首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
The Euclidean and MAX metrics have been widely used to model cue summation psychophysically and computationally. Both rules happen to be special cases of a more general Minkowski summation rule , where m = 2 and ∞, respectively. In vision research, Minkowski summation with power m = 3-4 has been shown to be a superior model of how subthreshold components sum to give an overall detection threshold. Recently, we have previously reported that Minkowski summation with power m = 2.84 accurately models summation of suprathreshold visual cues in photographs. In four suprathreshold discrimination experiments, we confirm the previous findings with new visual stimuli and extend the applicability of this rule to cue combination in auditory stimuli (musical sequences and phonetic utterances, where m = 2.95 and 2.54, respectively) and cross-modal stimuli (m = 2.56). In all cases, Minkowski summation with power m = 2.5-3 outperforms the Euclidean and MAX operator models. We propose that this reflects the summation of neuronal responses that are not entirely independent but which show some correlation in their magnitudes. Our findings are consistent with electrophysiological research that demonstrates signal correlations (r = 0.1-0.2) between sensory neurons when these are presented with natural stimuli.  相似文献   

3.
In this paper we propose a general variational segmentation model for multiphase texture segmentation based on fuzzy region competition principle. An important strength of the proposed framework is that different region terms (e.g. mutual information Kim et al. (2005) [1], local histogram Ni et al. (2009) [2] models for texture-based segmentation, and piecewise constant intensity model Chan and Vese (2001) [3] for intensity-based segmentation) can be included as appropriate to the problem. Constraints of different phases are considered by introducing Lagrangian multipliers into the energy functional, and a fast numerical solution is achieved by employing the fast dual projection algorithm Chambolle (2004) [4]. The proposed model has been applied to synthetic and natural images in order to make comparisons with other competing models in literature. Our results demonstrate superiority in dealing with multiphase texture segmentation problems. To demonstrate its usefulness in biomedical applications we have applied the new model to two retinal image segmentation problems: segmentation of capillary non-perfusion regions in fluorescein angiogram and segmentation of cellular layers of the retina in optical coherence tomography, and evaluated against the gold standard set by experts. The generalized overlap analysis shows good agreement for both applications. As a generic segmentation technique our new model has the potential to be extended for wider applications.  相似文献   

4.
5.
Redies C 《Spatial Vision》2007,21(1-2):97-117
Philosophers have pointed out that there is a close relation between the esthetics of art and the beauty of natural scenes. Supporting this similarity at the experimental level, we have recently shown that visual art and natural scenes share fractal-like, scale-invariant statistical properties. Moreover, evidence from neurophysiological experiments shows that the visual system uses an efficient (sparse) code to process optimally the statistical properties of natural stimuli. In the present work, a hypothetical model of esthetic perception is described that combines both lines of evidence. Specifically, it is proposed that an artist creates a work of art so that it induces a specific resonant state in the visual system. This resonant state is thought to be based on the adaptation of the visual system to natural scenes. The proposed model is universal and predicts that all human beings share the same general concept of esthetic judgment. The model implies that esthetic perception, like the coding of natural stimuli, depends on stimulus form rather than content, depends on higher-order statistics of the stimuli, and is non-intuitive to cognitive introspection. The model accommodates the central tenet of neuroesthetic theory that esthetic perception reflects fundamental functional properties of the nervous system.  相似文献   

6.
Action potential encoding in the cockroach tactile spine neuron can be represented as a single-input single-output nonlinear dynamic process. We have used a new functional expansion method to characterize the nonlinear behavior of the neural encoder. This method, which yields similar kernels to the Wiener method, is more accurate than the latter and is efficient enough to obtain reasonable kernels in less than 15 min using a personal computer. The input stimulus was band-limited white Gaussian noise and the output consisted of the resulting train of action potentials, which were unitized to give binary values. The kernels and the system input-output signals were used to identify a model for encoding comprising a cascade of dynamic linear, static nonlinear, and dynamic linear components. The two dynamic linear components had repeatable and distinctive forms with the first being low-pass and the second being high-pass. The static nonlinearity was fitted with a fifth-order polynomial function over several input amplitude ranges and had the form of a half-wave rectifier. The complete model gave a good approximation to the output of the neuron when both were subjected to the same novel white noise input signal.  相似文献   

7.
8.
Journal of Mathematical Biology - A general framework for threshold parameters in population dynamics is developed using the concept of target reproduction numbers. This framework identifies...  相似文献   

