首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
The perception of astringency and basic taste in mixtures and their interaction effects were investigated by two procedures. In Experiment 1, focused and nonfocused testing procedures were compared using mixtures of low and high concentrations of alum and basic taste solutions. Both procedures yielded taste and astringency intensities that were modality‐dependent. Nonfocused testing was used in Experiment 2 to investigate the interactions of astringent phenolic (tannic acid) and nonphenolic (alum) compounds with each basic taste. Sweetness of sucrose increased with increased concentration with or without alum or tannin present. Changes in salty, bitter, and sour taste intensities were modality‐dependent. Astringency either remained unchanged or decreased with the addition of sucrose, sodium chloride, citric acid, or caffeine depending upon the taste concentration. Bitterness of tannin and alum at high concentrations was suppressed by the addition of sucrose, sodium chloride, or citric acid; sourness also decreased in the presence of sucrose or sodium chloride as well as a high level of caffeine.  相似文献   

2.
Horio  T; Kawamura  Y 《Chemical senses》1998,23(4):417-421
The effects of physical exercise on preference for various sapid solutions was studied in 58 healthy university students. After 30 min of exercise using a bicycle ergometer at 50% VO2max (maximal oxygen uptake) intensity, a rating scale test on taste hedonic tone and the triangle test for taste absolute threshold were done. The test solutions were sucrose, NaCl, citric acid, caffeine and monosodium glutamate (MSG). Preference scale values for sucrose and citric acid increased after exercise, whereas the values for NaCl, caffeine and MSG were not changed. The absolute thresholds for all the sapid solutions did not differ for pre- and post-exercise. These findings indicate that in humans preference for sucrose and citric acid increase after physical exercise.   相似文献   

3.
A method for the first step in the selection of trained sensory assessors, based on their skill in identifying basic tastes in low concentration solutions, was defined and evaluated. The solutions used were: 0.4 and 0.8% sucrose; 0.03, 0.04 and 0.06% citric acid; 0.08 and 0.15% sodium chloride; 0.02 and 0.03% caffeine. Concentration ranges were found adequate. The criterion for selection was to have over 65% correct answers. Out of 226 candidates who received the test, 141 (62%) were selected. The probability of a candidate having more than a certain percentage of correct answers was calculated. The influence of the dilution water quality on the perception of basic tastes was significant; distilled water was the most adequate.  相似文献   

4.
The taste thresholds of caged cockatiels (Nymphicus hollandicus) for aqueous solutions of sodium chloride, citric acid, and sucrose were studied using two-choice taste-preference tests. The effects of location on the threshold were tested by putting the flavored solution in either the preferred or non-preferred locations (i.e. sides of cages) and offering water in the opposite location. Four parameters (total consumption, consumption from preferred side, consumption from non-preferred side, and proportion of test solution consumed) were measured at the end of 3-day test periods. Experiments were repeated with increasing concentration of test flavors until intake variables were significantly (p<0.05) affected. The results showed that birds distinguished (p<0.05) between purified water and 0.16 mol l(-1) sodium chloride, 0.36 mol l(-1) sucrose, or pH 5.5 citric acid. The likelihood of detecting a taste threshold was greater for sodium chloride and citric acid when these solutions were placed on the preferred side. In contrast, sucrose sensitivity was greater when this solution was offered on the non-preferred side.  相似文献   

