首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
As soon as they hatch, gallinaceous chicks follow broody hens. This matriarchal unit presents a temporal organization of activity. The ontogeny of this ultradian rhythm of activity was followed in Japanese quail during their first 3 weeks of life. Under controlled laboratory conditions, 12 groups of four chicks were recorded using an activity monitoring system. They were observed between the ages of 2 and 17 days. Chicks in groups presented an ultradian rhythm of activity, with a period that increased significantly from 14.3 ± 1.4 minutes when chicks were 2 days old to 26.0 ± 1.9 minutes when they were 16 days old. The increase of ultradian periodicity was particularly pronounced during their first and third weeks of life. Finally, the ultradian period was correlated positively with body weight of the chicks. (Chronobiology International, 17(6), 767–776, 2000)  相似文献   

2.
The function of ultradian rhythms is not yet clearly elucidated. In particular, short-term rhythms are expressed during early ontogeny, especially in broods of precocial birds. We investigated the relationship between the clarity of the ultradian rhythm of the activity/rest cycle of a group of young Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) and the level of social synchronisation and spatial cohesion between the birds within that group. The subjects were descended from two lines selected for either very pronounced rhythmic or arrhythmic circadian activity. We found a positive relationship between the clarity of the ultradian rhythm of the activity/rest cycle when birds were young and the clarity of the circadian rhythm of feeding activity when birds were older, but still immature. The temporal organisation of the behaviour of the chicks from these two lines was observed in outdoor aviaries, when they were 4, 8, 12 and 15 days old. The mean ultradian period expressed by groups of 12 chicks was variable, with a minimum of 6 minutes. The ultradian period lengthened regularly as chicks grew older, and reached approximately 40 min on day 15. The clarity of the ultradian rhythmicity of group activity was linked to the level of inter-individual social synchronisation and of spatial cohesion; the more pronounced the ultradian rhythms of a group, the greater the temporal and spatial cohesion of the chicks within the group. Moreover, these characteristics varied with the age of the chicks. Finally, chicks in the less rhythmic groups weighed less. These results stress the adaptive value of this temporal organisation strategy under natural conditions.  相似文献   

3.
The function of ultradian rhythms is not yet clearly elucidated. In particular, short-term rhythms are expressed during early ontogeny, especially in broods of precocial birds. We investigated the relationship between the clarity of the ultradian rhythm of the activity/rest cycle of a group of young Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) and the level of social synchronisation and spatial cohesion between the birds within that group. The subjects were descended from two lines selected for either very pronounced rhythmic or arrhythmic circadian activity. We found a positive relationship between the clarity of the ultradian rhythm of the activity/rest cycle when birds were young and the clarity of the circadian rhythm of feeding activity when birds were older, but still immature. The temporal organisation of the behaviour of the chicks from these two lines was observed in outdoor aviaries, when they were 4, 8, 12 and 15 days old. The mean ultradian period expressed by groups of 12 chicks was variable, with a minimum of 6 minutes. The ultradian period lengthened regularly as chicks grew older, and reached approximately 40 min on day 15. The clarity of the ultradian rhythmicity of group activity was linked to the level of inter-individual social synchronisation and of spatial cohesion; the more pronounced the ultradian rhythms of a group, the greater the temporal and spatial cohesion of the chicks within the group. Moreover, these characteristics varied with the age of the chicks. Finally, chicks in the less rhythmic groups weighed less. These results stress the adaptive value of this temporal organisation strategy under natural conditions.  相似文献   

4.
Synchronization of activity cycles is common in coveys of gallinaceous birds, which usually include a dozen chicks at birth. To study this synchronization more precisely, we recorded the ultradian rhythms of activity of seven groups of young Japanese quail reared in the laboratory, without brooding females. Four groups included only kin-related birds (K1-K4), two mixed groups consisted of associated chicks from two families (M1 and M2 groups), and the seventh group (NK group) included only non-kin chicks. Numbers of chicks in a group varied from eight to 14, depending on the group. They were observed twice during their two first weeks of life (day 3 and day 10) and all activities were recorded by scan sampling. An ultradian rhythm of activity, with a period less than 20 minutes, was found. This period varied among quail families. All chick groups were globally synchronized, except group M2 on day 3. Inter-individual levels of synchronization were calculated. Spearman rank correlation coefficients of the non-kin chicks were significantly lower than those of kin-related chicks. This was especially clear on day 3. Survival of newborn quails is linked to their presence within a covey or a group. Their survival, as well that of their brothers and sisters, may be enhanced by an ability to synchronize with kin better than with non-kin chicks.  相似文献   

