首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
The effect of consumption of bitter taste substances (caffeine and beer) to bitter taste sensitivity was examined by 19 healthy adults. For individual taste sensitivity, detection thresholds were used on 6 bitter substances (caffeine, iso-alpha-acids: beer bittering agents, quinine sulfate, L-tryptophan, L-phenylalanine and glycyl-L-phenylalanyl-L-phenylalanine) and 3 non-bitter substances (L-aspartic acid, aspartame and NaCl). Nonusers of caffeine had significantly higher sensitivity (lower threshold) for caffeine compared to moderate and heavy users. Slight consumers of beer had significantly higher sensitivity for iso-alpha-acids relative to heavy users of beer, Iso-alpha-acids were not detected in saliva in acute dosing test by using 6 subjects. The correlations between thresholds of 6 bitter substances were calculated. Significant correlations (p < 0.01) were noted in 2 cases between caffeine and quinine, and iso-alpha-acids and L-trypothan. These data suggest the significant relation between individual bitter taste sensitivity and the consumption of caffeine and beer (iso-alpha-acids).  相似文献   

2.
Results obtained in the determination of sensory thresholds with three different simplified methods were compared. The methods were: a modification of the ascending method of limits (1), one based on the method of the constant stimuli (2), and one based on the use of scales (3). The study was carried out on two sensorially simple systems, sucrose and aspartame aqueous solutions. The thresholds values for sucrose obtained by method 1 (1.41 g/L) and by method 2 (1.60 g/L) were similar and within the range obtained by method 3 (0.6–1.6 g/L). For aspartame the values obtained with methods 1 and 2 were also similar (0.004 and 0.005 g/L, respectively), but they were below the minimum value of the corresponding range (0.008–0.016 g/L) calculated with method 3. These results suggest that the sensory methods provoked less variability in thresholds values than other noncontrolled factors. The limitations and advantages of each method are discussed considering: experimental work, precision of results and practical validity.  相似文献   

3.
The influence of temperature on the threshold values of primary tastes   总被引:4,自引:2,他引:2  
The threshold values of taste substances are influenced by severalfactors. To learn about the effect of the temperature on stimulus,recognition and terminal thresholds, these threshold valueswere determined for sucrose, sodium chloride, caffeine, quininehydrochloride, citric and tartaric acid at temperatures of 10,20, 40 and 60°C in a panel of 19 tasters. The individualvalues were found to vary over a wide range, resulting in arelatively large standard deviation of the mean threshold values.A temperature-dependence was found for the stimulus and recognitionthresholds which was different for the different taste substances.The stimulus and recognition thresholds are lowest in the temperaturerange of 20 to 40°C. The threshold values increase withincreasing temperature, and except for citric acid, significantand highly significant differences existed particularly between20 and 60°C, whereas statistically verifiable results couldnot be obtained between 10 and 20°C. There was no verifiabletemperature dependence either for the terminal thresholds. Theterminal thresholds were found to lie rather in a relativelynarrow concentration range and to be largely independent ofthe temperature. The results suggest that in warm dishes andbeverage more taste substances are required to produce the sametaste intensity. A dependence of the individual thresholds uponage, sex and smoking habits could not be found.  相似文献   

4.
Taste aversion studies have demonstrated that rats conditioned to avoid monosodium glutamate (MSG) with amiloride added to reduce the intensity of the sodium component of MSG taste, generalize this aversion to aspartic acid and to L-AP4, but not to ionotropic glutamate receptor agonists. That is, MSG, L-AP4 and aspartate have similar tastes to rats. However, conditioned taste aversion methods are unable to show to what extent the tastes of two substances are different. If two substances activate the same afferent processes (e.g. taste receptors), they are likely to produce the same tastes, but if they activate different afferent processes, the subject may detect differences between the tastes of the substances. In this study, rats were tested to determine if they could discriminate between the tastes of these agonists and MSG. We also established the detection thresholds for NMDA, aspartic acid and L-AP4, with and without amiloride (a sodium channel antagonist). Taste threshold values were 1-4 mM for NMDA and aspartic acid and 0.5-2.5 microM for L-AP4. None were affected by 30 micro M amiloride. Rats could readily distinguish between the tastes of MSG and NMDA but they had difficulty discriminating between the tastes of aspartic acid and MSG. Rats could also easily distinguish between 10-100 mM MSG and 0.01-5 mM L-AP4. However, in two separate experiments error rates increased significantly when L-AP4 concentrations were between 10-100 mM, indicating that the tastes of L-AP4 and MSG were similar at these concentrations.  相似文献   

