共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
R. Stepansky B. Holzinger A. Schmeiser-Rieder B. Saletu M. Kunze J. Zeitlhofer 《Dreaming》1998,8(1):23-30
This study was based on a survey of a representative sample of 1000 Austrians who were questioned about their sleep and dream behavior. About two-thirds of the respondents reported that they generally recalled at least one dream per month. Dream recall frequency decreased with advancing age, but did not differ between men and women. Fifty-five percent of the respondents characterized the affective content of their dreams: 29% reported neutral, 20% positive, and 6% negative dreams. Four percent of the sample reported suffering from nightmares. These respondents more frequently reported snoring, interrupted sleep, daytime somnolence, anxiety and nervousness, depression, high dream recall, recurrent dreams, and dreaming in color. Twenty-six percent of the total sample reported that sometimes they realized during their dreams that they were dreaming. These respondents more frequently reported family problems, high dream recall, positive dream content, recurrent dreams, dreaming in color, and nightmares. 相似文献
2.
Aydin D Feychting M Schüz J Andersen TV Poulsen AH Prochazka M Klaeboe L Kuehni CE Tynes T Röösli M 《Bioelectromagnetics》2011,32(5):396-407
Whether the use of mobile phones is a risk factor for brain tumors in adolescents is currently being studied. Case--control studies investigating this possible relationship are prone to recall error and selection bias. We assessed the potential impact of random and systematic recall error and selection bias on odds ratios (ORs) by performing simulations based on real data from an ongoing case--control study of mobile phones and brain tumor risk in children and adolescents (CEFALO study). Simulations were conducted for two mobile phone exposure categories: regular and heavy use. Our choice of levels of recall error was guided by a validation study that compared objective network operator data with the self-reported amount of mobile phone use in CEFALO. In our validation study, cases overestimated their number of calls by 9% on average and controls by 34%. Cases also overestimated their duration of calls by 52% on average and controls by 163%. The participation rates in CEFALO were 83% for cases and 71% for controls. In a variety of scenarios, the combined impact of recall error and selection bias on the estimated ORs was complex. These simulations are useful for the interpretation of previous case-control studies on brain tumor and mobile phone use in adults as well as for the interpretation of future studies on adolescents. 相似文献
3.
Ventura AK Loken E Mitchell DC Smiciklas-Wright H Birch LL 《Obesity (Silver Spring, Md.)》2006,14(6):1073-1084
Objective: This study describes patterns of bias in self‐reported dietary recall data of girls by examining differences among girls classified as under‐reporters, plausible reporters, and over‐reporters on weight, dietary patterns, and psychosocial characteristics. Research Methods and Procedures: Participants included 176 girls at age 11 and their parents. Girls’ weight and height were measured. Three 24‐hour dietary recalls and responses to psychosocial measures were collected. Plausibility cut‐offs for reported energy intake as a percentage of predicted energy requirements were used to divide the sample into under‐reporters, plausible reporters, and over‐reporters. Differences among these three groups on dietary and psychosocial variables were assessed to examine possible sources of bias in reporting. Results: Using a ±1 standard deviation cut‐off for energy intake plausibility, 50% of the sample was categorized as plausible reporters, 34% as under‐reporters, and 16% as over‐reporters. Weight status of under‐reporters was significantly higher than that of plausible reporters and over‐reporters. With respect to reported dietary intake, under‐reporters were no different from plausible reporters on intakes of foods with higher nutrient densities and lower energy densities and were significantly lower than plausible reporters on intakes of foods with lower nutrient densities and higher energy densities. Over‐reporters reported significantly higher intakes of all food groups and the majority of subgroups, relative to plausible reporters. Under‐reporters had significantly higher levels of weight concern and dietary restraint than both plausible reporters and over‐reporters. Discussion: Techniques to categorize plausible and implausible reporters can and should be used to provide an improved understanding of the nature of error in children's dietary intake data and account for this error in analysis and interpretation. 相似文献
4.
Objective: To identify the determinants of underreporting BMI and to evaluate the possibilities of using self‐reported data for valid obesity prevalence rate estimations. Research Methods and Procedures: A cross‐sectional monitoring health survey was carried out between 1998 and 2002, and a review of published studies was performed. A total of 1809 men and 1882 women ages 20 to 59 years from The Netherlands were included. Body weight and height were reported and measured. Equations were calculated to estimate individuals’ BMI from reported data. These equations and equations from published studies were applied to the present data to evaluate whether using these equations led to valid estimations of the obesity prevalence rate. Also, size of underestimation of obesity prevalence rate was compared between studies. Results: The prevalence of obesity was underestimated by 26.1% and 30.0% among men and women, respectively, when based on reported data. The most important determinant of underreporting BMI was a high BMI. When equations to calculate individuals’ BMI from reported data were used, the obesity prevalence rate was still underestimated by 12.9% and 8.1% of the “true” obesity prevalence rate among men and women, respectively. The degree of underestimating the obesity prevalence was inconsistent across studies. Applying equations from published studies to the present data led to estimations of the obesity prevalence varying from a 7% overestimation to a 74% underestimation. Discussion: Valuable efforts for monitoring and evaluating prevention and treatment studies require direct measurements of body weight and height. 相似文献
5.