9.
10.
11.
How sensory organization for postural control matures in children is not clear at this time. The present study examined, in children aged 7 to 11 and in adults, the postural control modifications in quiet standing when somatosensory inputs from the ankle were disturbed. Since the reweighting of sensory inputs is not mature before 10, we hypothesized that postural stability was more affected in children than in adults when somatosensory inputs were altered and that this postural instability decreased as age increased during childhood. 37 children aged 7 to 11 years and 9 adults participated in the experiments. The postural task was a semi-tandem position with the right foot in front of the left one. Postural performance was measured by means of a force platform. Two experimental conditions were presented to the participants to maintain quiet standing: With or without altered somatosensory inputs (i.e., with or without ankles vibration). Results showed that postural stability--and thus how the reweighting process of the visual/somatosensory inputs matured--increased non-monotonically between 7 years of age and adult age: There was a linear improvement of postural stability from 7 to 10, followed by a more steady behaviour between 10 and 11 and then postural stability increased to reach the adults' level of performance.  相似文献   

12.
13.
Rowell CH 《Tissue & cell》1991,23(2):271-276
1. Thirty-seven pairs of mesothoracic interneurons respond selectively to visual or ocellar stimuli corresponding to deviations from course in flight, expressed as angular rotation around the three spatial axes. 2. Sensitivities to roll and yaw are very strongly associated. All interneurons showing a directional preference for yaw rotations showed the same preference for roll rotations. A few roll-sensitive cells were not directionally sensitive to yaw. Some interneurons respond exclusively to pitch rotations, most to both pitch and roll/yaw. 3. Approximately equal numbers of interneurons prefer pitch up, pitch down, roll/yaw to the ipsilateral side and roll/yaw to the contralateral side. All four possible combinations of pitch (up or down) with roll/yaw (ipsilateral or contralateral) preferences occur with equal probability. 4. No relationship between neuronal structure and directional properties could be discerned. 5. The average latency of the ocellar EPSPs recorded in the interneurons is not significantly different from the average latency of the ocellar spike in the descending neurons (at the same temperature and in the same ganglion). The average ocellar IPSP latency is 8.5 ms longer. The data support the hypothesis that most EPSPs are derived from monosynaptic inputs from the DNs, and most IPSPs from polysynaptic inputs. A few EPSPs are also derived from polysynaptic inputs. 6. Most of these neurons are sensitive to wind, at least some directionally so, in a manner functionally compatible with their visual or ocellar directionality, and most are excited. Two neurons respond to movement of small objects in the visual field, and 5 to high frequency sound.  相似文献   

14.
The EPs of the inferior colliculus and auditory cortex in anaesthetized guinea pigs and the long latency auditory EPs in alert humans were studied. The stimuli consisted of binaurally presented pairs of clicks used as a masker, and the probe, with a variable time delay between them. The greatest relative differences between out-of-phase and in-phase probe responses were observed at the beginning of the recovery course. They averaged as 1.6, 1.5 and 1.4 for the responses of the inferior colliculus, auditory cortex and long latency potentials, resp., at the stimuli intensities of 50-65 dB SPL, and then decreased to zero during the time course of the probe response recovery. Correlation of this parameter with the stimulus intensity was positive.  相似文献   

15.
The neural mechanisms for the perception of face and motion were studied using psychophysical threshold measurements, event-related potentials (ERPs), and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). A face-specific ERP component, N170, was recorded over the posterior temporal cortex. Removal of the high-spatial-frequency components of the face altered the perception of familiar faces significantly, and familiarity can facilitate the cortico-cortical processing of facial perceptions. Similarly, the high-spatial-frequency components of the face seemed to be crucial for the recognition of facial expressions. Aging and visuospatial impairments affected motion perception significantly. Two distinct components of motion ERPs, N170 and P200, were recorded over the parietal region. The former was related to horizontal motion perception while the latter reflected the perception of radial optic flow motion. The results of fMRI showed that horizontal movements of objects and radial optic flow motion were perceived differently in the V5/MT and superior parietal lobe. We conclude that an integrated approach can provide useful information on spatial and temporal processing of face and motion non-invasively.  相似文献   

