首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
The paths of Colorado beetles (Leptinotarsa decemlineata Say)in a featureless environment are circular. This behavior is explained by an internal asymmetry. To stabilize the path, the fixation reaction or the optomotor response must work against this asymmetry. The turning behavior was examined in stationary patterns of vertical stripes different at spatial wavelengths (). The internal asymmetry was tested in a horizontally striped pattern. A stable fixation reaction was found only for 120 °. The results suggest that larger intrinsic turning tendencies shifts the stable point of the fixation reaction. The same vertically striped patterns were rotated to examine the following reaction of the beetle. It is concluded that the fixation component of the response of these insects, in particular, does not differ in the two situations.  相似文献   

2.
The turning response to surface waves of clawed toads (Xenopus laevis) with an inactivated lateral line was reinvestigated to examine whether sensory systems other than the lateral line (second systems) are involved. Two methods were used to block the lateral line input: selective and reversible inactivation of the lateral line periphery using CoCl2 or chronic destruction with thermocautery. The time-course of the response recovery (response frequency, turning accuracy and reaction time) was recorded. Following CoCl2 inactivation 10 out of 13 animals did not respond to surface waves for at least 2 days. The remaining 3 animals gave sporadic turning responses. It is assumed that in these individuals a second system is permanently involved in the detection of surface waves parallel to the lateral line. Five days after the chronic destruction of the lateral line all animals again turned to the centre of surface waves. It is suggested that by this time the second system had become capable of substituting for the missing lateral line input. The response frequency and the accuracy of the turning response of lesioned animals varied considerably among individuals but was always lower than in untreated animals (tested up to 120 days).Abbreviation LED light emitting diode  相似文献   

3.
Following antennal contact with the honeydew of the black scale (Saissetia oleae Olivier), female adult green lacewings (Chrysoperla carnea Stephens) showed a reduced mean walking speed (inverse orthokinesis), an increased mean turning frequency (positive klinokinesis), and an increased mean turning angle. Similar, but generally less pronounced, behavioural changes occurred after contact with solutions of acid hydrolysed L-tryptophan and water. Reduced walking speed, lower mean turning frequency and increased mean turning angle were also observed in insects exposed to honeydew during the period prior to antennal contact with the honeydew. The observed alterations in behaviour in response to honeydew could increase the likelihood of both honeydew and prey being located in the field.
Résumé A la suite d'un contact antennaire avec le miellat de la cochenille noire (Saissetia oleae), les femelles adultes deChrysoperla carnea Stephens présentent une vitesse de déplacement moyenne réduite (orthokinèse inverse), une fréquence de changement de direction moyenne accrue (klinokinèse positive), et une augmentation de l'angle de changement de direction moyen. De manière similaire mais généralement moins accentuée, des changements de comportement se manifestent après contact avec des solutions de L-triptophane hydrolysé en milieu acide. Une vitesse de déplacement réduite, une fréquence moyenne de changement de direction plus faible et un angle de changement de direction moyen en augmentation sont également observés chez les insectes exposés au miellat durant la période précédant un contact antennaire avec le miellat. Les altérations de comportement observées en réponse au miellat pourrait allonger la vie à la fois du miellat et de la proie dans la nature.
  相似文献   

4.
Abstract Blue-light-induced cell division in single-celled Adiantum protonemata was studied by using two or three pulses (pulse duration: 30 s) separated by various periods and by using relatively long irradiation (e.g. 30 min). The results showed: (1) that the response is saturated by a single pulse, (2) that after the application of a saturating pulse, the protonemata gradually become responsive to another pulse, showing time-dependent saturation to the second pulse, and (3) that although reciprocity holds in the pulse-induced response, it becomes invalid as the exposure duration extends in the range of minutes. These results were analysed in view of a reaction model in which a molecular component is considered to exist in two forms A and B. The response to a single pulse is considered to result from a light-dependent conversion of the component (AB), and the restoration response measured by two pulses, from its dark reversion (BA). The analyses yielded (1) the value of the constant which relates the fluence rate to the rate constant of the light-induced reaction, and (2) the rate constant of the dark reaction. The model was extended to formulate the responses to long irradiations as a function of the integrated concentration of B over time. The responses predicted by the formula by using the parameter values estimated from the pulse responses were able to explain the responses measured for long irradiation.  相似文献   