5.
To study the effect of viscosity on the stimulus and recognitionthresholds of the four primary tastes, four thickeners whichare relatively free of any specific taste and odour (methylcellulose, guar seed flour, carob seed flour and tara seed flour)were selected from a greater number of such substances availableand the stimulus and recognition thresholds at the four viscositylevels, 1, 10, 100 and 1 000 cP determined for sucrose, sodiumchloride, caffeine and citric acid. It was found that with increasingviscosity also the stimulus and recognition thresholds increasedwhich means that higher concentrations of the taste substancesare necessary to produce the same taste intensity at higherviscosity. Significant differences were found particularly atthe highest viscosity level of 1 000 cP as compared to 1 cP.By relating the individual values to the corresponding valueat 1 cP it is possible to determine, bymeans of the so-calledamplification factors, the concentration increase that is necessarytoproduce the same taste intensity as at 1 cP. On the basisof regression straight lines, this amplification factor canbe determined for all substances and taste qualities. It becameobvious in this way that not only viscosity is important butalso the kind of thickener used.  相似文献   

6.
Molecular mechanisms of salty taste in mammals are not completely understood. We use genetic approaches to study these mechanisms. Previously, we developed a high-throughput procedure to measure NaCl taste thresholds, which involves conditioning mice to avoid LiCl and then examining avoidance of NaCl solutions presented in 48-h 2-bottle preference tests. Using this procedure, we measured NaCl taste thresholds of mice from 13 genealogically divergent inbred stains: 129P3/J, A/J, BALB/cByJ, C3H/HeJ, C57BL/6ByJ, C57BL/6J, CBA/J, CE/J, DBA/2J, FVB/NJ, NZB/BlNJ, PWK/PhJ, and SJL/J. We found substantial strain variation in NaCl taste thresholds: mice from the A/J and 129P3/J strains had high thresholds (were less sensitive), whereas mice from the BALB/cByJ, C57BL/6J, C57BL/6ByJ, CE/J, DBA/2J, NZB/BINJ, and SJL/J had low thresholds (were more sensitive). NaCl taste thresholds measured in this study did not significantly correlate with NaCl preferences or amiloride sensitivity of chorda tympani nerve responses to NaCl determined in the same strains in other studies. To examine whether strain differences in NaCl taste thresholds could have been affected by variation in learning ability or sensitivity to toxic effects of LiCl, we used the same method to measure citric acid taste thresholds in 4 inbred strains with large differences in NaCl taste thresholds but similar acid sensitivity in preference tests (129P3/J, A/J, C57BL/6J, and DBA/2J). Citric acid taste thresholds were similar in these 4 strains. This suggests that our technique measures taste quality-specific thresholds that are likely to represent differences in peripheral taste responsiveness. The strain differences in NaCl taste sensitivity found in this study provide a basis for genetic analysis of this phenotype.  相似文献   

7.
Recent research has implicated T1R1/T1R3 as the primary taste receptor in mammals for detecting L-amino acids, including L-monosodium glutamate (MSG) and L-alanine. Previous behavioral studies with rodents found only minimal evidence that these two substances share perceptual qualities, but those studies did not control for the taste of sodium associated with MSG. This study used several behavioral methods to compare the perceptual qualities of MSG and L-alanine in rats, using amiloride (a sodium channel blocker) to reduce the sodium component of MSG taste. Detection thresholds of L-alanine in rats ranged between 0.4 and 2.5 mM, with or without amiloride added, which are similar to threshold estimates for MSG. Conditioned taste aversion (CTA) found that rats showed strong cross-generalization of CTA between MSG and L-alanine when mixed with amiloride, indicating the two substances have similar perceptual qualities. Discrimination methods showed that rats easily discriminated between L-alanine and MSG unless the cue function of sodium was reduced. The discrimination became significantly more difficult at concentrations < 100 mM when amiloride was added to all stimuli and became even more difficult when NaCl was also added to L-alanine solutions to match the sodium concentrations of MSG. These results indicate that, perceptually, MSG and L-alanine have quite similar taste qualities and support the hypothesis that these two L-amino acids activate a common taste receptor. The differences in perceptual qualities also suggest separate afferent processing of one or both substances may also be involved.  相似文献   

