共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Soler Juan Jose; Soler Manuel; Perez-Contreras Tomas; Aragon Santiago; Moller Anders Pape 《Behavioral ecology》1999,10(6):707-713
Brood parasites dramatically reduce the reproductive successof their
hosts, which therefore have developed defenses againstbrood parasites. The
first line of defense is protecting thenest against adult parasites. When the
parasite has successfullyparasitized a host nest, some hosts are able to
recognize andreject the eggs of the brood parasite, which constitutes the
secondline of defense. Both defense tactics are costly and would be
counteractedby brood parasites. While a failure in nest defense implies
successfulparasitism and therefore great reduction of reproductive successof
hosts, a host that recognizes parasitic eggs has the opportunityto reduce the
effect of parasitism by removing the parasiticegg. We hypothesized that, when
nest defense is counteractedby the brood parasite, hosts that recognize
cuckoo eggs shoulddefend their nests at a lower level than nonrecognizers
becausethe former also recognize adult cuckoos. Magpie (Pica pica)
hoststhat rejected model eggs of the brood parasitic great spottedcuckoo
(Clamator glandarius) showed lower levels of nest defensewhen
exposed to a great spotted cuckoo than when exposed toa nest predator (a
carrion crow Corvus corone). Moreover, magpiesrejecting cuckoo eggs
showed lower levels of nest defense againstgreat spotted cuckoos than
nonrecognizer magpies, whereas differencesin levels of defense disappeared
when exposed to a carrion crow.These results suggest that hosts specialize in
antiparasitedefense and that different kinds of defense are antagonistically
expressed.We suggest that nest-defense mechanisms are ancestral, whereasegg
recognition and rejection is a subsequent stage in the coevolutionaryprocess.
However, host recognition ability will not be expressedwhen brood parasites
break this second line of defense. 相似文献
2.
Micro-evolutionary change and population dynamics of a brood parasite and its primary host: the intermittent arms race hypothesis 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Manuel Soler Juan J. Soler Juan G. Martinez Tomás Pérez-Contreras Anders P. Møller 《Oecologia》1998,117(3):381-390
A long-term study of the interactions between a brood parasite, the great spotted cuckoo Clamator glandarius, and its primary host the magpie Pica pica, demonstrated local changes in the distribution of both magpies and cuckoos and a rapid increase of rejection of both mimetic
and non-mimetic model eggs by the host. In rich areas, magpies improved three of their defensive mechanisms: nest density
and breeding synchrony increased dramatically and rejection rate of cuckoo eggs increased more slowly. A stepwise multiple
regression analysis showed that parasitism rate decreased as host density increased and cuckoo density decreased. A logistic
regression analysis indicated that the probability of changes in magpie nest density in the study plots was significantly
affected by the density of magpie nests during the previous year (positively) and the rejection rate of mimetic model eggs
(negatively). These results are consistent with a hypothesis (the intermittent arms race hypothesis) of spatially structured
cyclic changes in parasitism. During periods of parasitism, host defences continuously improve, and as a consequence, the
fitness gains for parasites decrease. When host defences against parasites reach a high level, dispersing parasites have a
selective advantage if they are able to emigrate to areas of low resistance. Once parasites have left an area hosts will lose
their defensive adaptations due to their cost in the absence of parasitism. The scene is then set for re-colonization by great
spotted cuckoos.
Received: 7 May 1998 / Accepted: 24 August 1998 相似文献
3.
Martín-Gálvez D Soler JJ Martínez JG Krupa AP Richard M Soler M Møller AP Burke T 《Journal of evolutionary biology》2006,19(2):543-550
Avian brood parasites reduce the reproductive output of their hosts and thereby select for defence mechanisms such as ejection of parasitic eggs. Such defence mechanisms simultaneously select for counter-defences in brood parasites, causing a coevolutionary arms race. Although coevolutionary models assume that defences and counter-defences are genetically influenced, this has never been demonstrated for brood parasites. Here, we give strong evidence for genetic differences between ejector and nonejectors, which could allow the study of such host defence at the genetic level, as well as studies of maintenance of genetic variation in defences. Briefly, we found that magpies, that are the main host of the great spotted cuckoo in Europe, have alleles of one microsatellite locus (Ase64) that segregate between accepters and rejecters of experimental parasitic eggs. Furthermore, differences in ejection rate among host populations exploited by the brood parasite covaried significantly with the genetic distance for this locus. 相似文献
4.