The aim of this study was to examine the frequency of dream recall in Chinese people and to develop nonintrusive procedures to facilitate dream recall. Guidelines for remembering dreams (dream recall instructions) were established based on a review of the existing literature on dream recall and its associated factors. The efficacy of the dream recall experience was explored using this procedure in neurologically healthy individuals. One hundred seventy Chinese participants were included in the study: 100 received the dream recall instructions, and 70 did not. The recall instructions were shown to be effective in triggering dream recall in the participants. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved) 相似文献
6.
Despite its resurgence within toxicology and, specifically, risk assessment, the concept of hormesis remains peripheral to current epidemiological practice. In this paper we examine some reasons for this, focusing on applications within occupational and environmental epidemiology. Unclear in the existing literature is whether hormesis pertains to a single biological mechanism or response, or the aggregate effect of all correlates of exposure. Although J-shaped and U-shaped relationships between risk factors and disease endpoints have been identified epidemiologically, it is unclear whether such patterns reflect biological hormesis or a combination of factors resulting in a hormetic-looking relationship. Given the potential importance of assessing hormetic responses in epidemiological studies, we identify and discuss key limitations of epidemiology in validly detecting and interpreting hormesis. For example, most observational occupational and environmental studies lack the ability to determine the dose received by each individual, and therefore poor surrogates of exposure are frequently used, potentially introducing considerable systematic and random error. Further, because exposure is not randomly assigned to humans, the potential for confounding is great. Finally, using a simple simulation to assess the impact of ignoring hormesis in the analysis of epidemiological data containing mild hormesis, we demonstrate a resulting “hormetic bias,” in which relative risks at exposure levels above the hormetic region are systematically overestimated. 相似文献
7.
8.
9.
The tendency for the onset of a genetic disease to occur at progressively earlier ages or with progressively greater severity in successive generations is known as anticipation. Following the discovery of trinucleotide repeat expansion as a plausible genetic mechanism for anticipation, interest in testing for anticipation has increased. Studies of anticipation can be biased when parents with late onset or children with early onset are preferentially ascertained. This paper presents a nonparametric approach to testing for age-at-onset anticipation that adjusts for such preferential ascertainment. The approach is illustrated through application to data on panic disorder. 相似文献
10.
Richard M. Pace III Rob Williams Scott D. Kraus Amy R. Knowlton Heather M. Pettis 《Conservation Science and Practice》2021,3(2):e346
Evaluations of the conservation status of the endangered North Atlantic right whale as well as many other wildlife species often rely extensively on counts and cause-of-death determinations of carcasses found accidentally or during dedicated surveys. Even when survey effort dedicated to a population is extensive, many deaths may go unseen. We used an abundance estimation model to derive estimates of cryptic mortality for North Atlantic right whales and found that observed carcasses accounted for only 36% of all estimated death during 1990–2017. We found strong evidence that total mortality varied over time, and that observed carcass counts were poor predictors of estimated annual numbers of whales dying. Importantly, there were substantial differences between fractions of deaths determined to be entanglement related during necropsy (49%) and the fraction of cryptic deaths suffering serious injuries related to entanglement (87%). Although we concluded that a single year's observations produced poor estimates of carcass detection rates due to the volatility of ratios of small counts, ratio estimates of data pooled over periods of consistent survey may offer better information on detection rates. Additionally, it appears unwise to consider cause of death determinations from detected carcasses as representative of cause-specific mortality rates in right whales given the large number of seriously injured whales from entanglement that are likely part of the unseen mortality. 相似文献
11.
抗生素在传染性疾病预防和治疗方面发挥着重要作用,但抗生素的滥用导致微生物在选择性压力作用下获得并维持耐药性。碳青霉烯类抗生素是一类用于治疗多重耐药菌感染的重要抗菌药物,曾被认为是治疗革兰氏阴性菌严重感染的最后一道防线。但随着碳青霉烯酶的出现,碳青霉烯类耐药革兰氏阴性菌的检出率在全球范围内迅速增长,严重威胁着全球的公共卫生安全。本综述重点介绍了碳青霉烯类耐药细菌的全球流行病学和临床相关耐药基因的检测方法,为合理使用抗生素、有效控制耐药性传播提供参考。 相似文献
12.
Kh. Ansarin L. Sahebi Y. Aftabi M. Khalili M. Seyyedi 《Journal of applied microbiology》2020,129(4):1062-1070
13.
14.