5.
Evidence supporting a taste component for dietary fat has prompted study of plausible transduction mechanisms. One hypothesizes that long-chain, unsaturated fatty acids block selected delayed-rectifying potassium channels, resulting in a sensitization of taste receptor cells to stimulation by other taste compounds. This was tested in 17 male and 17 female adult (mean +/- SE age = 23.4 +/- 0.7 yr) propylthiouracil tasters with normal resting triglyceride concentrations (87.3 +/- 5.6 mg/day) and body mass index (23.3 +/- 0.4 kg/m(2)). Participants were tested during two approximately 30-min test sessions per week for 8 wk. Eight stimuli were assessed in duplicate via an ascending, three-alternative, forced-choice procedure. Qualities were randomized over weeks. Stimuli were presented as room-temperature, 5-ml portions. They included 1% solutions of linoleic acid with added sodium chloride (salty), sucrose (sweet), citric acid (sour), and caffeine (bitter) as well as solutions of these taste compounds alone. Participants also rated the intensity of the five strongest concentrations using the general labeled magnitude scale. The suprathreshold samples were presented in random order with a rinse between each. Subjects made the ratings self-paced while wearing nose clips. It was hypothesized that taste thresholds would be lower and absolute intensity ratings or slopes of intensity functions would be higher for the stimuli mixed with the linoleic acid. Thresholds were compared by paired t-tests and intensity ratings by repeated measures analysis of variance. Thresholds were significantly higher (i.e., lower sensitivity) for the sodium chloride, citric acid, and caffeine solutions with added fatty acid. Sweet, sour, and salty intensity ratings were lower or unchanged by the addition of a fatty acid. The two highest concentrations of caffeine were rated as weaker in the presence of linoleic acid. These data do not support a mechanism for detecting dietary fats whereby fatty acids sensitize taste receptor cells to stimulation by taste compounds.  相似文献   

6.
Two simple methods were followed to determine detection thresholds for the taste of substances in aqueous solution. The methods applied were: a modification of the ascending method of limits and a method based on the use of scales. Detection thresholds were calculated for the four basic tastes (sweet, salty, acid, and bitterness), umami and metallic. Reference substances for each taste were sucrose, sodium chloride, citric acid, caffeine, monosodium glutamate and iron (II) sulfate heptahydrate and the results of the two methods were compared. We found that the threshold values calculated by method ASTM-679 was within the range of concentrations identified with the scales method.  相似文献   

7.
Anderson's (1981) information integration approach was usedto examine taste mixture integration for intensity and hedonicjudgments of sucrose/sodium chloride and sucrose/citnc acidsolutions. In Experiment I, total intensity and hedonic ratingswere made for factorial combinations of sucrose and sodium chlorideor citric acid. The total intensity judgments produced an integrationpattern characterized by extreme subadditivity at high soluteconcentrations. (Subadditivity refers to the tendency for totalmixture intensity to be rated as less than the sum of the unmixedcomponent intensities.) The intensity judgment integration patternswere essentially identical for the two mixture types. However,the pattern of integration for the sucrose/sodium chloride andsucrose/citric acid mixtures differed for the hedonic ratings.Sucrose tended to eliminate the unpleasantness associated withincreasing concentrations of citric acid, while it only modulatedthis trend for sodium chloride. In Experiment II, subjects ratedthe individual sweet, salty and sour components of the mixturesto determine whether mixture suppression of the component tastescould account for the subadditivity of the total intensity judgmentsand/or the pattern of results for the hedonic ratings. It wasfound that sucrose suppressed the sour component of the sucrose/citricacid mixtures more than the salty component of the sucrose/sodiumchloride solutions This difference in component suppressionseemed to account for the hedonic integration patterns of thetwo mixture types which suggests that mixture suppression isan important factor to consider when predicting the pleasantnessof simple taste mixtures.  相似文献   

8.
Detection thresholds for NaCl, KCl, sucrose, aspartame, acetic acid, citric acid, caffeine, quinine HCl, monosodium glutamate (MSG) and inosine 5'-monophosphate (IMP) were assessed in 21 young (19-33 years) and 21 elderly (60-75 years) persons by taking the average of six ascending two-alternative forced choice tests. A significant overall effect was found for age, but not for gender. However, an interaction effect of age and gender was found. The older men were less sensitive than the young men and women for acetic acid, sucrose, citric acid, sodium and potassium chloride and IMP. To detect the compound dissolved in water they needed a 1.32 (aspartame) to 5.70 times (IMP) higher concentration than the younger subjects. A significant decline in thresholds with replication was shown. The age effect found could be attributed predominantly to a generic taste loss.  相似文献   

9.
A group of Japanese and a group of Australians rated their likingfor solutions of seven tastants: sucrose, NaCl, citric acid,caffeine and three umami tastes (MSG, IMP, GMP). The patternsof response were similar in both groups for all of the tastants.Differences between the groups were evident at the higher concentrationsof citric acid, GMP and MSG, and also at the lowest concentrationof MSG. There were no differences in the hedonic ratings forsucrose, NaCl or caffeine. Analysis of the response patternsof individuals across the range of concentrations revealed thatthe mean response patterns were generally a good representationof each group. These data suggest that the two groups were moresimilar than different in their responses to tastants in solution.  相似文献   