JUAN J. SOLER JESUS M. AVILES MANUEL SOLER ANDERS P. MØLLER 《Biological journal of the Linnean Society. Linnean Society of London》2003,79(4):551-563
Brood parasitism in birds is one of the best examples of coevolutionary interactions in vertebrates. Coevolution between hosts and parasites is assumed to occur because the parasite imposes strong selection pressures on its hosts, reducing their fitness and thereby favouring counter-adaptations (e.g. egg rejection) which, in turn, select for parasite resistance (e.g. egg mimicry). Great spotted cuckoos ( Clamator glandarius ) are usually considered a brood parasite with eggs almost perfectly mimicking those of their host, the magpie ( Pica pica ). However, Cl. glandarius also exploits South African hosts with very different eggs, both in colour and size, while the Cl. glandarius eggs are similar to those laid in nests of European hosts. Here, we used spectrophotometric techniques for the first time to quantify mimicry of parasitic eggs for eight different host species. We found: (1) non-significant differences in appearance of Cl. glandarius eggs laid in nests of different host species, although eggs laid in South Africa and Europe differed significantly; (2) contrary to the general assumption that Cl. glandarius eggs better mimic those of the main host in Europe ( P. pica ), Cl. glandarius eggs more closely resembled those of the azure-winged magpie ( Cyanopica cyana ), a potential host in which there is no evidence of recent parasitism; (3) the appearance of Cl. glandarius eggs was not significantly related to the appearance of host eggs. We discuss three possible reasons why Cl. glandarius eggs resemble eggs of some of their hosts. We suggest that colouration of Cl. glandarius eggs is an apomorphic trait, and that variation between eggs laid in South African and European host nests is due to genetic isolation among these populations and not due to variation in colouration of host eggs. © 2003 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society , 2003, 79 , 551–563. 相似文献
5.
Adult great spotted cuckoos Clamator glandarius damage the eggsof their magpie Pica pica host without removing them from thenest or eating them but by producing the death of the embryo.Observations as well as experiments were used to test severalpredictions of two different possibilities: great spotted cuckooegg-damaging behavior is a parasitic tactic resulting froma direct selection process (the adaptation hypothesis), oregg damage is caused by thick-shelled cuckoo eggs which evolvedto avoid breakage during rapid laying (the nonadaptation hypothesis).Previously, we provided experimental evidence that egg damageincreased the breeding success of cuckoos when they laid lateduring the laying sequence of the magpie. However, when theylaid early, egg-damaging behavior did not increase cuckoo breedingsuccess, contrary to the adaptation hypothesis. In an experimental
study, when we simulated laying behavior by the great spottedcuckoo, we found that (1) the number of damaged magpie eggswas significantly lower than in natural parasitism, and (2)whereas in the experimental manipulations the number of damagedeggs did not depend on the number of magpie eggs, in natural
parasitism, the number of damaged eggs increased with clutchsize of the magpie. These results support the predictions ofthe adaptation hypothesis, implying that egg damage is notan incidental consequence of rapid egg laying, but an adaptation. 相似文献
6.
Manuel Soler Liesbeth De Neve Tomás Pérez‐Contreras Luis A. Rubio 《Biological journal of the Linnean Society. Linnean Society of London》2014,111(2):280-289
Altricial nestlings are under strong selection pressures to optimize digestive efficiency because this is one of the main factors affecting nestling growth and survival. Bird species vary in their ability to assimilate different nutrients and current theory predicts that nestlings should also be able to adjust their nutritional physiology to feeding frequency. Variation in parental provisioning to nestlings would select for flexibility in nestling digestive physiology, which would allow maximization of nutrient assimilation. In the present study, by making use of a brood parasite–host study system in which great spotted cuckoo nestlings (Clamator glandarius) are reared by magpie (Pica pica) host foster parents when sharing the nest with host nestlings, we tested several predictions of the adaptive digestive efficiency paradigm. A hand‐feeding experiment was employed in which we fed both great spotted cuckoo and magpie nestlings with exactly the same diet simulating one food abundance period and one food deprivation period. The results obtained show that cuckoo nestlings ingested more food, gained significantly more weight during the abundance period, and assimilated a higher proportion of the ingested food than magpie nestlings. These results demonstrate for the first time that cuckoo nestlings enjoy digestive adaptations that favour a rapid processing of the ingested food, thereby maximizing their intake rate but without decreasing digestive efficiency. © 2013 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2014, 111 , 280–289. 相似文献
7.
