共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
2.
《Somatosensory & motor research》2013,30(4):105-110
To investigate the influence of vision and motor imagery styles on equilibrium control, displacements of the supporting foot during spontaneous whole-body rotations (“pirouette”) by expert female ballet dancers were analyzed using three-dimensional kinematics. Four turn types were defined according to direction (clockwise, CW vs. counterclockwise, CCW) and supporting foot (SF, left vs. right). Visual influences were examined by including two visual conditions (blindfolded vs. full-vision). Motor imagery styles were determined using the Vividness of Movement Imagery Questionnaire (VMIQ) (Kinesthetic, n = 4 vs. Visual/Kinesthetic, n = 6). Turning direction preference was assessed by a closed-response questionnaire in which all dancers indicated that they preferred CW turn direction. Kinesthetic dancers showed more SF displacement during CCW (non-preferred direction) than CW (preferred direction) pirouettes. However, Visual/Kinesthetic dancers showed no significant effect of turn direction. Furthermore, Kinesthetic dancers showed no significant effect of vision on SF displacement whereas Visual/Kinesthetic dancers showed significantly higher SF displacement when vision was occluded. Thus there appears to be a selective effect of vision on Visual/Kinesthetic dancers, and a selective effect of turn direction on Kinesthetic dancers. These results suggest that perceptual styles should be taken into consideration when training tasks that require fine equilibrium control because the factors that perturb balance differ depending on perceptual style. 相似文献
3.
Role of the primary motor cortex in the early boost in performance following mental imagery training
Recently, it has been suggested that the primary motor cortex (M1) plays a critical role in implementing the fast and transient post-training phase of motor skill consolidation, known to yield an early boost in performance. Whether a comparable early boost in performance occurs following motor imagery (MIM) training is still unknown. To address this issue, two groups of subjects learned a finger tapping sequence either by MIM or physical practice (PP). In both groups, performance increased significantly in the post-training phase when compared with the pre-training phase and further increased after a 30 min resting period, indicating that both MIM and PP trainings were equally efficient and induced an early boost in motor performance. This conclusion was corroborated by the results of an additional control group. In a second experiment, we then investigated the causal role of M1 in implementing the early boost process resulting from MIM training. To do so, we inhibited M1 by applying a continuous theta-burst stimulation (cTBS) in healthy volunteers just after they learnt, by MIM, the same finger-tapping task as in Experiment #1. As a control, cTBS was applied over the vertex of subjects who underwent the same experiment. We found that cTBS applied over M1 selectively abolished the early boost process subsequent to MIM training. Altogether, the present study provides evidence that MIM practice induces an early boost in performance and demonstrates that M1 is causally involved in this process. These findings further divulge some degree of behavioral and neuronal similitude between MIM and PP. 相似文献
4.
Human movement control requires adequate coordination of different movements, which is particularly important when different
motor tasks are simultaneously executed by the same effector(s) (e.g. a muscle or a joint). The process of movement execution
involves a series of highly nonlinear elements; for instance, a motor unit of a muscle produces force only in the direction
of muscle shortening, thus representing a threshold operator that transforms the bipolar (i.e. excitatory or inhibitory) information
at its spinal input into a purely unipolar signal (i.e. muscle force). This tripartite research report addresses the contribution
of the nonlinearity of neuromuscular elements to the coordination of different motor tasks simultaneously executed by the
same limb. In this first part of the series, a new hypothesis for such a single-muscle multiple-task coordination is presented
which suggests an essentially threshold-linear coordination mechanism. Control signals generated by the central nervous system
for each individual movement independently and feedback information from peripheral receptors are linearly superimposed. This
compound control/feedback signal is processed by a nonlinear limiter element reflecting the discontinuous properties of the
muscle and its reflex circuitry. It is shown that threshold-linear interaction of descending commands and afferent feedback
information can lead to complex interdependent patterns of compound motor action. This includes the possibility of gating
(i.e. the ability of one movement pattern to constrain or even impede the execution of another pattern) and of delayed response
initiation when simultaneously performing more than one voluntary motor task. A theoretical analysis of the threshold-linear
coordination mechanism and an extensive experimental validation of the model is provided in part II and part III of the report.
Received: 6 October 1998 / Accepted in revised form: 2 June 1999 相似文献
5.
Hidefumi Sasaki Yukio Urabe Noriaki Maeda Toshiaki Suzuki 《Somatosensory & motor research》2018,35(1):18-24
Purpose: This study aimed to validate the preliminary steps of motor image voluntary training in patients who are prone to falling as toe flexion muscle strength decreases.Materials and methods: We recorded the F-wave in 30 healthy subjects (20 men, 10 women; mean age, 22.5?±?2.1?years). First, in a resting condition, the muscle was relaxed during the F-wave recording. Subsequently, the motion of the left flexor hallucis brevis muscle is photographed. F-waves were recorded immediately and at 5, 10, and 15?min after motor imagery. The amplitude of the F/M ratio and persistence were measured. The intervention group watched the exercise task video used for F-wave measurement daily for 1?month, whereas the non-intervention group did not. The second measurement was performed 1?month later in each group.Results: In the first measurement of the amplitude of the F/M ratio in both intervention and non-intervention groups, the image condition was significantly increased compared with the resting condition, but there was no significant difference in persistence. A significant decrease in the amplitude of the F/M ratio after image conditioning was observed in the second measurement of the intervention group.Conclusion: Although spinal nerve function excitement was enhanced during motor imagery, movement suppression was promoted, and spinal nerve excitability was suppressed when repeating the simple task. In the future, gradually upscaling the difficulty level of the toe flexion motor task used in the motor image may be necessary to prevent falls. 相似文献
6.