10.
The influence of temperature on the threshold values of primary tastes   总被引:4,自引:2,他引:2  
The threshold values of taste substances are influenced by severalfactors. To learn about the effect of the temperature on stimulus,recognition and terminal thresholds, these threshold valueswere determined for sucrose, sodium chloride, caffeine, quininehydrochloride, citric and tartaric acid at temperatures of 10,20, 40 and 60°C in a panel of 19 tasters. The individualvalues were found to vary over a wide range, resulting in arelatively large standard deviation of the mean threshold values.A temperature-dependence was found for the stimulus and recognitionthresholds which was different for the different taste substances.The stimulus and recognition thresholds are lowest in the temperaturerange of 20 to 40°C. The threshold values increase withincreasing temperature, and except for citric acid, significantand highly significant differences existed particularly between20 and 60°C, whereas statistically verifiable results couldnot be obtained between 10 and 20°C. There was no verifiabletemperature dependence either for the terminal thresholds. Theterminal thresholds were found to lie rather in a relativelynarrow concentration range and to be largely independent ofthe temperature. The results suggest that in warm dishes andbeverage more taste substances are required to produce the sametaste intensity. A dependence of the individual thresholds uponage, sex and smoking habits could not be found.  相似文献   

11.
Taste receptor organ activity and preference of sodium chloride solution in rats with deficit of substance P (SP) were studied. Total impulse activity of chorda tympani nerve of 7-8 week old rats was recorded under nembutal anesthesia. The taste responses to four solutions (sucrose, quinine sulfate, sodium chloride and citric acid) were decreased in rats injected with capsaicin in comparison with rats injected with vehicle. The rats injected with capsaicin preferred water to sodium chloride (two-bottle technique). On the contrary the rats injected with capsaicin preferred the salt solution. These data together with previous studies show the important role of peptide SP in taste receptor activity and "salt appetite".  相似文献   

12.
Vanne  M; Tuorila  H; Laurinen  P 《Chemical senses》1998,23(3):295-301
Memory for sweet taste intensities in different media during 125 h was investigated using three concentrations (w/w) of sucrose: 4.21% (0.125 M), 8.28% (0.25 M) and 16.06% (0.5 M). Sucrose was dissolved in four media [water with no tastant and water with 0.73% (0.125 M) sodium chloride, 0.04% (0.002 M) citric acid and 0.04% (0.002 M) caffeine] as standard stimuli. Subjects (n = 39) were assigned into four groups, each group performing the memory task in one medium only. After tasting each standard the subjects reproduced the subjective taste intensity immediately and after 12 min and 1, 5, 25 and 125 h by mixing portions of low (0%) and high (29.75%; 1 M) concentrations (w/w) of sucrose and by tasting and retasting (ad libitum procedure). The produced sucrose concentrations increased significantly from the first session to the 125 h time interval. There was a significant difference between immediately reproduced standard concentrations and concentrations produced after all time intervals. Relative differences from standard (delta i/i) differed only between concentrations produced immediately and after 125 h time interval. The low (4.21%) concentration showed larger differences from standard than the high (16.06%) concentration. The added tastant had no effect on the results.   相似文献   

13.
Six squirrel monkeys were presented with solutions representingthe four primary tastes. The solutions included various concentrationsof glucose or sodium saccharine (sweet), sodium chloride (salty),citric acid (sour), and quinine sulfate or sucrose octaacetate(bitter). A 24 hr two-bottle choice technique was employed.Amount of food, water, and solution consumed every 24 hr wasrecorded. The results showed that the maximum intake for glucosesolution was with the 5.0% concentration, although maximum caloricintake was with the 1.25% concentration where there was a potentiationof food intake. Water was preferred over sodium saccharine atthree of the four concentrations which were tested, and waterwas preferred over or equally to the concentrations of sodiumchloride and citric acid that were used. However, quinine sulfateand sucrose octaacetate were preferred over or equally to waterat most of the concentrations which were tested. In conducting the research described in this report, the investigatorsadhered to the ‘Guide for Laboratory Animal Facilitiesand Care’, as promulgated by the Committee on the guidefor Laboratory Animal Resources, National Academy of Sciences- National Research Council.  相似文献   