Juan Jos Soler Juan Gabriel Martinez Manuel Soler Anders Pape Mller 《Evolution; international journal of organic evolution》1999,53(3):947-956
Host responses toward brood parasitism have been shown to differ among populations depending on the duration of sympatry between host and parasite, although populations not currently parasitized show rejection behavior against parasitic eggs. The persistence of rejection behavior in unparasitized host populations and rapid increases of rejection rate in parasitized ones have sometimes been explained as the result of gene flow of rejecter genes from sympatry to allopatry (rejecter-gene flow hypothesis). We present data on the rejection behavior of magpies (Pica pica) the main European host of the great spotted cuckoo (Clamator glandarius), in 15 populations (nine sympatric six allopatric) across their distribution range in Europe. Rejection rates of mimetic and nonmimetic model eggs were significantly higher in sympatric than in allopatric magpie populations, although differences in rejection rate of both mimetic and nonmimetic model eggs between magpie populations were significantly correlated even after controlling tor phylogenetic effects, with differences between sympatric and allopatric magpie populations being larger for mimetic than for nonmimetic model eggs. Differences in rejection of mimetic model eggs were related to both genetic and geographic distances between populations, but differences in rejection rate of nonmimetic model eggs were unrelated to these distances. However, when comparing only sympatric populations, differences in rejection rate of both mimetic and nonmimetic model eggs were related to geographic distances. A multiple autocorrelation analysis revealed that differences among populations in rejection rates of mimetic model eggs had a strong geographic component whereas the main component of rejection rate of nonmimetic model eggs was genetic rather than geographic. These results support the rejecter-gene flow hypothesis. We discuss differences in rejection rates of mimetic and nonmimetic model eggs that suggest the egg-recognition ability of the host is genetically based, but is affected by a learning process for fine tuning of recognition. 相似文献
8.
The spatial habitat structure of host populations explains the pattern of rejection behavior in hosts and parasitic adaptations in cuckoos 总被引:6,自引:1,他引:6
Roskaft Eivin; Moksnes Arne; Stokke Bard G.; Moskat Csaba; Honza Marcel 《Behavioral ecology》2002,13(2):163-168
In this article we present tentative support for predictionsderived from a spatial habitat structure hypothesis arguingthat common cuckoos Cuculus canorus, the most common obligatebrood parasite in Europe, only breed in areas where they haveaccess to vantage points in trees. Thus, species in which somepopulations breed near trees while other populations breed farther
from trees have a different cuckoohost population dynamicthan species in which all populations always breed in the vicinityof trees. Parasitism rate, mimicry of brood parasite eggs withthose of the hosts, and rejection behavior of hosts varieswith the host breeding habitat. Cuckoos are best adapted toexploit species in which some populations breed near trees while
other populations breed in open areas because such hosts arenot always accessible to cuckoos, and thus gene flow amongunparasitized and parasitized populations delays the evolutionof host adaptations. Adaptive behavior in cuckoos as well asin their hosts can be predicted from the spatial habitat structurehypothesis. 相似文献
9.
Arms races between brood parasites and their hosts provide model systems for studying the evolutionary repercussions of species interactions. However, how naive hosts identify brood parasites as enemies remains poorly understood, despite its ecological and evolutionary significance. Here, we investigate whether young, cuckoo-naive superb fairy-wrens, Malurus cyaneus, can learn to recognize cuckoos as a threat through social transmission of information. Naive individuals were initially unresponsive to a cuckoo specimen, but after observing conspecifics mob a cuckoo, they made more whining and mobbing alarm calls, and spent more time physically mobbing the cuckoo. This is the first direct evidence that naive hosts can learn to identify brood parasites as enemies via social learning. 相似文献
10.
