共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
Malcolm L. Hunter 《Animal behaviour》1980,28(2):468-475
Territorial male great tits (Parus major L.) usually forage on the ground, sing from perches with a mean height of 9.3 m, and perform most other activities between 2 and 4 m. Analyses of their habitat's visual and acoustical stratification indicate that great tits sing from high perches to make themselves hard to locate visually rather than to make their songs carry farther. The birds' selection of inconspicuous singing spots is further substantiated by their striking preference for hawthorn trees, which have an extremely dense crown and produce leaves very early in the year. 相似文献
3.
4.
5.
Linda Partridge 《Animal behaviour》1976,24(3):534-544
I have tried to show (a) that results in the literature suggest that blue tits and coal tits survive better in their usual habitats of broad-leaved and conifer woods respectively; (b) that the higher mortality in the ‘wrong’ habitat may be attributed at least in part to an inability to acquire an adequate supply of food; (c) from my own and other people's field observations, that wild coal tits and blue tits use different feeding techniques; (d) from my own laboratory experiments, that each species is ‘best’ at the feeding techniques which it in general uses in the wild, and also that each species prefers to use the feeding techniques at which it is ‘best’; (e) that the feeding techniques of coal tits may be adapted to exploit the type of food supply found in conifers, while those of blue tits may be adapted for feeding in broad-leaved trees. 相似文献
6.
Fabrizio Grieco 《Animal behaviour》2002,64(4):517-526
Previous work on food-provisioning behaviour in blue tits suggested that the parents could gather larger prey items only by making longer foraging excursions, for example, by being more selective or by reaching more distant (and less exploited) feeding sites. Here, I show that within-nest, within-day variation in size of prey delivered by the parent could be explained by the time since its last visit. In unmanipulated conditions, size of larvae tended to increase with the time spent away from the nest. A significant positive relationship was more likely at high provisioning rates, suggesting that periods of intense feeding limited the size of prey delivered to the brood. To assess the effect of less intense feeding on prey size, I experimentally increased food availability to the tits. The parents could decide whether to eat the extra food or feed it to the nestlings. In both cases, food supplementation could result in longer time lags between natural feedings. Food-supplemented parents consumed the extra food and fed it to their nestlings, made longer foraging trips and delivered larger natural larvae than controls. In this group, size of larvae was more constant during the observation period and was independent of the time since the parent's last visit. This suggests that, below some value of visit rate, prey size is no longer limited by the duration of the foraging trip. The results support the view that tits continually vary visit rate and prey size. There is some evidence that these adjustments are made by changing food selectivity in response to changes in the state of the brood and of the parents.Copyright 2002 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved . 相似文献
7.
Indrikis Krams Jolanta VrublevskaDina Cirule Inese KivlenieceTatjana Krama Markus J. RantalaElin Sild Peeter Hõrak 《Comparative biochemistry and physiology. Part A, Molecular & integrative physiology》2012,161(4):422-428
Animals display remarkable individual variation in their capacity to mount immune responses against novel antigens. According to the life-history theory, this variation is caused by the costs of immune responses to the hosts. We studied one of such potential costs, depletion of somatic resources in wintering wild-caught captive passerines, the great tits (Parus major) by immune challenging the birds with a novel antigen, killed Brucella abortus (BA) suspension. We found that despite mild temperature conditions in captivity and ad libitum availability of food, immune challenge depleted somatic resources (as indicated by a body mass loss) and elevated relative proportion of heterophils to lymphocytes (H/L ratio) in the peripheral blood of birds. However, body mass loss did not covary with an increase in H/L ratios between two sampling events, which indicates that these two markers of health state describe different aspects of individual physiological condition. Antibody titres were not associated with the extent of body mass loss during the development of immune response, which shows that the somatic cost of immune response was not proportional to the amount of antibody produced. Birds with high pre-immunisation H/L ratios mounted weaker antibody response, which is indicative of stress-induced suppression of humoral immune response and is consistent with the concept of an antagonistic cross-regulation between different components of the immune system. The latter finding suggests a novel diagnostic value of H/L ratios, which reinforces the utility of this simple haematological index for prediction of the outcomes of complicated immune processes. 相似文献
8.
9.
10.
11.
Marianne Gabirot Aurora M. Castilla Pilar López José Martín 《Biochemical Systematics and Ecology》2010
Chemical signals can be the basis of interspecific recognition and speciation in many animals. The Columbretes Islands wall lizard, Podarcis atrata is very close genetically to the mainland Iberian wall lizard Podarcis hispanica. However, a previous study suggested that chemosensory interspecific recognition would avoid reproductive interactions and hybridization between these two species. These results suggested that chemicals used in intraspecific communication might differ in composition and/or proportions between these two species. In this paper, we used gas chromatography–mass spectrometry (GC–MS) to characterize the chemical composition of the lipophilic fraction from femoral gland secretions of male P. atrata and P. hispanica. The analysis showed that chemicals found in femoral secretions varied in composition and proportions between species and between populations. Seven steroids and two unidentified waxy esters, were exclusive of P. atrata lizards from the islands. In contrast, nine steroids and other six compounds were only found in mainland P. hispanica. There were also differences in proportions of shared compounds between species. Moreover, all these differences were higher between P. atrata and P. hispanica than between any population of P. hispanica. Chemical differences might be consequence of genetic differences, but they could also be explained by adaptation to different habitats with different climatic conditions or diet resources. Compounds that are specific of each species, or differences in the pattern of compounds, could explain species recognition. Therefore, these results of chemical composition and previous studies of chemosensory recognition reinforce the fact that the genetic differences between P. hispanica and P. atrata may result in an effective reproductive isolation between these two taxa. 相似文献
12.