14.
The absence of population specificity of taste spectra in fish was confirmed. It was found that the three-spined stickleback Gasterosteus aculeatus of populations of the North (Norway), Baltic (Latvia), and Okhotsk (Kamchatka Peninsula) seas has similar taste preferences to classical taste substances (sodium chloride, calcium chloride, and sucrose—10%; citric acid—5%) and to 21 free amino acids (L-isomers, 0.1–0.001 M). For fish of all populations, glutamine, glutamic and aspartic acids, and alanine have the most attractive taste; cysteine, asparagine, and histidine have slightly less attractive taste. In Baltic Sea and Sea of Okhotsk sticklebacks, relatively not numerous amino acids that cause a significant decrease in pellet consumption—phenylalanine, tryptophane, leucine, and tyrosine—coincide (in the North Sea stickleback, substances with deterrent taste were not revealed). In sticklebacks of different populations, no differences in manifestation of feeding behavior were found, and correlations between different elements of fish response to pellets are similar or close the same. It was shown that intraoral sensory testing of food objects in three-spined stickleback can proceed along two alternative behavioral stereotypes similar in fish of the studied populations. The dependence of stereotypes of intraoral testing on taste qualities of the food object was revealed for the first time.  相似文献   

15.
Green BG  George P 《Chemical senses》2004,29(7):617-628
Individual differences in taste perception have been explained in part by variations in peripheral innervation associated with the genetic ability to taste the bitter substances PTC and PROP. In the present study we report evidence of another source of individual differences that is independent of taste stimulus, taste quality, or gustatory nerve. Individuals who perceived taste from thermal stimulation alone (thermal taste) gave significantly higher taste ratings to chemical stimuli--often by a factor of >2:1--than did individuals who perceived no taste from thermal stimulation. This was true for all taste stimuli tested (sucrose, saccharin, sodium chloride, citric acid, quinine sulfate, MSG and PROP), for all three gustatory areas of the mouth (anterior tongue, posterior tongue and soft palate) and for whole-mouth stimulation. Moreover, the same individuals reported stronger sensations from the olfactory stimulus vanillin, particularly when it was sensed retronasally. The generality of the thermal-taster advantage and its extension to an olfactory stimulus suggests that it arises from individual differences in CNS processes that are involved in perception of both taste and flavor.  相似文献   

16.
The effects of sodium deprivation for 10 d, a period sufficient to induce sodium appetite, on gustatory nerve discharges in rats were determined. Chorda tympani responses to concentration series of sodium chloride, sucrose, hydrochloric acid, and quinine hydrochloride were recorded and analyzed without the experimenter knowing the animal's deprivation condition. After deprivation, both whole nerve and single nerve fiber responses to sodium chloride were smaller; NaCl-best fibers, those more responsive to sodium chloride than to sucrose, hydrochloric acid, or quinine, were most affected. Thresholds had not changed; however, slopes of the stimulus-response functions for sodium chloride were lowered. Comparable changes in responses to the other stimuli did not occur. These results were discussed with respect to a possible relationship between changes in sodium chloride responsivity and changes in sodium intake, differences between methods of inducing sodium appetite, coding of taste quality and intensity, and mechanisms which might effect the responsivity change.  相似文献   

17.
Detection of a Target Taste in a Complex Masker   总被引:3,自引:3,他引:0  
Detection thresholds for sodium chloride were compared in aqueoussolution, in mixture with a sucrose masker, in mixture witha citric acid masker, and in mixture with both of these maskerstogether. Separately the two maskers raised the threshold ofsodium chloride by three to four times, and together by overnine times, a result consistent with independence (additivity)of the two masking effects. To achieve comparable masking witheither sucrose alone or with citric acid alone would requireincreasing their masking concentrations by about ten times.Hence multiple masking can be a far more efficient means ofconcealing a taste, whether an unpleasant one (e.g. the bittertaste of medicine) or a pleasant one (e.g. a salty or sweetcondiment). Multiple masking has dietary and culinary significance,especially for middle aged and elderly persons concerned aboutsalt intake, because their thresholds for NaCl, whether withor without maskers, are typically two or three times higherthan those of youthful persons. Chem. Senses 22: 529–534,1997.  相似文献   

