctuary, local communities, and government authorities resulted in peaceful handling of the situation and the
humane retrieval of 27 of the escaped chimpanzees. As the number of chimpanzees in African sanctuaries increases, they are
responsible more than ever for minimizing hazards to surrounding communities. It is important for sanctuaries to develop understanding
and to raise support from local communities and government authorities to help them avoid crises and continue their activities. 相似文献
16.
Tatyana Humle Christelle Colin Matthieu Laurans Estelle Raballand 《International journal of primatology》2011,32(2):456-473
The release of wild or captive-bred mammals within their historical ranges typically aims to reestablish populations in areas
where they have become extinct or extirpated, to reinforce natural populations, or to resolve human–wildlife conflicts. Such
programs, which also typically in parallel help foster the protection of the release site, concern a wide range of endangered
mammalian species, including our closest living relatives: chimpanzees. In June 2008, the Chimpanzee Conservation Center (CCC),
which is located in the High Niger National Park (HNNP) in Guinea, released a group of 12 chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) comprised of 6 females and 6 males (8–20 yr old). The selected release site lies 32 km from the sanctuary in the Mafou,
a core area of HNNP where wild chimpanzees are also known to occur. The purpose of this release was therefore to reinforce
the natural chimpanzee population within the Mafou core area and to promote the protection of the HNNP. Nearly 2 yr postrelease,
9 chimpanzees still remain free-living. Two thirds of the release chimpanzees were equipped with VHF-GPS store-on-board tracking
collars. We used data from retrieved collars to explore the release chimpanzees’ habitat use, individual day range, and core
area use (50% and 80%) during the first year of the release. Males traveled significantly further than females. Although minimum
day range did not differ between the sexes or vary seasonally, some release males were active for longer during the day than
the females. Males also ranged over larger areas and used a wider network of core areas than the females. Habitat use was
similar to that recorded in wild chimpanzees in the HNNP. As of September 2010, 2 males and 3 females form a group at the
release site. Two of these females gave birth to healthy offspring respectively 16 and 20 mo postrelease. Another female successfully
immigrated into a wild chimpanzee community. We suggest that the success of this chimpanzee release can be attributed to the
CCC’s lengthy rehabilitation process and the savanna-mosaic habitat of the HNNP. This release demonstrates that under special
socioecological circumstances, the release of wild-born adult chimpanzees of both sexes is a viable strategy, which can also
function as an effective conservation tool. 相似文献
17.
Female chimpanzees mate promiscuously during a period of extended receptivity marked by prominent sexual swelling. Recent
studies of wild chimpanzees indicate that subtle variations in swelling size could act as a reliable cue of female fertilization
potential both within and between cycles (Emery and Whitten Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 54, 340–351, 2003; Deschner et al.
Hormones and Behavior, 46, 204–215, 2004). Copulation rates increase during the periovulatory period and during conception cycles (Deschner et al.
Hormones and Behavior, 46, 204–215, 2004; Emery Thompson American Journal of Primatology, 67, 137–158, 2005a), suggesting that males may be able to assess female fertilization potential. We asked whether facultative timing of copulation
in Kanyawara chimpanzees was due to increased male mating interest or to increased female proceptivity during the most fecund
days. We assessed multiple measures of male mating effort in cycles aligned relative to the day of detumescence and compared
periovulatory days to other days of maximal swelling, and conception cycles to nonconception cycles. The rate and proportion
of male initiative in soliciting sexual behavior increased during periods of highest fertilization potential. Males were also
more likely to interrupt copulations, associate with estrous females, and compete with other males when females were most
likely to conceive. Females initiated copulations more frequently during conception cycles but did not visibly shift mating
behavior within cycles. Our results support the hypothesis that male chimpanzees have the ability to assess the profitability
of mating attempts, a trait that may act as a counter-adaptation to female strategies to obscure paternity. We discuss potential
cues and the implications for female reproductive strategies. 相似文献
18.
David Morgan Crickette Sanz Jean Robert Onononga Samantha Strindberg 《International journal of primatology》2016,37(6):718-737
Accurate and precise surveys of primate abundance provide the basis for understanding species ecology and essential information for conservation assessments. Owing to the elusive nature of wild apes and the vast region of dense forest they inhabit, population estimates of central chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) and western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) have largely relied on surveys of their nests. Specific information about the nesting behavior of apes permits the estimation of the number of nests built (nest creation rate). Similarly, information on nest characteristics and environmental factors can be used to estimate the time it takes nests to decay (nest decay rate). Nest creation and decay rates are then used to convert nest density estimates to absolute ape densities. Population estimates that use site-specific estimates of nest creation and decay rates are more accurate and precise. However, it is common practice to generalize these conversion factors across sites because of the additional cost of studies required to gather the information to estimate them. Over a 9-mo study period, we detected and monitored the time to decay of gorilla nests (N = 514) and chimpanzee nests (N = 521) in northern Republic of Congo. We investigated the influence of nest characteristics and environmental factors on nest survivorship and estimated the mean time to nest decay (or equivalently survival) using MARK. Key factors influencing nest decay rate included ape species, forest type, nest height, mean rainfall, nest structure, nest type, and primary aspects of nest construction. Our findings highlight the synergistic effect of behavior and environment on great ape nest degradation, as well as providing practical insights for improving measures to monitor remaining populations of these endangered species. 相似文献
19.
Female sexual strategies affect male strategies and can play an important role in shaping mating systems. We investigated
female sexual behaviour within five groups of grey-cheeked mangabeys in Kibale National Park, Uganda, and tested the hypothesis
that females exhibit mate choice using as indications the prevalence of (1) females soliciting matings by presenting to males
and (2) females refusing to mate with approaching males. In addition, we describe how these behaviours as well as grooming
and copulation calls are distributed over high-ranking, low-ranking and migrating males and discuss these patterns with regard
to trade-offs that could play a roll in female mate choice in multi-male groups. Females were promiscuous and initiated almost
half of the matings, with both resident and migrating males. More than half of male mating approaches were refused by peak
females. Female mate choice in this species may depend on individual female preferences, oestrus phase and male tactic. 相似文献
20.
New combinations are proposed in anticipation of the Polygonaceae treatment in the forthcoming volume of Intermountain Flora:
Polygonum kelloggii var. esotericum, P. kelloggii var. watsonii
,
Rumex densiflorus var. pycnanthus
,
R. salicifolius var. utahensis, and R. occidentalis var. tomentellus. Typifications are proposed to facilitate ongoing studies in Polygonaceae and to maintain current usage. 相似文献