共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
George H. Young Anthony M. Coelho Jr. Claud A. Bramblett 《Primates; journal of primatology》1982,23(4):511-519
This paper focuses on the development of behavior in a mixed-longitudinal sample of 77 baboons (Papio sp. — 46 males and 31 females) from birth to 2 years of age. Eleven behaviors measured as frequency of performance and frequency
of reception were used to test null hypotheses of gender and rearing differences among thePapio sp. subjects. The behaviors measured included: groom, mount, present, open-mouth threat, hard bite, displace, avoid, grapple,
play bite, charge and chase. The data indicate that gender differences exist in the development of grooming, sociosexual behavior
and aggression and play. The data also indicate that subjects raised in the nursery environment were behaviorally indistinguishable
from mother-reared peers on the basis of the 11 behavioral criteria. 相似文献
2.
J. R. Butler 《BMJ (Clinical research ed.)》1989,298(6681):1128-1129
3.
4.
Smith K Alberts SC Altmann J 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2003,270(1514):503-510
Adult female cercopithecines have long been known to bias their social behaviour towards close maternal kin. However, much less is understood about the behaviour of paternal kin, especially in wild populations. Here, we show that wild adult female baboons bias their affiliative behaviour towards their adult paternal half-sisters in the same manner and to the same extent that they bias their behaviour towards adult maternal half-sisters. Females appear to rely heavily on social familiarity as a means of biasing their behaviour towards paternal half-sisters, but may use phenotype matching as well. 相似文献
5.
6.
H B Valman 《BMJ (Clinical research ed.)》1980,280(6220):1049-1051
7.
J. K. Chadwick-Jones 《International journal of primatology》1991,12(2):145-161
A theoretical and illustrative analysis of sequences of actions between pairs of olive baboons (Papio cynocephals anubis) is carried out using the social contingency model originally suggested by Jones and Gerard (1967) in social psychology.
The model provides the basis for a step-by-step analysis and a typology. Signals from body postures, facial expressions, and
vocalizations are subject to close study by using the model, in a similar fashion to its use in social skills analysis (Argyle,
1988). Sequences are categorized according to individual and joint strategies. Examples are given of three categories of contingency,
reactive, mutual, and asymmetrical, which are adapted from their social psychological usage. The model is extended by a new
category for competitive relationships between baboons: a conflictive category. The contingencies, as they are shown graphically,
have a superficial resemblance to Tinbergen's (1951) zigzag diagrams, but their purpose is distinct in focusing on the detail
of both infrequent and repeated interactions. The overall aim of the contingencies is to improve the analysis of social behavior
at the proximate level and the model is potentially applicable to observation records at a level of detail that is often not
utilized in reporting field studies. 相似文献
8.
9.
We have previously studied the relationship between social subordinance (by approach-avoidance criteria) and physiology among male olive baboons (Papio anubis) living freely in a national park in Africa. In stable hierarchies, subordinate individuals have elevated basal glucocorticoid concentrations and a blunted glucocorticoid response to stress, as well as a prompt suppression of testosterone concentrations during stress. These facets have been interpreted as reflecting the chronic stress of social subordinance. In the present report, we find that these endocrine features do not mark all subordinate individuals. Instead, endocrine profiles differed among subordinate males as a function of particular stylistic traits of social behavior. A subset of subordinate males was identified who had significantly high rates of consortships, a behavior usually shown only by high-ranking males. Such behavior predicted the beginning transition to dominance, as these males were significantly more likely than other subordinates to have moved to the dominant half of the hierarchy over the subsequent 3 years. In keeping with this theme of emerging from subordinance, these individuals also had significantly larger glucocorticoid stress-responses, another feature typical of dominant males. However, these subordinate males also had significantly elevated basal glucocorticoid concentrations; it is suggested that this reflects that stressfulness of their overt and precocious strategy of reproductive competition. In support of this, subordinate males with high rates of covert “stolen copulations” did not show elevated basal glucocorticoid concentrations. A second subset of subordinate males were the most likely to initiate fights or to displace aggression onto a third party after losing a fight. These males had significantly or near-significantly elevated testosterone concentrations, compared to the remaining subordinate cohort. Moreover, these males had significantly lower basal glucocorticoid concentrations; this echoes an extensive literature showing that the availability of a displacement behavior (whether aggressive or otherwise) after a stressor decreases glucocorticoid secretion. In support of this interpretation suggesting that it was the initiation of these aggressive acts which attenuated glucocorticoid secretion, there was no association between glucocorticoid concentrations and participation (independent of initiation) in aggressive interactions. Thus, these findings suggest that variables other than rank alone may be associated with distinctive endocrine profiles, and that even in the face of a social stressor (such as subordinance), particular behavioral styles may attenuate the endocrine indices of stress. Am. J. Primatol. 42:25–39, 1997. © 1997 Wiley-Liss, Inc. 相似文献
10.