5.
Experiments were conducted on the searching behavior and searching efficiency of the lady beetleCoccinella septempunctata bruckii Mulsant under conditions of various prey distributions and prey densities. The larvae changed their searching behavior before and after feeding. Before feeding the larvae moved quickly and the searching paths were nearly linear. But after feeding the speed decreased and turning angle increased. The speed and turning angle reverted gradually and recovered the initial pattern 95 s after feeding. The searching efficiency differed depending on the prey distribution. At low prey density, searching was most efficient when prey were distributed uniformly. But at middle and high prey densities, searching was most efficient when prey items were highly aggregated. The observed searching behavior of 4th instarC. septempunctata larvae was likely to be optimal considering the natural distribution of colonies of their prey, aphids.  相似文献   

6.
This study confirms for a phylogenetically basal terrestrial vertebrate that dopaminergic modulations interfere with the visually directed appetitive and consummatory feeding behaviors orienting and snapping, respectively. (1) In common toads Bufo bufo, intralymphatic administration of the dopamine D2/D1-receptor agonist apomorphine led to a dose-dependent facilitation of prey-snapping in response to moving objects. The snapping activity reached a maximum 15–35 min after apomorphine injection. (2) To changes in configurational stimulus features, the basic pattern of discrimination was maintained; however, the acuity of discrimination was reduced due to the high snapping response level. (3) The apomorphine-induced facilitation of snapping was accompanied by a suppression of prey-oriented lunging and turning. Toads snapped only if prey occurred frontally in the visual field at a relatively short distance. The snapping behavior was fixed in its form and stereotyped regarding its immediate release. (4) About 90 min after apomorphine administration, prey-oriented turning behavior was restored and displayed a facilitatory rebound. (5) In comparative experiments with the species B. marinus, both prey-oriented turning and snapping responses were suppressed by apomorphine in a dose-dependent manner. (6) After pre-treatment with the dopamine antagonist haloperidol, apomorphine showed no measurable effect on the visual release of prey orienting or snapping. (7) The results contribute to the sensorimotor and the motivation hypothesis of dopamine function proposed for higher vertebrates and stimulate a comparative discussion of anatomic homologies and functional analogies. Accepted: 10 July 1996  相似文献   

7.
The role played by the cerebral cortex on turning responses elicited by electrical stimulation of P-LP was studied in 9 chronic cats. In three animals the sensorimotor cortex was bilaterally ablated and the threshold values for inducing a turning response were determined. None of the cats showed abnormal threshold values. In six cats the cerebral cortical area known to have reciprocal anatomical connections with P-LP was unilaterally removed. Seven days after surgery, 50% of the electrodes gave no responses; 25% evoked responses with high thresholds and 25% with normal thresholds. One month after surgery the percentages were: 14, 18 and 68 respectively. Histological findings show important fiber and neuronal degeneration in P-LP. It is postulated that this area of the cerebral cortex plays, a facilitatory role on the P-LP ability to induce turning response and that the recovery of this ability can be explained by the development of P-LP denervation supersensitivity.  相似文献   

8.
ABSTRACT. The effects of spatially uniform but temporally changing air humidity stimuli on the orientation behaviour of the large pine weevil ( Hylobius abietis L.) and the flour beetle ( Tenebrio molitor L.) were studied in a specially constructed vertical air flow chamber. Moist-acclimatized but hygronegative pine weevils were exposed for 10 s to a moist air stream ( c. 85% R.H.) and then for 10 s to a dry air stream ( c. 50% R.H.). The Tenebrio were divided into two groups acclimatized to moist and dry air respectively. For each group the humidity during the initial 5 s in the air stream was the same as during acclimatization, and then changed to the alternative level. In Hylobius the change from moist to dry air caused a decrease in spatial displacement brought about by a decrease in walking speed and an increase in the amount of turning per unit time. In Tenebrio the change from dry to moist air caused a decrease of walking speed and a turning-back reaction based, presumptively, on idiothetic information about the insect's previous walking direction. The average angle turned during 2 s after the humidity change was 165°. In moist-adapted beetles the dry air stream caused an identical orthokinetic arrestment, but no klinokinetic or klinotactic reaction. Klinokinesis is redefined as a change in the circular variance of turning angles per unit time, which does not change the mean direction (or directions in cases of multimodal distributions) of the circular distribution. Accordingly, klinotaxis consists of a change in the mean directions) of the circular distribution of angular velocities. In both species the turning frequency was more constant than turning per unit distance, suggesting a temporal control in the nervous system of klinokinetic and klinotactic behaviour.  相似文献   