18.
The taste of alcohol for rats as revealed by aversion generalization tests   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
In six experiments, naive rats were trained to avoid alcoholby pairing its presentation with lithium chloride-induced illness.Rats were then tested for aversion generalization by presentingvarious test solutions. Rats trained to avoid either 3, 6 or9% (v/v) alcohol generalized the aversion to a sucrose + quininehydrochloride solution. The five remaining binary combinationsof sucrose, sodium chloride, hydrochloric acid and quinine hydrochloridefailed to produce significant generalization in trained rats(Experiment 1). In further experiments, rats trained to avoid6% alcohol showed significant aversion generalization to a varietyof sucrose + other ‘bitter’ solutions (Experiment2) and sucrose + acid solutions (Experiment 3). Varying theconcentration of hydrochloric acid in a sucrose + acid mixtureproduced small but uniform degrees of aversion generalization(Experiment 4). Rats trained to avoid 6% alcohol did not generalizethe aversion to sucrose alone, regardless of concentration (Experiment5). Finally, in Experiment 6, rats trained to avoid 6% alcoholsuppressed consumption over a range of alcohol concentrations.These results confirm that, for rats, the taste of alcohol hasa complex set of characteristics; sweet taste in combinationwith other tastes appears to be the most similar as it is tothese solutions that rats with alcohol aversions show the mostgeneralized avoidance.  相似文献   

19.
The effects of 0.01, 0.06, 0.15 and 0.29 M sucrose, 0.02, 0.09and 0.26 M sodium chloride, and 0.0005, 0.002 and 0.005 M citricacid upon the recognition threshold of capsaicin in ethanoland water solution were determined. The effects of 2, 18 and60°C on the recognition threshold of capsaicin were measuered.These temperature effects were also measured when 0.15 M sucrose,or 0.09 M sodium chloride was added to the medium. Eight samplesranging from 0.06 to 0.07 mg/l capsaicin were presented to ataste panel. Results showed a trend (p <0.001) of maskingpungency with the increasing concentrations of sucrose. A trend(p <0.01) of enhancement of pungency in 60°C solutionsand masking of pungency in 2°C samples was found. A trend(p <0.05) of masking and enhancing of pungency was foundwhen samples containing 0.15 M sucrose were similarly heatedand chilled. In samples which contained 0.09 M sodium chlorideand which were tested at 60°C and 2°C the sodium chloridereduced the masking and enhancing effects of the temperatureof the solutions. Unlike sucrose, sodium chloride and citricacid at 18°C were found to have no strong trend in affectingthe threshold level of capsaicin.  相似文献   

20.
The objective of this study was to investigate whether the sensory performance of assessors in a sensory panel maybe explained by complexity of evaluated product. We aimed to investigate whether we could observe a decline in sensory performance when increasing the complexity of the product. The products increased in number of constituents from mixtures of sucrose, sodium chloride, citric acid and caffeine in water, to the foods ice tea and tomato soup constituting different levels of the same substances. Candidates who succeeded evaluating one product were not always successful evaluating others. Few subjects were successful in everything. The conclusion was that there is only minor systematic decline with increasing complexity of products. The authors emphasize that definition of complexity involves more than just counting number of constituents and taste sensations, and suggest that minor differences in the task given to the assessor might explain different performances.

PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS


Practical use of the research presented in the present paper is in a sensory evaluation context. It is important for the users of sensory data to find out how the profiling should be organized to achieve optimum output, and in specific, the need for extensive training when dealing with a more complex product. The present study hypothesized that sensory assessors would have more difficulties evaluating a more complex product. However, the results showed that panel leaders should be more concerned with the task variables in the sensory evaluation. Even a minor shift in task variables had a stronger impact on the performance and reliability of the assessors than increasing number of constituents and/or stimuli sensations of the product. This study did not demonstrate a need for extensive training when dealing with a more complex product as hypothesized.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号