We analysed the spatial and temporal pattern of egg laying in great spotted cuckoo females using microsatellite typing to
determine parentage of the eggs and nestlings found in host (magpie) nests. The results showed that there were no exclusive
laying territories in the study area. Cases of multiparasitism could be due to single females laying two or more eggs in a
nest, or to several females using the same nest. In the latter case multiparasitism was due to a shortage of available host
nests. We argue that the need for very large laying areas and the likely small cost of sharing parental care for chicks make
the costs of defending territories higher than the benefits, which has constrained the evolution of territoriality in this
species.
Received: 16 March 1998 / Accepted: 15 June 1998 相似文献
11.
Mahler B Confalonieri VA Lovette IJ Reboreda JC 《Journal of evolutionary biology》2007,20(5):1918-1923
Obligate avian brood parasites can be host specialists or host generalists. In turn, individual females within generalist brood parasites may themselves be host specialists or generalists. The shiny cowbird Molothrus bonariensis is an extreme generalist, but little is known about individual female host fidelity. We examined variation in mitochondrial control region sequences from cowbird chicks found in nests of four common Argentinean hosts. Haplotype frequency distributions differed among cowbird chicks from nests of these hosts, primarily because eggs laid in nests of house wrens Troglodytes aedon differed genetically from those laid in nests of the other three hosts (chalk-browed mockingbird Mimus saturninus, brown-and-yellow marshbird Pseudoleistes virescens, and rufous-collared sparrow Zonotrichia capensis). These differences in a maternally inherited marker indicate the presence of a nonrandom laying behaviour in the females of this otherwise generalist brood parasite, which may be guided by choice for nest type, as house wrens nest in cavities whereas the other three species are open cup nesters. 相似文献
12.
Langmore N.E.; Kilner R.M.; Butchart S.H.M.; Maurer G.; Davies N.B.; Cockburn A.; Macgregor N.A.; Peters A.; Magrath M.J.L.; Dowling D.K. 《Behavioral ecology》2005,16(4):686-692
Exploitation of hosts by brood parasitic cuckoos is expectedto stimulate a coevolutionary arms race of adaptations and counteradaptations.However, some hosts have not evolved defenses against parasitism.One hypothesis to explain a lack of host defenses is that thelife-history strategies of some hosts reduce the cost of parasitismto the extent that accepting parasitic eggs in the nest is evolutionarilystable. Under this hypothesis, it pays hosts to accept cuckooeggs if (1) the energetic cost of raising the cuckoo is low,(2) there is time to renest, and (3) clutch size is small. Weparasitized the nests of host and nonhost species with nonmimeticmodel eggs to test whether the evolution of egg recognitionby cuckoo hosts could be explained by life-history variablesof the host. The most significant factor explaining rates ofrejection of model eggs was whether or not a species was a cuckoohost, with hosts rejecting model eggs at a higher rate thannonhosts. Egg-rejection rates were also explained by visibilitywithin the nest and by cuckoo mass. We found little supportfor the life-history model of egg rejection. Our results suggestthat parasitism is always sufficiently costly to select forhost defenses and that the evolution of defenses may be limitedby proximate constraints such as visibility within the nest. 相似文献
13.
Mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) is being used increasingly to explore the evolution of host specificity in avian brood parasites. A stable coevolutionary equilibrium between multiple phylogenetically unrelated hosts and a brood parasitic species predicts that mtDNA diversity in the parasite should be relatively deep and phylogenetically structured. Also, the different intraspecific clades resulting from parasitism to multiple sympatric hosts should themselves occur sympatrically. However, mtDNA diversity in brood parasites is as susceptible to effects of historical population dynamics as in any species. We demonstrate the relevance of these dynamics to the use of mtDNA in understanding coevolution between an Australian brood-parasite, Horsfield's Bronze-Cuckoo Chalcites basalis and its hosts, Malurus fairy-wrens and Acanthiza thornbills. Previous ecological and behavioural analyses argue that Malurus- and Acanthiza-specific host races exist in C. basalis. Yet mtDNA diversity in C. basalis is low and phylogenetically unstructured (mean sequence divergence 0.15 +/- 0.07%, range 0.00%-0.31%) and tests of mtDNA neutrality and range expansion vs. population stability (Tajima's D, Fu & Li's F* and D*, Fu's F(S), mismatch analyses) all indicate that C. basalis has expanded its range very recently, probably within the last few tens of thousands of years following climatic amelioration after a peak of aridity in the late Pleistocene. The low mtDNA diversity and its lack of phylogenetic structure in C. basalis deny the existence of evolutionarily long-term stable host races in C. basalis but not the possibility of recently evolved ones. They highlight the need for renewed behavioural and ecological study of the relationship between C. basalis and its hosts. Our findings illustrate the need to understand the evolutionary context in which a brood parasite and its hosts have evolved if mtDNA data are to be used in testing hypotheses concerning the origin and maintenance of host specificity. They also add to the growing body of work illustrating the use of mismatch analyses and Fu's F(S) in detecting range expansions. 相似文献
14.