9.
Abstract The paths of Colorado beetles ( Leptinotarsa decemlineata Say) in a featureless environment are circular, like those of other species studied. The turning velocity may reach 35o/s and is due to an internal asymmetry, which may change spontaneously. Normally, all control loops of the insect, like fixation or optomotor responses, must work against this asymmetry to stabilize the insect's path. Stationary vertical patterns damp this turning tendency, but their effect is not strong enough to induce a straight path. Only 70% of the turning tendency can be so eliminated. This reaction is termed optomotor response because it can be adequately described with the parameter turning velocity alone. The insect's path was stabilized more effectively when pattern wavelengths were greater than 60o. The insects seemed to fixate these wider stripes. This reaction is termed fixation because the correlation between pattern components and insect's course becomes prominent.
A comparison was made between these reactions to stationary patterns and to turning patterns. No differences could be found in the behavioural reactions to the different situations. This suggests that the insect does not use an internal representation of its spontaneous turning tendency to discriminate between the type of turning of the optical environment. These results can be explained by a simple feedback control loop with an additive interaction between the internal turning command and feedback signals from the eyes.  相似文献   

10.
The walking behaviour of the beetle Tenebrio molitor was studied on a compensating running globe under optical stimuli. The animals show a clear frequency distribution for the duration of continuous walking between abrupt stops. The turning behaviour after a light is switched on or off is correlated with these frequency distributions. The circadian rhythmic changes of activity and of the sensitivity of the eyes influence the turning behaviour. A model describes the phenomenon of ‘sensory storage’ as the result of an integration over the reaction probabilities given by the frequency distributions.  相似文献   

11.
Characterisation of the arrestment responses of Trichogramma evanescens   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Summary Contact kairomones and oviposition in a host egg stimulated arrestment behaviour in Trichogramma evanescens, characterised by a reduction in walking speed and increased turning. Previous oviposition experience did not influence a parasitoid's response to contact kairomones, but successive encounters with kairomone patches resulted in parasitoids habituating to the contact chemical. Oviposition on a kairomone patch did not reverse this habituation effect. It was concluded that contact kairomones and host eggs will both contribute independently to the duration of a patch visit. The selection of patches by T. evanescens will depend on its response to kairomones. Results from this study indicate that the application of contact kairomones to field crops will not necessarily increase the probability of parasitoids finding hosts.  相似文献   

12.
ABSTRACT. The zigzagging behaviour of male Plodia interpunctella flying up a plume of sex pheromone was investigated in a horizontal wind tunnel by detailed analysis of the moths' ground tracks, groundspeeds, orientations and airspeeds. The moths ‘homed in’ on the source of the pheromone plume by progressively reducing airspeed and turning more into wind, thereby reducing groundspeed and the distance between track reversals and so narrowing down their zigzags (Fig. 16). Track angles and times between reversals were unaffected. Removing the wind-borne pheromone plume while a moth was flying along it confirmed that zigzagging can be an anemotactic response to losing the scent rather than a chemotactic response to the plume. For the first 1–2 s after the moth entered pheromone-free air the zigzagging was indistinguishable from that shown when the plume remained; thereafter it widened progressively until the moths were flying to and fro at c. 90° to the wind. The after-effect of odour stimulation persisted for many zigs and zags and many seconds (Figs. 4 and 5). Moths flying along pheromone plumes compensated efficiently for differences of wind speed, showing similar distributions of track angles to wind, and of ground-speeds, in winds of 0.1, 0.2 and 0.3 ms-1 (Figs. 12 and 13). Groundspeed varied with track angle to wind and this relationship was also similar in the three wind speeds (Fig. 14). This constancy of track angles and groundspeeds was due to the moths both increasing their airspeeds and turning more into wind at the higher wind speeds (Fig. 17). Thus the direction of the apparent movement of the ground pattern beneath the moths varied with wind speed. It is inferred that the moths, although unable to sense the wind directly, are able to compensate for changes in wind speed by integrating the wind-dependent optomotor input with information about their own airspeed, or with information about their own turning movements. Maintaining some ‘preferred’ relationship between these inputs by adjustments of orientation and airspeed, would then serve to maintain a given combination of track angle and groundspeed independently of wind speed. The preferred relationship is repeatedly re-set by the changing olfactory input from the pheromone plume, which also controls the switching between left and right of the upwind direction.  相似文献   