Earlier work has shown that when mice are sensitized with irradiated metacercariae the numbers of immature flukes that can be recovered from the peritoneal cavity 2 days after reinfection with normal metacercariae is significantly less than the numbers recovered from non-sensitized control mice. Experiments are now described which investigate the reason for this difference. An inflammatory cellular reaction, most marked in sensitized mice occurs in the intestinal wall but this does not delay the migration of challenge flukes into the peritoneal cavity. No effective protective mechanism operates at the intestinal wall because similar numbers of flukes are present in the livers of sensitized and non-sensitized mice at 12 and 14 days after infection. When livers of sensitized and non-sensitized mice were examined 2 days after infection significantly more flukes had already reached the liver in the sensitized group. This indicates that immature flukes migrate more quickly from the peritoneal cavity in mice previously sensitized with irradiated metacercariae and would account for the difference in the number of flukes recovered from the peritoneal cavity of sensitized and non-sensitized mice at 2 days after infection. 相似文献
13.
14.
Six hand-reared great kiskadees (Pitangus sulphuratus) were given still and moving models of prey. Series A bore various combinations of eyes, tail, neck and motion; eyes and tail were the most strongly directing. Series B bore various colours and patterns, both as covering the whole model, and as covering only one-third (middle or end). The birds were not inhibited by any plain colour tested but strongly avoided patterns resembling coral snakes. Given sufficient contrast, kiskadees were attracted to the painted section of the partly painted models, regardless of location (middle or end). This attraction to the contrasting section, plus the directed responses to eyes, tail and motion, should allow kiskadees to take small vertebrates like lizards quickly and efficiently. 相似文献
15.
In cooperatively breeding birds multiple maternity and paternity of broods is not uncommon, reproduction often being shared among group members as well as with extragroup members. We investigated the extent of extrapair paternity and intraspecific brood parasitism in a population of cooperatively breeding long-tailed tits. Our aim was to determine the frequency and cause of mixed parentage and to investigate whether shared maternity or paternity was associated with decisions made by helpers. Genetic analyses using eight microsatellite loci showed that extrapair paternity was low (2.4-6.9% of nestlings in 16-29% of broods), and that intraspecific brood parasitism was negligible. Mate switching and extrapair copulations were both observed, but mate switching was not responsible for the mixed paternity we recorded. Some extrapair offspring were assigned to males that became helpers at the nest containing their extrapair young, but these males were also close neighbours of the cuckolded males and so were the most likely males to gain extrapair paternity. There was no evidence that the existence of a direct reproductive stake in a brood played an important role in the helping decisions of either male or female helpers. Copyright 2002 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. 相似文献
16.
According to the ''energetic-bottleneck'' hypothesis, incubation in birds is constrained by the availability of energy. Hence, uniparental incubators are predicted to respond to a change in energy supply by adjusting, positively or negatively, the time spent warming the clutch. Energetic constraints on incubation in the great tit (Parus major) were demonstrated by heating nests, so that the night-time thermostatic component of daily energy expenditure in females was reduced by comparison with a control group. Birds in heated nests increased the time allocated to incubation during the day by 55 minutes, consistent with the predictions of the energetic-bottleneck hypothesis. Daily energy expenditure of all birds was inversely related to night-time ambient temperature, and did not differ between warmed and control birds on mild nights. When temperatures were low, however, escalation of daily costs was less for birds in heated nests. It is suggested that the balance of the energy budget may effect a proximate control on the constancy of incubation, with likely implications for reproductive success. 相似文献
17.
18.
Different song types are associated with certain behaviours including intrasexual aggression, mating, and initiation of song bouts. All song types are used in territorial advertisement. Songs associated with sexual and aggressive behaviours are similar in structure; female mate choice may have driven sexual selection in favour of the more aggressive males. Song sequence is non-random with repetition of all song types and alternation of some, and songs with similar messages are often associated in sequence. Predictions from alternative models were tested, but the significance of the large song repertoire in this species is largely explained by the message content of the different song types. 相似文献
19.
20.
The chance of encounter—and hence mating—between two animals may be limited by social as well as spatial factors, thus dividing a population into a series of discrete territories with little movement between them. Only within the sub-units will random mating take place, and drift may cause a considerable amount of non-adaptive genetical change if the units are very small. Work on House mouse ( Mus musculus ) populations in the laboratory and particular ecological situations have suggested that the effective breeding size of these units may be as small as four. This would mean that a considerable amount of random change would be expected in mouse populations. However, a six year study (1964–69) of movement and territoriality on the 244 acre (100 ha) Welsh island of Skokholm during which over 3000 animals were marked and released, showed that more than 20% of individuals breed in an area other than the one in which they were born, i.e. a considerable amount of population churning takes place. This conclusion is supported by evidence of the spread of three rare biochemical variants in the population. Chance seems to play little part in the determination of the genetical constitution of the Skokholm mice. In general, estimates of the size of effective breeding units must always be qualified by an understanding of the ecology of the population in question. 相似文献