Matthew I. M. Louder Wendy M. Schelsky Amber N. Albores Jeffrey P. Hoover 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2015,282(1814)
Avian obligate brood parasites, which rely solely on hosts to raise their young, should choose the highest quality hosts to maximize reproductive output. Brown-headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater) are extreme host generalists, yet female cowbirds could use information based on past reproductive outcomes to make egg-laying decisions thus minimizing fitness costs associated with parasitizing low-quality hosts. We use a long-term (21 years) nest-box study of a single host, the prothonotary warbler (Protonotaria citrea), to show that local cowbird reproductive success, but not host reproductive success, was positively correlated with the probability of parasitism the following year. Experimental manipulations of cowbird success corroborated that female cowbirds make future decisions about which hosts to use based on information pertaining to past cowbird success, both within and between years. The within-year pattern, in particular, points to local cowbird females selecting hosts based on past reproductive outcomes. This, coupled with high site fidelity of female cowbirds between years, points to information use, rather than cowbird natal returns alone, increasing parasitism rates on highly productive sites between years. 相似文献
15.
De Mársico MC Reboreda JC 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2008,275(1650):2499-2506
Obligate avian brood parasites show dramatic variation in the degree to which they are host specialists or host generalists. The screaming cowbird Molothrus rufoaxillaris is one of the most specialized brood parasites, using a single host, the bay-winged cowbird (Agelaioides badius) over most of its range. Coevolutionary theory predicts increasing host specificity the longer the parasite interacts with a particular avian community, as hosts evolve defences that the parasite cannot counteract. According to this view, host specificity can be maintained if screaming cowbirds avoid parasitizing potentially suitable hosts that have developed effective defences against parasitic females or eggs. Specialization may also be favoured, even in the absence of host defences, if the parasite's reproductive success in alternative hosts is lower than that in the main host. We experimentally tested these hypotheses using as alternative hosts two suitable but unparasitized species: house wrens (Troglodytes aedon) and chalk-browed mockingbirds (Mimus saturninus). We assessed host defences against parasitic females and eggs, and reproductive success of the parasite in current and alternative hosts. Alternative hosts did not discriminate against screaming cowbird females or eggs. Egg survival and hatching success were similarly high in current and alternative hosts, but the survival of parasitic chicks was significantly lower in alternative hosts. Our results indicate that screaming cowbirds have the potential to colonize novel hosts, but higher reproductive success in the current host may favour host fidelity. 相似文献
16.
17.
On the origin of brood parasitism in altricial birds 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
The probability that obligate interspecific brood parasitism(OP), among altricial birds evolved directly from the normalbreeding (no parasitism, NP) mode or indirectly through intraspecificnest parasitism (INP) was examined by using maximum-likelihoodand parsimony approaches. We examined the probability of ancestralstates at 24 key nodes in order to test our hypotheses. Thestate of the most basal node in a tree of 565 genera of altricialbirds is equivocal; however, the state probability of NP atthis node is about 5.5-fold more likely than the state of obligateparasite. A similar trend was observed for basal nodes of mostfamilies examined. The INP state was supported only in the Hirundinidae.The high incidence of INP among martins and swallows explainsthis finding. Contrary to our predictions, even in other groupswhere there is a high incidence of INP and OP, such as in thetribe Icteri and the Old World finches, the probability of NPbeing ancestral was very high. We conclude that in all casesbut one (Hirundinidae) obligate, and probably facultative, broodparasitism evolved directly from normal breeding mode ratherthan indirectly through some other form of parasitism. 相似文献
18.