13.
R. A. Chylla  G. Garab  J. Whitmarsh 《BBA》1987,894(3):562-571
We used two different techniques to measure the recovery time of Photosystem II following the transfer of a single electron from P-680 to QA in thylakoid membranes isolated from spinach. Electron transfer in Photosystem II reaction centers was probed first by spectroscopic measurements of the electrochromic shift at 518 nm due to charge separation within the reaction centers. Using two short actinic flashes separated by a variable time interval we determined the time required after the first flash for the electrochromic shift at 518 nm to recover to the full extent on the second flash. In the second technique the redox state of QA at variable times after a saturating flash was monitored by measurement of the fluorescence induction in the absence of an inhibitor and in the presence of ferricyanide. The objective was to determine the time required after the actinic flash for the fluorescence induction to recover to the value observed after a 60 s dark period. Measurements were done under conditions in which (1) the electron donor for Photosystem II was water and the acceptor was the endogenous plastoquinone pool, and (2) Q400, the Fe2+ near QA, remained reduced and therefore was not a participant in the flash-induced electron-transfer reactions. The electrochromic shift at 518 nm and the fluorescence induction revealed a prominent biphasic recovery time for Photosystem II reaction centers. The majority of the Photosystem II reaction centers recovered in less than 50 ms. However, approx. one-third of the Photosystem II reaction centers required a half-time of 2–3 s to recover. Our interpretation of these data is that Photosystem II reaction centers consist of at least two distinct populations. One population, typically 68% of the total amount of Photosystem II as determined by the electrochromic shift, has a steady-state turnover rate for the electron-transfer reaction from water to the plastoquinone pool of approx. 250 e / s, sufficiently rapid to account for measured rates of steady-state electron transport. The other population, typically 32%, has a turnover rate of approx. 0.2 e / s. Since this turnover rate is over 1000-times slower than normally active Photosystem II complexes, we conclude that the slowly turning over Photosystem II complexes are inconsequential in contributing to energy transduction. The slowly turning over Photosystem II complexes are able to transfer an electron from P-680 to QA rapidly, but the reoxidation of QA is slow (t1/2 = 2 s). The fluorescence induction measurements lead us to conclude that there is significant overlap between the slowly turning over fraction of Photosystem II complexes and PS IIβ reaction centers. One corollary of this conclusion is that electron transfer from P-680 to QA in PS IIβ reaction centers results in charge separation across the membrane and gives rise to an electrochromic shift.  相似文献   

14.
Drosophila melanogaster is able to detect a small visual object hidden in a background of identical texture, as long as there is relative motion between their retinal images. The properties of figure-ground discrimination in the walking fly are studied under experimental conditions where the positions of figure and ground oscillate sinusoidally with similar frequency and similar amplitude but with different phase. The following points have been established. (a) The average turning reaction of the stationarily walkingDrosophila depends on phase; contrary to results obtained with the flyingMusca (Reichardt and Poggio, 1979), antiphasic oscillation of figure and ground does not suppress the attrativeness of the figure. (b) A translatory response has been found which also depends on the phase difference of the oscillatory movements of figure and ground. (c) The time course of the responses and its intra- and inter-individual variability do not seem to fit into a rigid model of figure-ground discrimination.  相似文献   

15.
Trichoblasts ofGibasis geniculata andTradescantia, fluminensis were observedin vivo under the polarizing microscope with the first-order red plate of a retardation of 530 mμ. It was revealed that the cell wall’s birefringence in plane view was negative with respect to the apicobasal axis of the cell in young cells before elongation, turning positive after elongation. Birefringence has never been observed when the cellular polarity was disturbed by colchicine. Therefore, it seems that the cellular polarity of root hair formation is controlled by anisotropic structures of the cell wall.  相似文献   

16.
Summary Seven fully excavated 16 year-old root systems of Sitka spruce were analysed. All roots in excess of 1 cm diameter at their origin on the stumps were analysed, data being collected until root diameter declined to less than 0.5 cm.Root morphology and distribution was identified as a balance between systematic biological mechanisms and their disruption by environmental factors, particularly changes of soil density and soil surface contours. The biological mechanisms have been modelled and the model is capable of simulating root systems in response to a few simple input variablese.g. the number of roots originating at stumps, stem ratius, total number of roots of all ordersetc.Additionally equations have been developed to estimate the distribution of root diameter, and root weight at all distances from tree stems and a similar equation permits the estimation of tree diameter at chosen heights. These latter estimates being utilized to calculate the turning moment of stems when blown by the wind.The influence of the wind on turning moment is explored for simulated root systems of differing strength and gross morphology.  相似文献   