8.
A number of behavioral experiments have attempted to measurethe absolute taste thresholds in the rat. Certain aspects ofthe procedures and methods of data analysis used in these experimentsare reviewed with the suggestions that: (1) Although comparisonshave commonly been made between the taste threshold values reportedin different experiments, these are not valid because of theabsence of a common definition of threshold. (2) The high thresholdmodel on which these experiments are based may be inappropriate.(3) It is possible to measure taste sensitivity rather thantaste thresholds, and (4) Had these points been considered inprevious experiments different conclusions might have resultedregarding the effects of adrenalectomy on NaCl sensitivity.  相似文献   

9.
用脑干电位的互相关函数客观估计听力阈值   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
陈琳  陈俊强 《生理学报》1989,41(2):196-202
将脑干诱发电位记录分为大小相等的两个子集均,求其互相关函数。互相关函数在原点附近的平均幅度作为估计听力阈值的参数,并通过实验得出表达该参数与阈上刺激强度关系的直线回归方程,从而求出听力阈值的估计公式。把三种阈上刺激强度下的参数值分别代入估计公式,平均结果作为听力阈值的估计。对8个有不同听力损失的病耳进行了估计,其结果与主观阈值平均相差4.6dB。  相似文献   

10.
Effects of gustatory nerve transection on salt taste have been studied extensively in rats and hamsters but have not been well explored in the mouse. We examined the effects of chorda tympani (CT) nerve transection on NaCl taste preferences and thresholds in outbred CD-1 mice using a high-throughput phenotyping method developed in our laboratory. To measure taste thresholds, mice were conditioned by oral self-administration of LiCl or NaCl and then presented with NaCl concentration series in 2-bottle preference tests. LiCl-conditioned and control NaCl-exposed mice were given bilateral transections of the CT nerve (LiCl-CTX, NaCl-CTX) or were left intact as controls (LiCl-CNT, NaCl-CNT). After recovery from surgery, mice received a concentration series of NaCl (0-300 mM) in 48-h 2-bottle tests. CT transection increased NaCl taste thresholds in LiCl-conditioned mice and eliminated avoidance of concentrated NaCl in control NaCl-exposed mice. This demonstrates that in mice, the CT nerve is important for detection and recognition of NaCl taste and is necessary for the normal avoidance of high concentrations of NaCl. The results of this experiment also show that the method of high-throughput phenotyping of salt taste thresholds is suitable for detecting changes in the taste periphery in mouse genetic studies.  相似文献   

11.
Two taste sensitivity measures (detection threshold and time-intensity [T/I]) were applied to evaluate bitter taste sensitivity of caffeine in both caffeine nonusers (n = 12) and users (n=12, caffeine consumption level; > 300mg/day). Taste thresholds of caffeine were significantly different between nonusers and users in two test sessions (p < 0.01 in first and p < 0.05 in second). In T/I measures, only the difference in peak height was observed between the two groups in one of two test sessions. The other two measures, half width and area of T/I curves showed no significant difference.  相似文献   

12.
Astringent substances and pungent substances were studied usinga multichannel taste sensor with lipid membranes. The electric-potentialpattern constructed of eight outputs from the membranes hasinformation of taste quality and intensity. Pungent substances,such as capsaicin, pipeline and allyl isothiocyanate, had noeffect on the membrane potentials of the lipid membranes. Onthe other hand, astringent substances such as tannic acid, catechin,gallic acid and chlorogenic acid changed the potentials remarkably.A principal component analysis of the patterns in electric potentialchanges caused by tbe taste substances revealed the astringencyis located between bitterness and sourness.  相似文献   