5.
The locomotor activity of the millipede Glyphiulus cavernicolus (Spirostreptida), which occupies the deeper recesses of a cave, was monitored in light-dark (LD) cycles (12h light and 12h darkness), constant darkness (DD), and constant light (LL) conditions. These millipedes live inside the cave and are apparently never exposed to any periodic factors of the environment such as light-dark, temperature, and humidity cycles. The activity of a considerable fraction of these millipedes was found to show circadian rhythm, which entrained to a 12:12 LD cycle with maximum activity during the dark phase of the LD cycle. Under constant darkness (DD), 56.5% of the millipedes (n = 23) showed circadian rhythms, with average free-running period of 25.7h ± 3.3h (mean ± SD, range 22.3h to 35.0h). The remaining 43.5% of the millipedes, however, did not show any clear-cut rhythm. Under DD conditions following an exposure to LD cycles, 66.7% (n = 9) showed faint circadian rhythm, with average free-running period of 24.0h ± 0.8h (mean ± SD, range 22.9h to 25.2h). Under constant light (LL) conditions, only 2 millipedes of 11 showed free-running rhythms, with average period length of 33.3h ± 1.3h. The results suggest that these cave-dwelling millipedes still possess the capacity to measure time and respond to light and dark situations. (Chronobiology International, 17(6), 757-765, 2000)  相似文献   

6.
Patterns of circadian and ultradian rhythms in the heart rate (HR) are described in a full-term baby with birth asphyxia and convulsions. A 24h HR recording was carried out at the age of 1, 15, 56, 289, and 295 days; West syndrome diagnosis was made when the patient was 3 months old. The HR showed no circadian rhythm in the follow-up, whereas it is known that the circadian rhythm appears in healthy infants at the age of 1 month and remains thereafter. This observation may be an indirect indicator of the interference of West syndrome with centers of neurological maturity. (Chronobiology International, 17(4), 591-595, 2000)  相似文献   

7.
Both a circadian clock and an ultradian clock (period 4—5 h) have previously been described for the ciliated protozoon Tetrahymena. The present communication demonstrates the existence of yet another cellular clock: an ultradian rhythm with a period of about 30 min. The period was found to be well temperature-compensated over the range studied, i.e., between 19°C and 33°C. Ultradian rhythmicity was initiated by dilution of stationary-phase cultures, which were kept previously in a light-dark cycle, into fresh medium. LD treatment during stationary phase was an absolute requirement, since cultures kept in either LL or DD did not produce the ultradian rhythmicity after refeeding. The clock exerts control over respiration; the observed oscillation in oxygen uptake is just a hand of the clock: after a limitation of oxygen supply had ended, the rhythm resumed with the same phase and period as that in control cultures. The clock exerts temporal control also over cell division; in the refed culture cell division resumed with an oscillation in the number of dividing organisms. The period of this oscillation corresponded to that of the rhythm in respiratory activity, indicating that the same ultradian clock may exert control over different cellular functions. Analysis of a second Tetrahymena strain indicates that period length of the ultradian clock is a strain-specific characteristic.  相似文献   

8.
Three types of rhythmic movements of Phaseolus vulgaris L. (pole beans) were examined collectively and their characteristics compared. Although the ultradian rhythms of shoot circumnutation and leaf movement, as well as the circadian rhythm of leaf movement, occurred simultaneously, each rhythm could be expressed independently of the other two. Shoot circumnutation and ultradian leaf movements displayed the same period (80 min at 25°C and Q10⋍2), while the period of the circadian leaf movements was not temperature dependent (Q10⋍1). Interaction into the plant between two ultradian rhythms (shoot circumnutation and ultradian leaf movement) with the same period and coexistence in the pulvinus of an ultradian with a circadian rhythm are discussed.  相似文献   