Terry Maple Mark E. Wilson Evan L. Zucker S. F. Wilson 《Primates; journal of primatology》1978,19(3):593-602
Reproductive behavior is a topic of extreme importance in the literature of primate behavior. This report concerns the mother-infant
interaction system in a captiveborn, mother-reared infant orang-utan during the first six months of its life. Of particular
interest are the sexual behaviors directed by the mother toward the infant, and the regular stimulation of the infant's genitals.
The behaviors described herein are compared to other mother-infant pairs of this and other anthropoid species.
This research has been supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health to the Yerkes Regional Primate Center (RR00165),
the Emory University Psychology Department (HD00208), and a faculty research grant to the first author from Emory University's
McCandless Fund for Biomedical Research. 相似文献
11.
Background
During infancy, rapid changes in physical growth affect the size and shape of the body segments. To understand the effects of growth on movement, it is first necessary to quantify rates of development during the acquisition of important motor milestones. The goal of this longitudinal study was to quantify the physical growth of infant body segments during the initial stages of independent walking.Methods
Ten infants (N = 10) aged between 28 and 55 weeks at the beginning of the study were tested biweekly (every two weeks) for three months. A 13-segment mathematical model of the human body was used to estimate the inertial parameters of the infant body segments at each session. An analysis of variance was used to test for significant differences in segment masses between biweekly measures. Polynomial contrasts were used to test for linear trends in the growth data.Results
Significant differences between biweekly measures of segment mass were found only for the head/neck (F(5,45) = 3.42, p < 0.05), upper trunk (F(5,45) = 4.04, p < 0.01), and lower trunk (F(5,45) = 3.49, p < 0.01). The lower trunk demonstrated a linear increase in mass (F(1,9) = 4.56, p < 0.05). However, the upper trunk demonstrated a quadratic trend in growth (F(1,9) = 9.13, p < 0.01), while the head/neck segment showed a cubic trend in growth (F(1,9) = 3.80, p < 0.05). Significant differences in axial segment masses were also found between subjects (F(9,45) = 5.92, p < 0.001).Conclusion
Given that postural control proceeds in a cephalocaudal manner, the lower trunk segment would be brought under control last, in terms of the axial segments. Increases in the mass of this segment could constrain the system, thereby acting as a control parameter for the onset and development of motor patterns.12.
We have previously studied the relationship between dominance rank and physiology among male olive baboons (Papio anubis) living freely in a national park in Africa. In stable hierarchies, such males have distinctive secretory profiles of glucocorticoids and of testosterone. We find that these endocrine features are not, in fact, purely markers of social dominance; instead, they are found only among dominant males with particular stylistic traits of social behavior. One intercorrelated stylistic cluster revolved around the intensity with which the male is involved in sexual consortships (e.g., frequency of copulation, of grooming, degree to which feeding is suppressed by being in consortship). Males most involved in such consortships had the lowest basal cortisol concentrations and smaller cortisol stress-responses. A second stylistic cluster revolved around the degree of social affiliation (e.g., rate of grooming and interacting positively with non-estrus females and infants). Males who were highly affiliated had low basal cortisol concentrations and an attenuated cortisol stress-response. A third cluster revolved around the degree to which males could distinguish between highly threatening interactions with rivals and neutral or mildly threatening ones. Males most adept at this had lower basal cortisol concentrations. These behavioral/endocrine clusters were independent of each other. This suggests that the same adaptive physiological feature (e.g., low basal cortisol concentrations) may arise from different and independent personality styles. © 1992 Wiley-Liss, Inc. 相似文献
13.
Chemotherapy of Pseudomonas aeruginosa infections in children during the first three months of life]
The data on the efficacy of antibacterial drugs and their combinations in treatment of 150 three-month old infants with generalized infection due to Pseudomonas aeruginosa are presented. The clinical isolates of P. aeruginosa were found to be carriers of multiple drug resistance which markedly complicated the chemotherapy. Only combined antibacterial therapy of such infants proved to be rational. High activity of aminoglycosides, azlocillin and cefotaxime against P. aeruginosa and the synergistic action of their combinations observed in the patients permitted to recommend the use of combinations of the above drugs in the empirical chemotherapy. 相似文献
14.
15.
We studied 821 infants who were bottle-fed from birth to determine whether non-milk solids begun within the first 3 monts of life affect early growth. The infants were seen seven to 10 days and three months after birth. They were divided into three groups-those who had started solids before 6 weeks of age (657 infants); those who had started solids between the ages of 6 weeks and 3 months (124 infants); and those given cow''s milk formulae alone during the first three months (40 infants). The infants were weighed and measured seven to 10 days after birth and at three months. Mean weekly rates of weight and length gain over the 3 months were calculated and did not differ significantly between the three groups. As no record was made of quantities of food taken we cannot explain the failure of solid foods to affect growth. Studies of how young infants utilise these foods are needed. 相似文献
16.