Norovirus (NoV) and human astrovirus (HAstV) are important causative agents of acute gastroenteritis in children and adults. They are comprised of multiple genotypes and attention should be paid to genotype changes or emergence of new genetic variants. To study the prevalence and diversity of NoV GI, GII, and HAstV circulating in eastern China, we conducted a three-year environmental surveillance in a coastal city of Yantai. Thirty-six sewage samples were collected, processed, and examined for the presence of viral genomes by PCR. The results showed that NoV GI, GII, and HAstV were detected in all 36 samples. Six NoV GI genotypes, 11 NoV GII genotypes, and 5 HAstV serotypes were identified; GI.6, GII.17, and HAstV-5 were the most prevalent types, respectively. Persistent existence of NoV GII.17 Kawasaki 308 variant was observed during whole study period. Phylogenetic analysis reflected multiple transmission lineages in local population for both viruses. Our results reflect continuous presence of enteric viruses in sewage, improve our understanding on their molecular epidemiology, and demonstrate surveillance on sewage is an effective approach in understanding the local circulation of enteric viruses. 相似文献
15.
16.
In order to investigate the relationship between dream recall frequency and personality, 116 college undergraduates kept a dream log for 21 consecutive nights and completed self-report measures assessing fantasy-proneness, psychological absorption, and imaginative involvement. Consistent with most previous literature in this area, with one exception, there were no significant associations found between dream recall and the personality measures. The one exception to this pattern was for fantasy proneness and this correlation was of a small magnitude and only obtained for women. We conclude that dream recall frequency is largely independent from stable personality traits and can better be understood in terms of expectancy and attitudinal factors. 相似文献
17.
In its basic form, the Will Rogers phenomenon takes place when an increase in the average value of each of two sets is achieved by moving an element from one set to another. This leads to the conclusion that there has been an improvement, when in fact essentially nothing has changed. Extended versions of this phenomenon can occur in epidemiological studies, rendering their results unreliable. After describing epidemiological and clinical studies that have been affected by the Will Rogers phenomenon, this paper presents a simple method to correct for it. The method involves introducing a transition matrix between the two sets and taking probability weighted expectations. Two real-world biometrical examples, based on migration economics and breast cancer epidemiology, are given and improvements against a naïve analysis are demonstrated. In the cancer epidemiology example, we take account of estimation uncertainty. We also discuss briefly some limitations associated with our method. 相似文献
18.
19.
ROSEMARIE TEDESCHI CRISTINA VISENTIN ALBERTO ALMA DOMENICO BOSCO 《The Annals of applied biology》2003,142(3):285-290
Adults of Cacopsylla melanoneura, vector of the apple proliferation (AP) phytoplasma, were collected every 2 weeks from January until May in 2000 and 2001 by the beating tray method in eight apple orchards of the Aosta Valley (northwestern Italy). Total DNA was extracted from batches of five insects and amplified with the universal phytoplasma primers P1/P7 in direct PCR. A nested PCR assay was then performed on P1/P7 amplicons using the primers fO1/rO1, specific for the AP‐ phytoplasma group. The digestion of fO1/rO1 amplicons with Ssp I restriction endonuclease confirmed that C. melanoneura adults harboured the AP phytoplasma. The data obtained with PCR were used to estimate the proportion of AP‐positive insects in over wintered and offspring adults. Percentages of AP‐positive insects of 3.6% and 0.8% were estimated in 2000 among over wintered and offspring psyllids respectively. In 2001 only the over wintered insects were found infected, with an estimated proportion of 2.8%. The seasonal abundance of the vector was measured using yellow sticky traps. C. melanoneura was always present at a low population level, and the highest density was recorded from mid‐February until mid‐March in both years. The results show that the overwintered population is higher and spends a longer period in apple orchards, suggesting the crucial role of the overwintered adults in vectoring AP. 相似文献
20.
Caitlin Karniski Eric M. Patterson Ewa Krzyszczyk Vivienne Foroughirad Margaret A. Stanton Janet Mann 《Marine Mammal Science》2015,31(3):839-852
Activity budget data are essential for determining behavioral responses to physiological and ecological variables. Yet, few studies are available to investigate the robustness, accuracy, and biases of the methods used to estimate activity budgets for cetaceans. In this study, we compare activity budgets of 55 adult female bottlenose dolphins in Shark Bay, Australia derived from two methods: surveys (n = 6,903) and focal follows (n = 1,185, totaling 2,721 h of observation). Activity budgets estimated from survey data differed in all behavioral states compared to focal follow data. However, when controlling for temporal autocorrelation, only time spent socializing and time spent traveling remained disparate between the methods. To control for biases associated with assigning group‐level behavior to individuals, we also compared survey and focal follow activity budgets for lone females. Here we found differences between methods in time spent foraging and traveling regardless of whether we controlled for temporal autocorrelation, which suggests detection biases likely play a role in explaining differences in activity budget estimates between the two methodologies. Our results suggest that surveys are less representative of individual‐level activity budgets, and thus, when individual‐level knowledge about behavior is needed, focal follows are preferred. 相似文献