The evolution of egg size in the brood parasitic cuckoos 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
We compared genera of nonparasitic cuckoos and two groups ofparasitic cuckoos: those raised together with host young ("nonejectors")and those in which the newly hatched cuckoo either ejects thehost eggs or chicks, or kills the host young ("ejectors"). Nonejectorsare similar to their hosts in body size and parasitize largerhosts than do ejectors, which parasitize hosts much smallerthan themselves. In both types of parasite, the cuckoo's eggtends to match the host eggs in size. To achieve this, nonejectorshave evolved a smaller egg for their body size than have nonparasiticcuckoos, and ejectors have evolved an even smaller egg. Amongejector cuckoo genera, larger cuckoos have larger eggs relativeto the eggs of their hosts, and the relationship between cuckooegg volume (mass of the newly-hatched cuckoo) and host egg volume(mass to be ejected) did not differ from that predicted by weight-liftingallometry. However, comparing among Cuculus cuckoo species,the allometric slope differed from the predicted, so it is notclear that egg size is related to the need to give the cuckoochick sufficient strength for ejection. Comparing the two mostspeciose ejector genera, Chrysococcyx cuckoos (smaller and parasitizedome-nesting hosts) lay eggs more similar in size to their host'seggs than do Cuculus cuckoos (larger and parasitize open cupnestinghosts). Closer size-matching of host eggs in Chrysococcyx mayreflect the following: (1) selection to reduce adult body massto facilitate entry through small domed nest holes to lay, and(2) less need for a large egg, because longer incubation periodsin dome-nesting hosts allow the young cuckoo more time to growbefore it need eject host eggs. 相似文献
19.
Canvasback ducks (Aythya valisineria) suffer both intra- andinterspecific brood parasitism. During 3 years in Manitoba,80% of canvasback nests (n = 179 nests with completed clutches)were parasitized by redheads (A. americana), other canvasbacks,or both, with an average of 4.7 parasitic eggs per parasitizednest. Parasitism had significant negative effects on the reproductivesuccess of nesting canvasbacks, although the proximate mechanismsinvolved differed from those operating in altricial species.Accidental displacement of eggs when parasitic females forcedtheir way onto host nests was the principal negative effectof parasitism, reducing the number of host eggs that were incubatedand ultimately hatched. Parasitism by redheads was relativelymore costly to canvasbacks than was intraspecific parasitism,with approximately 0.31 and 0.17 host eggs displaced per parasiticredhead and canvasback egg laid, respectively. No additionalnegative effects of parasitism on the hatchability of host eggsoccurred subsequent to parasitic laying. Posthatch survivalof canvasback ducklings was lower in broods from parasitizednests but was unrelated to the presence or absence of redheadducklings. Canvasback hosts resisted intrusions by parasiticfemales but showed no evidence of discrimination against parasiticeggs or ducklings. Because most costs of parasitism in thissystem are inflicted at the time of parasitic laying, subsequentrejection of parasitic eggs or ducklings is probably of littlebenefit to canvasback hosts, while the evolution of behaviorthat might prevent parasitic laying in the first place, suchas more vigorous nest defense, may be constrained by its highcosts 相似文献
20.
The sight of an adult brood parasite near the nest is an insufficient cue for a honeyguide host to reject foreign eggs 下载免费PDF全文
Hosts of brood‐parasitic birds typically evolve anti‐parasitism defences, including mobbing of parasitic intruders at the nest and the ability to recognize and reject foreign eggs from their clutches. The Greater Honeyguide Indicator indicator is a virulent brood parasite that punctures host eggs and kills host young, and accordingly, a common host, the Little Bee‐eater Merops pusillus frequently rejects entire clutches that have been parasitized. We predicted that given the high costs of accidentally rejecting an entire clutch, and that the experimental addition of a foreign egg is insufficient to induce this defence, Bee‐eaters require the sight of an adult parasite near the nest as an additional cue for parasitism before they reject a clutch. We found that many Little Bee‐eater parents mobbed Greater Honeyguide dummies while ignoring barbet control dummies, showing that they recognized them as a threat. Surprisingly, however, neither a dummy Honeyguide nor the presence of a foreign egg, either separately or in combination, was sufficient to stimulate egg rejection. 相似文献