9.
During the first weeks of life, preterm neonates show fewer circadian rhythms in their physiological parameters than full-term neonates. To determine whether preterm neonates differ in their temporal adaptation to the daynight cycle from full-term neonates at the early age of 1 week, we compared activity-rest behavior of both groups. Activity-rest behavior of 10 neurologically healthy preterm neonates (born in 34th to 36th week of gestation) and 10 neurologically healthy full-term neonates (born in 37th to 42nd week of gestation) was monitored longitudinally for 8 successive days in the first 2 weeks of life. Actigraphy was used to register and display time patterns of activity and rest in neonates by using small actometers, which resemble a wristwatch. Nursing/feeding was recorded using the actometer's integrated event marker button. Recordings for preterm neonates were conducted in the hospital; recordings for full-term neonates were carried out in the hospital and in their homes. In addition to the actigraphic recordings, a standardized diary was kept regularly. To assess periodic characteristics, frequency components of activity-rest behavior were analyzed using fast Fourier transformation (FFT). Amounts of daily sleep time, nightly sleep time, and sleep time during 24h were compared. Nursing/feeding epochs were also analyzed for 5 preterm and 5 full-term neonates to compare their food intake behavior. The majority of preterm neonates showed a multitude of ultradian frequencies in their spectra. In contrast, several full-term neonates showed a distinct circadian frequency. In preterm neonates, average nightly sleep and average daily sleep of all recorded days were very similar, but after the fourth day of life, only average nightly sleep increased. In full-term neonates, average nightly and daily sleep time of all recorded days differed by about 1h. Average sleep time during 24h for preterm and full-term neonates was similar. Preterm neonates showed longer intervals between events of food intake than full-term neonates. The circadian peaks in the frequency spectra of full-term neonates may indicate the initial adaptation in the first week of life to a 24h day. This is in agreement with our results concerning the different durations of nightly and daily sleep. The increase in nightly sleep time of preterm neonates may be attributed to the progressing adaptation to a circadian activity- rest pattern. (Chronobiology International, 18(4), 697-708, 2001)  相似文献   

10.
Studies suggest some physiologic, cognitive, and behavioral 24h rhythms are generated by cyclic components that are shorter in period than circadian. The aim of this study was [1] to examine the hypothesis that 24h human performance rhythms arise from the integration of high-frequency endogenous components and [2] to quantify the contribution of each higher frequency component to the phenotype of the rhythm. We monitored the performance of 9 experienced pilots by employing an array of cognitive-based tests conducted in a flight simulator so that, over the 6-day experiment, data were obtained for each 2h interval of the 24h. The activity-rest schedule of the subjects, no matter the exact clock time schedule of sleep and activity, always consisted of 14h activity (when they carried out regular professional duties) and 10h rest, with at least 8h of sleep. The simulated combat scenarios consisted of simple and complex tasks associated with target interception, aircraft maneuvering, and target shooting and downing. The results yielded two indices: the number of prominent periodicities in the time series and the relative magnitude of the amplitude of each relative to the construction of the composite 24h waveform. Three cyclic components (8h, 12h, and 24h) composed the observed 24h performance pattern. The dominant period and acrophase (peak time) of the compound output rhythm were determined by the interplay between the amplitudes of the various individual ultradian components. Task complexity (workload) increases the expression of the ultradian entities in the 24h pattern. We constructed a model composed of the multiple ultradian components; the composite output defined a “time span” (of 2h-4h duration) as opposed to an exact “time point” of high and low performance, endowing elevated functional capability. (Chronobiology International, 18(6), 987-1003, 2001)  相似文献   

11.
A circadian rhythm for visual sensitivity has been intensively assessed in animals. This rhythm may be due to the existence of a circadian clock in the mammalian eye, which could account for fluctuating sensitivity to light over the day in certain species. However, very few studies have been devoted to the human visual system. The present experiment was designed to assess a possible rhythm of visual sensitivity using a psychophysical method over the whole 24h period. Twelve subjects underwent visual detection threshold measures in a protocol that allowed one point every 2h. The results show that the visual detection threshold changes over the 24h period, with high thresholds in the morning, a progressive decrease over the day and the early night, and an increase during the last part of the night. These data suggest that a circadian rhythm influences visual sensitivity to mesopic luminance in humans. (Chronobiology International, 17(6), 795-805, 2000)  相似文献   

