共查询到14条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Habitat preferences of the secretive forest buffalo (Syncerus caffer nanus) in Central Africa 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
The forest buffalo Syncerus caffer nanus is one of the three subspecies of African buffalo inhabiting the rainforests of Western and Central Africa. Because of its secretive behaviour and main habitat (dense rainforests), there is little quantitative information on the habitat preferences of this buffalo. We present here the first data on the frequencies of this species along a habitat gradient ranging from clearings and rivers to forests, as well as the characteristics of the buffalo's resting places. We recorded information from a buffalo herd during the period January 2002–January 2004 in the Bai-Hokou area (Dzanga-Ndoki National Park, Central African Republic). Resting places were firstly compared with available habitat (i.e. resting vs. random sites) and, successively, comparisons were made between diurnal versus nocturnal and wet versus dry season resting places. Forest buffalos were found to be highly dependent on clearings, as well as on the more open forest stands, characterized by large trees and open canopy. Such preferences could be due to the tendency of the buffalos to rest all together; open patches are likely to facilitate social interactions between the members of the herd. 相似文献
2.
3.
4.
Sharon L. Deem Fiona Maisels Richard G. Robbins 《European Journal of Wildlife Research》2005,51(1):60-62
A field necropsy was performed on a recently dead adult forest buffalo, Syncerus caffer nanus, found in the Nouabalé-Ndoki National Park, Republic of Congo. Based on the gross and histological lesions, the cause of death was determined to be conspecific fighting. Ectoparasites collected from this forest buffalo were Amblyomma sp. (larvae) and Rhipicephalus ziemanni Neumann. Endoparasites collected included a paramphistomid trematode, Carmyerius gregarius, and oocysts of Eimeria spp., paramphistomid eggs (presumably from C. gregarius), metastrongylid lungworm larvae, and strongylid eggs. Parasitic findings from this forest buffalo were compared to previous reports from savanna and forest buffalo. 相似文献
5.
6.
Most analyses of the relationship between group size and foodintake of social carnivores have shown a discrepancy betweenthe group size that maximizes energy intake and that which ismost frequently observed. Around southern Vancouver Island,British Columbia, killer whales of the so-called transient formforage in small groups, and appear to prey exclusively on marinemammals. Between 1986 and 1993, in approximately 434 h of observationson transient killer whales, we observed 138 attacks on fivespecies of marine mammals. Harbor seals were most frequentlyattacked (130 occasions), and the observed average energy intakerate was more than sufficient for the whale's energetic needs.Energy intake varied with group size, with groups of three havingthe highest energy intake rate per individual. While groupsof three were most frequently encountered, the group size experiencedby an average individual in the population (i.e., typical groupsize) is larger than three. However, comparisons between observedand expected group sizes should utilize only groups engagedin the behavior of interest. The typical size of groups consistingonly of adult and subadult whales that were engaged primarilyin foraging activities confirms that these individuals are foundin groups that are consistent with the maximization of energyintake hypothesis. Larger groups may form for (1) the occasionalhunting of prey other than harbor seals, for which the optimalforaging group size is probably larger than three; and (2) theprotection of calves and other social functions. Key words:dispersal, foraging, group hunting, harbor seals, killer whales,optimal group size, social structure. [Behav Ecol 7: 408-416(1996)] 相似文献
7.
When to use social information: the advantage of large group size in individual decision making
下载免费PDF全文

Correct decision making is crucial for animals to maximize foraging success and minimize predation risk. Group-living animals can make such decisions by using their own personal information or by pooling information with other group members (i.e. social information). Here, we investigate how individuals might best balance their use of personal and social information. We use a simple modelling approach in which individual decisions based upon social information are more likely to be correct when more individuals are involved and their personal information is more accurate. Our model predicts that when the personal information of group members is poor (accurate less than half the time), individuals should avoid pooling information. In contrast, when personal information is reliable (accurate at least half the time), individuals should use personal information less often and social information more often, and this effect should grow stronger in larger groups. One implication of this pattern is that social information allows less well-informed members of large groups to reach a correct decision with the same probability as more well-informed members of small groups. Thus, animals in larger groups may be able to minimize the costs of collecting personal information without impairing their ability to make correct decisions. 相似文献
8.