17.
The responses of Eretmocerussp. and Encarsia deserti(Gerling & Rivnay), parasitoids of Bemisia tabaci(Gennadius) to host secretions were investigated. Upon contact with honeydew excreted from the host, the parasitoids' walking speed decreased, whereas the rate of angular turning increased (the walking speed of Eretmocerussp. and Encarsia desertidecreased from 2.0 to 0.8 and from 0.8 to 0.3 mm/s, respectively, and the respective angular turning rates increased from 123.3 to 489.6 and from 267.9 to 979.2 deg/cm). Moreover, the wasps generally turned back toward the secretion as soon as they lost contact with it. This induced arrestment responses resulted in increased searching time by the parasitoids. Laboratory bioassays were conducted in which paper disks in petri dishes containing different amounts of honeydew, equivalent to the secretions of 1, 5, or 50 larvae, were offered to female wasps. The wasps responded by arrestment even to honeydew produced by a single larva. While the response of Eretmocerussp. was quantity dependent, being more intense at higher amounts of honeydew, that of E. desertiwas of equal intensity at all honeydew quantities assayed.  相似文献   

18.
类胡萝卜素裂解双加氧酶(carotenoid cleavage dioxygenase, CCD)家族成员能催化类胡萝卜素裂解生成挥发性芳香物质并参与植物激素的合成。为探究茶树CsCCD基因家族成员生物学功能及基因表达模式,采用生物信息学手段进行了茶树全基因组CsCCD基因家族成员的鉴定,预测分析了其基因结构、保守基序、染色体定位、蛋白的理化性质、进化特性、互作网络、启动子顺式作用元件,并通过RT-qPCR测定了茶树不同叶位、乌龙茶加工过程中光照处理下CsCCD的相对表达量。共鉴定出11个茶树CsCCD基因家族成员,含有1–11个外显子、0–10个内含子不等;平均氨基酸个数为519 aa,平均分子质量为57 643.35 Da;聚类分析显示,CCD1、CCD4、CCD7、CCD8和NCED5个亚族各自聚成一类。茶树CsCCD基因家族主要含有胁迫响应元件、激素响应元件、光响应元件与多因素响应元件,且以光响应元件最多(142个)。进一步对茶树CsCCD基因在茶树不同叶位及乌龙茶加工过程中LED补光晾青过程的表达模式分析发现,CsCCD1、CsCCD4在成熟叶中表达量高于嫩叶及嫩茎,且随做青...  相似文献   

19.
This study examined the stimulus control of feeding in the carnivorous grasshopper mouse, Onychomys leucogaster. Quantitative analysis of the actions involved in feeding showed that overall feeding behaviors of lab-reared and field-caught mice resembled one another but that the sequence of specific actions was variable. Responses by mice to cotton swabs dipped in solutions showed that after eating several crickets, the presence of prey odor more readily elicited a feeding response. The odor and moisture cues associated with exposed viscera were shown by the use of modified prey to strongly affect the orientation of feeding. Using modified prey without exposed viscera, tactile cues associated with the head and thorax proved to be important guides in the feeding response. Feeding orientation towards the head was independent of that towards the thorax. Together these data showed that the feeding sequence resembled a reaction chain and that much of the stereotypy of the mouse's feeding response resulted from the position and alignment of the cues that orient the feeding response.  相似文献   

20.
Summary In tethered flying house-flies (Musca domestica) visually induced turning reactions were monitored under open-loop conditions simultaneously with the spike activity of four types of steering muscles (M.b1, M.b2, M.I1, M.III1). Specific behavioral response components are attributed to the activity of particular muscles. Compensatory optomotor turning reactions to large-field image displacements mainly occur when the stimulus pattern oscillates at low frequencies. In contrast, turning responses towards objects are preferentially induced by motion of relatively small stimuli at high oscillation frequencies. The different steering muscles seem to be functionally specialized in that they contribute to the control of these behavioral responses in different ways. The muscles I1, III1 and b2 are preferentially active during small-field motion at high oscillation frequencies. They are much less active during small-field motion at low oscillation frequencies and large-field motion at all oscillation frequencies which were tested. M.b2 is most extreme in this respect. These steering muscles thus mediate mainly turns towards objects. In contrast, M.b1 responds best during large-field motion at low oscillation frequencies and, thus, is appropriate to control compensatory optomotor responses. However, the activity of this muscle is also strongly modulated during small-field motion at high oscillation frequencies and, therefore, may be involved also in the control of turns towards objects. These functional specializations of the different steering muscles in mediating different behavioral response components are related to the properties of two parallel visual pathways that are selectively tuned to large-field and small-field motion, respectively.Abbreviations FD (cell) figure detection (cell) - HS (cell) horizontal (cell)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号