16.
The human brain processes different aspects of the surrounding environment through multiple sensory modalities, and each modality can be subdivided into multiple attribute-specific channels. When the brain rebinds sensory content information (‘what’) across different channels, temporal coincidence (‘when’) along with spatial coincidence (‘where’) provides a critical clue. It however remains unknown whether neural mechanisms for binding synchronous attributes are specific to each attribute combination, or universal and central. In human psychophysical experiments, we examined how combinations of visual, auditory and tactile attributes affect the temporal frequency limit of synchrony-based binding. The results indicated that the upper limits of cross-attribute binding were lower than those of within-attribute binding, and surprisingly similar for any combination of visual, auditory and tactile attributes (2–3 Hz). They are unlikely to be the limits for judging synchrony, since the temporal limit of a cross-attribute synchrony judgement was higher and varied with the modality combination (4–9 Hz). These findings suggest that cross-attribute temporal binding is mediated by a slow central process that combines separately processed ‘what’ and ‘when’ properties of a single event. While the synchrony performance reflects temporal bottlenecks existing in ‘when’ processing, the binding performance reflects the central temporal limit of integrating ‘when’ and ‘what’ properties.  相似文献   

17.
With regular trains of stimuli at a high frequency, the contribution of each stimulus to the force generated over time declines from the second to about the tenth stimulus, but then begins to increase again. This late increase is referred to as tetanic potentiation in analogy with the post-tetanic potentiation of the twitch after such a period of stimulation. With regular trains of stimuli at a low frequency, a progressive decrease in the essentially unfused twitches (negative staircase) is observed in the slow soleus muscle of the cat, while a progressive increase (positive staircase) is observed for the fast plantaris muscle. The time constant for the approximately exponential changes observed is on the order of 10 s. Random trains of stimuli were applied at intermediate frequencies and analyzed in terms of general methods of analysis for nonlinear systems. Systematic decreases in the magnitude and increases in the time course of the average tension per stimulus were observed with increasing mean rates of stimulation. Similar changes were observed for short intervals between stimuli within a given random train at a constant mean rate. These changes can be described in terms of an early depression and a later facilitation described in the previous papers in this series.  相似文献   

18.
Richards (1985) showed that veridical three-dimensional shape may be recovered from the integration of binocular disparity and retinal motion information, but proposed that this integration may only occur for horizontal retinal motion. Psychophysical evidence supporting the combination of stereo and motion information is limited to the case of horizontal motion (Johnston et al., 1994), and has been criticised on the grounds of potential object boundary cues to shape present in the stimuli. We investigated whether veridical shape can be recovered under more general conditions. Observers viewed cylinders that were defined by binocular disparity, two-frame motion or a combination of disparity and motion, presented at simulated distances of 30 cm, 90 cm or 150 cm. Horizontally and vertically oriented cylinders were rotated about vertical and horizontal axes. When rotation was about the cylinder's own axis, no boundary cues to shape were introduced. Settings were biased for the disparity and two-frame motion stimuli, while more veridical shape judgements were made under all conditions for combined cue stimuli. These results demonstrate that the improved perception of three-dimensional shape in these stimuli is not a consequence of the presence of object boundary cues, and that the combination of disparity and motion is not restricted to horizontal image motion.  相似文献   

19.
Damage to motor neurons induces regeneration processes including axonal growth and change of synaptic properties. Sensory axons that run along the motor axons are also damaged, but their possible role in the motor neuron''s regeneration is generally ignored. Here, the effect of eliminating some sensory inputs from intact motor axons on the motor axon''s properties was studied. Micro-dissecting one of the segmental, bilateral, sensory stretch receptor pairs of the crayfish abdomen induced the deep extensor abdominal motor axons to grow and changed their synaptic properties. The results demonstrate directly, probably for the first time, that change in sensory neuron activity can induce motor axons to grow, form new synapses, and change their synaptic properties.  相似文献   

20.
MOTIVATION: In analyses of microarray data with a design of different biological conditions, ranking genes by their differential 'importance' is often desired so that biologists can focus research on a small subset of genes that are most likely related to the experiment conditions. Permutation methods are often recommended and used, in place of their parametric counterparts, due to the small sample sizes of microarray experiments and possible non-normality of the data. The recommendations, however, are based on classical knowledge in the hypothesis test setting. RESULTS: We explore the relationship between hypothesis testing and gene ranking. We indicate that the permutation method does not provide a metric for the distance between two underlying distributions. In our simulation studies permutation methods tend to be equally or less accurate than parametric methods in ranking genes. This is partially due to the discreteness of the permutation distributions, as well as the non-metric property. In data analysis the variability in ranking genes can be assessed by bootstrap. It turns out that the variability is much lower for permutation than parametric methods, which agrees with the known robustness of permutation methods to individual outliers in the data.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号