Ecological and social determinants of birth intervals in baboons 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Birth rates in primates have long been proposed to result froman interaction between ecological and social factors. We analyzeda variety of social and environmental variables to determinewhich ones best explain the observed variation in interbirthintervals across 14 baboon populations. Both the number offemales in the group and mean annual temperature were foundto be important, and a multivariate equation containing thequadratic components of both these variables accounts for almostall the observed variance in interbirth intervals. The quadraticrelationship with temperature is explained in terms of theenergetic costs of maintaining a stable body temperature at
both low and high temperatures. The quadratic relationship withnumber of females results from relationships with both foodavailability and the costs of increasing intragroup competitionas group size increases. Although females may be able to exerta certain degree of choice in their reproductive schedulingdecisions, they are ultimately constrained by limits imposedupon them by the complex interactions between their ecologicaland demographic environment. 相似文献
17.
The levels of azithromycin in the chorion tissue within the first 3 months of the pregnancy term (42 to 56 days post coitus) were determined. Three days after the use of azithromycin (Sumamed) the antibiotic concentration in the villi tissue was 0.065 to 0.26 mcg/mg (the average of 0.129 mcg/mg). The azithromycin concentration above 0.125 mcg/mg was detected in 38.5% of the women. The results confirmed the fact of significant azithromycin accumulation in the chorion tissue. The percentage of the azithromycin transplacental transfer within the above mentioned pregnancy term varied from 2.3 to 9.3 (the average of 4.6). 相似文献
18.
Joan B. Silk Jacinta C. Beehner Thore J. Bergman Catherine Crockford Anne L. Engh Liza R. Moscovice Roman M. Wittig Robert M. Seyfarth Dorothy L. Cheney 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2009,276(1670):3099-3104
Sociality has evolved in many animal taxa, but primates are unusual because they establish highly differentiated bonds with other group members. Such bonds are particularly pronounced among females in species like baboons, with female philopatry and male dispersal. These relationships seem to confer a number of short-term benefits on females, and sociality enhances infant survival in some populations. However, the long-term consequences of social bonds among adult females have not been well established. Here we provide the first direct evidence that social relationships among female baboons convey fitness benefits. In a group of free-ranging baboons, Papio cynocephalus ursinus, the offspring of females who formed strong social bonds with other females lived significantly longer than the offspring of females who formed weaker social bonds. These survival benefits were independent of maternal dominance rank and number of kin and extended into offspring adulthood. In particular, females who formed stronger bonds with their mothers and adult daughters experienced higher offspring survival rates than females who formed weaker bonds. For females lacking mothers or adult daughters, offspring survival was closely linked to bonds between maternal sisters. These results parallel those from human studies, which show that greater social integration is generally associated with reduced mortality and better physical and mental health, particularly for women. 相似文献
19.
Maestripieri D Mayhew J Carlson CL Hoffman CL Radtke JM 《Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology》2007,78(1):56-68
Little is known about the mating system and social organization of Guinea baboons. This study investigated whether Guinea baboons have a harem-based mating system similar to that of hamadryas and gelada baboons and whether one-male mating units also correspond to social units. Ten adult females in a captive multi-male multi-female group of Guinea baboons were focally observed 2 h per week for 12 weeks, and all observed copulations within the group were recorded. Some males copulated with a single female while others had harems of 2-4 females. All females copulated with a single male except 1 female that switched harems early in the study. The focal females had higher rates of social interaction with their harem members, especially their harem male, than with individuals outside the harem. Females appeared to be subordinate to the harem male but little or no physical aggression or herding behavior from the male was observed. Variation in female social interactions within the harem was not accounted for by their sexual interactions with the male or their genetic relatedness with the females. Females, however, appeared to maintain social relationships with their female relatives in other harems. Taken together, the results of this study show that both mating and affiliative interactions in Guinea baboons are concentrated within one-male units and that the social dynamics within and between these units share some similarities as well as differences with those of hamadryas and gelada baboons. 相似文献
20.
The evolution of life history traits is a topic of growing interest in primatology. Traits associated with fertility, such as age at menarche and age at first birth, have great significance for natural selection, and knowing the genetic basis of such demographic traits may improve our understanding of population dynamics. Knowledge of the heritability of reproductive traits may also have practical implications for the management of captive breeding colonies. A maximum likelihood method was used to estimate heritability of age at first birth for a sample of female olive baboons resident at the Southwest Foundation for Biomedical Research. Only animals born at the Foundation that were caged in mixed sex groups and that had previously given birth were included in the sample (n = 316). There were 117 independent individuals and 199 individuals in 35 pedigrees composed of 2 to 26 members. Age at first birth ranged from 3.85 years to 13.11 years. Age at first birth is highly heritable (h2 = 0.87) with no evidence for maternal effects or a dominance genetic component. This level of genetic variability in a fitness-related trait is contrary to evolutionary expectation and may reflect the uniform environment of a captive breeding situation. Thus, the heritability observed in this population may be taken as an upper bound for that in natural populations. © 1995 Wiley-Liss, Inc. 相似文献