12.
Observations on the rhythmic activity of 71 juvenile specimens of the inter-tidal blenny Zoarces viviparus reveal an endogenous pattern of swimming at three different periodicities. Circatidal swimming, with activity peaks phased to high water or the ebb of the subjective 12.4-h tides, was found in 50 fish and was the predominant pattern seen immediately after collection, when the rhythm generally persisted for between 3 and 12 cycles. Discrete activity peaks, with a free running period of approximately 24 h were also evident in the swimming pattern of eight fish. A circadian influence was also manifest as a modulation in amplitude, phase shifts and changes in free-running period of the circa-tidal rhythm. Overall, the activity level declined with time but those fish that remained active long enough showed a semi-lunar rhythm, with maximum activity at the time of the spring tides. A comparison of the behavior of animals collected at different times of the year suggests a seasonal variation in the persistence of circatidal swimming. The results are consistent with a control system involving circatidal, circadian, and semi-lunar oscillators. (Chronobiology International, 18(1), 27-46, 2001)  相似文献   

13.
In the free-running circadian locomotor activity rhythm of a 7-year-old male owl monkey (Aotus lemurinus griseimembra) kept under constant light and climatic conditions (LL 0.2 lux, 25°C ± 1°C, 60 ± 5% relative humidity [RH]), a second rhythm component developed that showed strong relative coordination with the free-running activity rhythm of 24.4h and a 24h rhythm. The simultaneously recorded feeding activity rhythm strongly resembled this rhythm component. Therefore, it seems justified to infer that there was an internal desynchronization between the two behavioral rhythms or their circadian pacemakers, that is, between the light-entrainable oscillator located in the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) and a food-entrainable oscillator located outside the SCN. This internal desynchronization may have been induced and/or maintained by a zeitgeber effect of the (irregular) 24h feeding schedule on the food-entrainable oscillator. The weak relative coordination shown by the activity rhythm indicates a much weaker coupling of the light-entrainable oscillator to the food-entrainable oscillator than vice versa. (Chronobiology International, 17(2), 147-153, 2000)  相似文献   

14.
15.
In constant conditions (constant darkness [DD], 20°C), the sand beach isopod Cirolana cookii exhibits spontaneous rhythmic swimming activity with an average free-running period of 12.5h. The rhythms are seen as temporal adaptations to a complex intertidal environment. These results support a dual circalunidian clock model for tidal rhythms in which two components of the rhythm have characteristic periods and active phase lengths and are hypothesized to be controlled by separate circalunidian clocks. A quantitative model successfully simulates many of the properties of endogenous swimming rhythms of C. cookii, including free-running behavior, entrainment, and phase-response curves (PRCs). (Chronobiology International 17 (1), 29-41, 2000)  相似文献   

16.
Neuropeptide Y (NPY) is a potent orexigenic peptide that is implicated in the feeding response to a variety of stimuli. The current studies employed mice lacking NPY (Npy−/−) and their wild-type (Npy+/+) littermates to investigate the role of this peptide in the feeding response to circadian and palatability cues. To investigate the response to a circadian stimulus, we assessed food intake during the 4-h period following dark onset, a time of day characterized by maximal rates of food consumption. Compared to Npy+/+ controls, intake of Npy/− mice was reduced by 33% during this period (0.6 ± 0.1 g versus 0.9 ± 0.1 g; p ≤ 0.05). In contrast, intake did not differ between genotypes when measured over a 24-h period (3.7 ± 0.2 g versus 3.5 ± 0.3 g; p = ns). Furthermore, reduced dark cycle 4 h food intake in Npy/− mice was not evident after a 24-h fast (1.4 ± 0.1 g for both genotypes; p = ns), despite a pronounced delay in the initiation of feeding (636 ± 133 s versus 162 ± 29 s; p ≤ 0.05). To investigate the role of NPY in the feeding response to palatability cues, mice were presented with a highly palatable diet (HP) for 1 h each day (in addition to having ad libitum access to chow) for 18 days. Npy+/+ mice rapidly increased daily HP intake such that by the end of the first week, they derived a substantial fraction of daily energy from this source (41 ± 3%). By comparison, HP intake was markedly reduced in Npy−/− mice during the first week (24 ± 7% of daily energy intake, p ≤ 0.05 versus Npy+/+), although it eventually increased (by Day 9) to values comparable to those of Npy+/+ controls. These experiments suggest that NPY contributes to the mechanism whereby food intake increases in response to circadian and palatability cues and that mechanisms driving food intake in response to these stimuli differ from those activated by energy restriction.  相似文献   