13.
It iS believed that people's sensitivity to taste declines with age but the evidence is inconclusive. This study was designed to test the hypothesis that taste recognition thresholds (TRTs) for sweetness are higher in older than in younger individuals, using groups of 16 younger subjects (18–30) and 16 older subjects (60–85). Three test substances were used: sucrose, aspartame and saccharin. A questionnaire recorded variables which might have affected TRTs, but data failed to show any trend that might have biased the principle variate-age. There was a significant alteration with age of recognition thresholds, at least for sucrose and saccharin. The differences between the groups for the three sweeteners were due to the fact that all the very sensitive subjects were young. None of the older subjects had particularly poor discrimination: all but one had TRTs within the range of younger subjects. Although there are age-related taste changes, they are much less dramatic than commonly occurs with other senses, such as sight and hearing. The findings of this study have implications for institutional catering and the dietary management of older people using non-sugar sweeteners.  相似文献   

14.
Astringent and bitter sensations are characteristic sensory qualities of black tea. Three different classes of potential astringent reference standards (two concentrations each of alum and tannic acid and three fruit juices) were evaluated in this study. The perceived astringency, bitterness and sourness of each were profiled using computerized time-intensity and compared with the astringent intensity of a standardized brew of black tea. The differences in temporal profiles of potential reference standards across taste attributes were evident and intensity ratings were found to be dependent upon the stimulus and its concentration. Both concentrations of tannic acid were evaluated as the highest in perceived bitterness. For the juices, a strong sour taste was perceived in addition to astringency. It was concluded that the best reference standard for the astringency of black tea is a solution of 0.7 g/L alum as it is low in perceived bitterness and sourness.  相似文献   

15.
Twenty judges performed a variety of chemosensory tasks in order to select the best scores to form a panel for coffee evaluation. An average of correct responses (P%), one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and principal components analysis (PCA) were compared. The tests involved: ability to recognize the four basic tastes, identification and matching of odors, taste intensity evaluation and perception of small differences in taste. P% accounted for 71.17 ± 4.34% and 10 of the judges had scores greater than the final average. ANOVA and PCA resulted in 2 different panels consisting of 9 and 12 judges, respectively. The panel was composed by the nine panelists selected by the three methods. The other three panelists that were doubtful could improve to the point of acceptance with additional training. These methods should be used simultaneously to have more security in the acceptance or rejection of panelists.  相似文献   

16.
Molecular and behavioral studies have identified heterodimers of the T1R family as receptors for detecting the tastes of sweet (T1R2 + T1R3) and umami (T1R1 + T1R3). However, behavioral studies have reported conflicting findings with T1R3 knockout (KO) mice. One study showed a complete or nearly complete loss of preference for sweet and umami substances by KO mice, whereas KO mice in another study showed only a partial reduction in preferences for sucrose and monosodium glutamate (MSG), the prototypical umami substance. The present experiments used psychophysical methods to assess how sensitive T1R1-KO mice are to sucrose and MSG and discrimination methods to determine if these mice could distinguish between the tastes of sucrose and MSG. Detection thresholds of T1R3-KO mice and wild-type (WT) C57Bl mice were nearly identical for sucrose and MSG. Mice of both genotypes were easily able to discriminate between the tastes of sucrose and MSG. When amiloride (a sodium channel blocker) was added to all solutions to reduce the taste of Na+, discrimination accuracy of both genotypes of mice decreased but more so for the T1R3-KO mice than the WT mice. However, even when the sodium taste of MSG was neutralized, both genotypes could still discriminate between the two substances well above chance performance. These results suggest that sucrose and MSG can be detected by taste receptors other than T1R2 + T1R3 and T1R1 + T1R3 and that the conflicts between the previous studies may have been due to the methodological limitations.  相似文献   