Phylogenetic analysis of coadaptation in behavior, diet, and body size in the African antelope 总被引:2,自引:3,他引:2
Several authors have suggested that African antelope (familyBovidae) exemplify coadaptation of ecological, behavioral,and morphological traits. We tested four hypotheses relatedto the ecology and behavior of 75 species of African antelopeusing both conventional statistical techniques and techniques
that account for the nonindependence of species by consideringtheir phylogenetic relationships. Specifically, we tested thehypotheses that (1) dietary selectivity is correlated negativelywith body mass, (2) dietary selectivity is correlated negativelywith group size, (3) gregarious species either flee or counterattackwhen approached by predators, but solitary and pair-livingspecies seek cover to hide, and (4) body mass and group sizeare correlated positively. Each of these hypotheses was examinedfor the global data set (family Bovidae) and, when possible,within the two antelope subfamilies (Antilopinae and Bovinae)and within 7 of the 10 antelope tribes. The results of ourconventional and phylogenetically corrected analyses supportedthe hypotheses that group and body size vary predictably with
feeding style and that antipredator behavior varies with groupsize. The hypothesis that body mass and group size are correlatedpositively was supported by conventional statistics, but thesetwo traits were only weakly related using a phylogeneticallycorrected analysis. Moreover, qualitative and quantitativecomparisons within each of the eight major African antelope
tribes generally gave little support for the four hypothesestested. Thus, although our analyses at the subfamily levelprovided results that were consistent with prior hypotheses,our analyses at the level of tribes were equivocal. We discussseveral possible explanations for these differences. 相似文献
9.
10.
Wildfires may be started naturally by lightning or artificially by humans. In the savanna regions of southern Africa, lightning fires tend to occur at the start of the wet season, during October and November, while anthropogenic fires are usually started during the dry season, between July and August. A long-term field manipulation experiment initiated in the Kruger National Park in 1952 was used to explore whether this seasonal divergence affects tree abundance, spatial pattern, size and architecture. After 44 years of prescribed burning treatments that simulated the seasonal incidence of lightning and anthropogenic fires, mean densities of the locally-dominant shrub, Colophospermum mopane, were 638 and 500 trees ha–1 respectively. Trees in burnt plots had aggregated distributions while trees in unburnt plots had random distributions. Significant differences (p < 0.001) were recorded in a range of morphological parameters including tree height, canopy diameter, mean stem circumference and number of stems. The incidence of resprouting also differed significantly between treatments, with burnt trees containing a high proportion of coppiced stems. The differences in tree size and architecture between the mid-dry season and early-wet season burning plots suggest that anthropogenic fires applied during July and August cannot substitute for a natural lightning fire regime. Anthropogenic fire yields a landscape that is shorter, more scrubby and populated by numerous coppiced shrubs than the landscape generated by natural lightning fire conditions. 相似文献
11.
12.
M. M. MARIETTE S. R. K. ZAJITSCHEK C. M. GARCIA R. C. BROOKS 《Journal of evolutionary biology》2010,23(8):1772-1782
Abstract In recent years, it has become evident that frequency dependence in the attractiveness of a particular phenotype to mates can contribute to the maintenance of polymorphism. However, these preferences for rare and unfamiliar male phenotypes have only been demonstrated in small, controlled experiments. Here, we tested the preference for unfamiliar mates in groups of six to 96 individuals over 13 days, in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). We observed individual behaviour in situ to test whether fish discriminate two unfamiliar individuals among many familiar ones. We found that unfamiliar males and females were preferred over the familiar fishes in all groups and that this effect decayed over time. Increasing group sizes and levels of sexual activity did not hamper the preference for unfamiliar mates, providing further support for the role of frequency dependent mate choice in the maintenance of trait polymorphism in natural populations. 相似文献
13.
Naofumi Nakagawa 《International journal of primatology》1992,13(1):73-96
The distribution of four affiliative behaviors (proximity within 3 m, allogrooming, contact calling, and co-night-resting)
were examined in a group of wild patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) during a nonmating, nonbirth season. To a greater or lesser extent, dominance rank and kinship influenced these behaviors
of the adult females. Since high-ranking females tended to exhibit some of these behaviors with high frequency, they were
considered to be acting as the focus of affiliative behaviors and as the center for group cohesion. Furthermore, related adult
females also tended to exhibit some of these behaviors with a high frequency toward one another, so that matrilineal kinship
was also seen to be an important factor for group cohesion. In contrast, the harem male tended to exhibit these behaviors
at a low frequency and/or had no affiliative partner for any of them. Thus, it appears that the social organization of the
patas group is concentric, being composed of high-ranking females in the center, low-ranking females at the periphery, and
the harem male at the distant periphery. 相似文献