17.
The influence of environmental (extracellular) pH on the sporulation rhythm in Neurospora crassa was investigated for wild-type (frq+) and the mutants chr, frq1, frq7, and frq8. In all mutants, including wild type, the growth rate was found to be influenced strongly by extracellular pH in the range 4-9. On the other hand, for the same pH range, the period length of the sporulation rhythm is little influenced in wild type, chr, and frq1. A loss of pH homeostasis of the period, however, was observed in the mutants frq7 and frq8, which also are known to have lost temperature compensation. Concerning the influence of extracellular pH on growth rates, a clear correspondence between growth rates and the concentration of available H2PO4- ion has been found, indicating that the uptake of H2PO4- may be a limiting factor for growth under our experimental conditions. The loss of pH compensation in the frq7 and frq8 mutants may be related to less easily degradable FRQ7,8 proteins when compared with wild-type FRQ. Results from recent model considerations and experimental results predict that, with increasing extra-and intracellular pH, the FRQ7 protein degradation increases and should lead to shorter period lengths. (Chronobiology International, 17(6), 733-750, 2000)  相似文献   

18.
The variation in spontaneous meal patterning was studied in three genotypes (tau +/+, tau +/-and tau -/-) of the Syrian hamster with an altered circadian period. Feeding activity was monitored continuously in 13 individuals from each genotype in constant dim light conditions. All three genotypes had on average six feeding episodes during the circadian cycle (about 20h in homozygous tau mutants and 22h in heterozygotes compared with 24h in wild-type hamsters). Thus, homozygous tau mutant hamsters had significantly more feeding episodes per 24h than wild types, and heterozygotes were intermediate. The average duration of feeding bouts (FBs) was indistinguishable (around 30 minutes) among the three genotypes, whereas the intermeal (IM) intervals were significantly shorter for homozygote tau mutant hamsters (99 minutes), intermediate for heterozygotes (116 minutes), and the longest for wild-type hamsters (148 minutes). Thus, the meal-to-meal duration was on average 25% shorter in homozygous tau mutants (16% in heterozygous) than in wild-type hamsters. The reduction of the circadian period has a pronounced effect on short-term feeding rhythms and meal frequency in hamsters carrying the tau mutation. (Chronobiology International, 18(4), 657-664, 2001)  相似文献   

19.
A clutch of young chicks housed with a mother hen exhibit ultradian (within day) rhythms of activity corresponding to the brooding cycle of the hen. In the present study clear evidence was found of ultradian activity rhythms in newly hatched domestic chicks housed in groups larger than natural clutch size without a mother hen or any other obvious external time-keeper. No consistent synchrony was found between groups housed in different pens within the same room. The ultradian rhythms disappeared with time and little evidence of group rhythmicity remained by the third night. This disappearance over time suggests that the presence of a mother hen may be pivotal for the long-term maintenance of these rhythms. The ultradian rhythm of the chicks may also play an important role in the initiation of brooding cycles during the behavioural transition of the mother hen from incubation to brooding. Computer simulations of individual activity rhythms were found to reproduce the observations made on a group basis. This was achievable even when individual chick rhythms were modelled as independent of each other, thus no assumptions of social facilitation are necessary to obtain ultradian activity rhythms on a group level.  相似文献   

20.
TGR(mREN2)27 (TGR) transgenic rats develop hypertension due to the mouse mRen-2 gene inserted in their genome. At 5 weeks of age, the blood pressure of TGR rats starts rising, until a maximum is reached at 10 weeks of age. Adult TGR rats show peak values of blood pressure (BP) during the light phase, while heart rate (HR) and motor activity (MA) peak at night. In the present experiment, we evaluated the evolution of circadian rhythms in motor activity, heart rate, and blood pressure of TGR and Sprague-Dawley (SD) rats under 12h light-dark cycles (LD 12:12). Results confirmed that the blood pressure of TGR rats starts to increase at 5 weeks of age, reaching a plateau by the 11th week. Parallel to the increase in blood pressure levels, there was a decrease in the period length of the blood pressure rhythm, a delay in the onset of the alpha phase of the blood pressure rhythm with respect to that of motor activity and heart rate, and a decrease in heart rate levels. In all of the variables studied, the alpha phase of SD rats always started before darkness, whereas that of TGR rats started after lights off. In general, heart rate and motor activity levels of TGR rats were higher than those of SD rats. The amplitude of the circadian rhythms studied was greater in TGR rats than in SD rats. The present results suggest that the different evolution of circadian rhythms in TGR and SD rats might be due to differences in the functioning of the entrainment pathway or the circadian clock itself, which can be detected in young rats and that are probably caused by the expression of the mouse transgene. (Chronobiology International, 18(4), 627-640, 2001)  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号