17.
T Kumazawa  T Nomura  K Kurihara 《Biochemistry》1988,27(4):1239-1244
Various bitter substances were found to depolarize liposomes. The results obtained are as follows: (1) Changes in the membrane potential of azolectin liposomes in response to various bitter substances were monitored by measuring changes in the fluorescence intensity of 3,3'-dipropylthiocarbocyanine iodide [diS-C3(5)]. All the bitter substances examined increased the fluorescence intensity of the liposome-dye suspension, which indicates that the substances depolarize the liposomes. There existed a good correlation between the minimum concentrations of the bitter substances to depolarize the liposomes and the taste thresholds in humans. (2) The effects of changed lipid composition of liposomes on the responses to various bitter substances vary greatly among bitter substances, suggesting that the receptor sites for bitter substances are multiple. The responses to N-C=S substances and sucrose octaacetate especially greatly depended on the lipid composition; these compounds depolarized only liposomes having certain lipid composition, while no or hyperpolarizing responses to these compounds were observed in other liposomes examined. This suggested that the difference in "taster" and "nontaster" for these substances can be explained in terms of difference in the lipid composition of taste receptor membranes. (3) It was confirmed that the membrane potential of the planar lipid bilayer is changed in response to bitter substances. The membrane potential changes in the planar lipid bilayer as well as in liposomes in response to the bitter substances occurred under the condition that there is no ion gradient across the membranes. These results suggested that the membrane potential changes in response to bitter substances stem from the phase boundary potential changes induced by adsorption of the substances on the hydrophobic region of the membranes.  相似文献   

18.
Wine vinegar is a product obtained from wine acidification which contains at least 5% by wt. of acetic acid, in general without any additives or colorings.
Aspects studied in this work include: the determination of the taste group thresholds (geometric mean of the individual best-estimate thresholds "BET") of two different acids (citric and acetic acids) in aqueous solution and spanish vinegars produced from table and sherry wines. The results obtained suggest that wine vinegar can be considered something more than just an acidulant agent.
In order to evaluate differences among wine vinegars, discriminant tests for twenty-five spanish vinegars (sherry, table and flavored vinegars) were applied. Six of the twelve attributes freely chosen by assessors allowed grouping of the spanish wine vinegars according to their sensory aspects.  相似文献   

19.
Electrical potential oscillations were obtained across a liquid membrane composed of nitrobenzene/picric acid placed between two aqueous phases in the presence of various taste (i.e. salty, sweet and bitter) substances. The influence of these compounds on electrical oscillations was studied using Fourier analysis to establish a "fingerprint" of the substance that can be correlated with its taste index. Various concentrations of each substance were tested to obtain a Fourier spectrum with discrete peaks which can be further processed. The electrical oscillations consisted of a number of weak damped oscillators, and the Fourier spectra of these signals were found to have a number of discrete peaks of decreasing amplitude at low frequencies (0-0.5 Hz). A correlation of the frequency of the first peak of the Fourier spectrum with the taste index was found for bitter substances, whereas for salty substances the amplitude of the first two peaks of the spectrum was correlated with the taste index.  相似文献   

20.
The application of sensory methodology for measuring deodorizing effect of an air conditioner equipped with electric plasma was introduced. Deodorizing effect was measured using chemical and sensory methods at different time (0, 30 and 60 min) and mode (control, blowing and cooling) of an air conditioner. Smoke from a roll of cigarette in a closed room was used as a source of odor and the concentrations of acetic acid and ammonia were measured as odorous chemical components. As one of the sensory methods triangle test was used and as a first step to obtain deodorizing effects by triangle test, the threshold of each panelist was obtained as the log dilution ratio of odor concentration at which the difference from odorless air was detected. The odor concentration at each time and mode was calculated using the threshold of the panel and the deodorizing effect was obtained on the basis of the odor concentration. In addition to a triangle test, scaling methods such as category scaling or magnitude estimation were used to measure deodorizing effect of an air conditioner. Deodorizing effects by scaling methods were calculated based on odor intensity with time at each mode. The regression analysis was done between the efficacy of deodorizing effect by sensory test and those by acetic acid and ammonia, the R2 values of the regression equations for triangle test, category scale, and magnitude estimation were 0.84, 0.72 and 0.69, respectively. Deodorizing effect by triangle test explained the decrease of acetic acid and ammonia better than those by category scaling or magnitude estimation while high cost and time consuming labor involved in triangle tests reduced the merit. The results of this study demonstrated that various sensory methods could be used to measure deodorizing effect of air conditioners and further researches on fast and reliable methods are needed to establish the official procedures.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号