Nobuo Kawabata 《Biological cybernetics》1974,15(4):237-242
Dynamics of the EEG activity — during the performance of a mental task — was investigated by the nonstationary power spectrum method. The performance of the mental arithmetic is associated with suppression of the alpha wave. The suppression of the alpha wave is not as marked as the alpha blocking accompanying opening of the eyes. Alpha wave suppression during the mental task was nonsymmetric around the center frequency of the alpha wave, and lower frequency components were suppressed more than high frequency components. An explanation of these observations is proposed in terms of decrease in the periods of alpha prevalence during performance of mental tasks. 相似文献
7.
Visual- and motor imagery rely primarily on perceptual and motor processes, respectively. In healthy controls, the type of imagery used to solve a task depends on personal preference, task instruction, and task properties. But how does the chronic loss of proprioceptive and tactile sensory inputs from the body periphery influence mental imagery? In a unique case study, we investigated the imagery capabilities of the chronically deafferented patient IW when he was performing a mental rotation task. We found that IW''s motor imagery processes were impaired and that visual imagery processes were enhanced compared to controls. These results suggest that kinaesthetic afferent signals from the body periphery play a crucial role in enabling and maintaining central sensorimotor representations and hence the ability to incorporate kinaesthetic information into the imagery processes. 相似文献
8.
J. F. Soechting 《Biological cybernetics》1973,14(1):25-34
The relationship between the movement's parameters and the motor output during the execution of certain intentional motor tasks subsequent to a ballistically initiated movement is determined. The two tasks considered are to arrest the movement and to accelerate it as fast as possible. These experiments are the same as described in the preceding paper (Viviani and Terzuolo, 1973). It is shown that the motor output is dependent on sensory input in normal subjects and that this dependence is absent in cerebellar patients. The phase relations between motor output and angular displacement in normal subjects indicate the likelihood of fusimotor dynamic activity to the muscle spindles when the task is to arrest the movement. Instead, when normal subjects are instructed to accelerate the movement, an appropriately timed fusimotor static activity, i.e. alpha-gamma linkage, is indicated. The appropriately timed switching of fusimotor static and dynamic activation is attributed to the presence of cerebellar activities. 相似文献
9.
The physiological basis of mental states can be effectively studied by combining cognitive psychology with human neuroscience. Recent research has employed mental motor imagery in normal and brain-damaged subjects to decipher the content and the structure of covert processes preceding the execution of action. The mapping of brain activity during motor imagery discloses a pattern of activation similar to that of an executed action. 相似文献
10.
Mental imagery has been proposed to contribute to a variety of high-level cognitive functions, including memory encoding and retrieval, navigation, spatial planning, and even social communication and language comprehension. However, it is debated whether mental imagery relies on the same sensory representations as perception, and if so, what functional consequences such an overlap might have on perception itself. We report novel evidence that single instances of imagery can have a pronounced facilitatory influence on subsequent conscious perception. Either seeing or imagining a specific pattern could strongly bias which of two competing stimuli reach awareness during binocular rivalry. Effects of imagery and perception were location and orientation specific, accumulated in strength over time, and survived an intervening visual task lasting several seconds prior to presentation of the rivalry display. Interestingly, effects of imagery differed from those of feature-based attention. The results demonstrate that imagery, in the absence of any incoming visual signals, leads to the formation of a short-term sensory trace that can bias future perception, suggesting a means by which high-level processes that support imagination and memory retrieval may shape low-level sensory representations. 相似文献
11.
12.
Dietrich A 《Methods (San Diego, Calif.)》2008,45(4):319-324
Sports and exercise psychology finds itself in a most unfortunate situation these days. While all other branches of the psychological sciences help themselves freely to the glitzy new toys of modern neuroscience—MRI and PET, mostly—exploring the neural underpinnings of whatever cognitive function they are interested in exploring, the sport sciences are left out of the fun for the simple reason that these imaging instruments preclude motion—the very thing then that is the subject of interest to them. There are several legitimate ways around this problem but the one that seems to be most popular is, I think, not—legitimate, that is. The basic idea, unduly sharpened here, is the following. Neuroimaging studies have shown that imagined and actual motion share the same neural substrates or, alternatively, imagining an action corresponds to a subliminal activation of the same brain areas required for its execution. It follows from this, the arguments runs, that motor imagery can be used as a proxy for real motor performance, et voilà, the sports sciences can go wild with all the snazzy brain imaging tools after all—just like everyone else. This notion is, I believe, misbegotten, a house of cards that threatens to cast a long shadow over the field. The present article, then, is, to be frank, intended to put a machete to this kind of thinking. It does this by exposing this conclusion to be based on an unholy marriage of selective data reporting and gross overgeneralization. The result is a wild goose chase fueled by wishful thinking. 相似文献
13.
As the old adage goes: practice makes perfect. Yet, the neural mechanisms by which rote repetition transforms a halting behavior into a fluid, effortless, and “automatic” action are not well understood. Here we consider the possibility that well-practiced motor sequences, which initially rely on higher-level decision-making circuits, become wholly specified in lower-level control circuits. We review studies informing this idea, discuss the constraints on such shift in control, and suggest approaches to pinpoint circuit-level changes associated with motor sequence learning. 相似文献
14.
The purpose of this study was to determine the effect of arousal in men and women on the moment-to-moment performance of a simple motor task. We examined the control of a precision task in the presence and absence of imposed stressors. Twenty-nine subjects (14 men, 15 women; 18--44 yr) were randomly assigned to either a control group or one of two stressor groups, Mental Math or Electric Shock. Subjects presented with Math and Shock stressors, which lasted 10 min, experienced significant increases in cognitive and physiological arousal compared with baseline and control subjects. Heart rate, systolic blood pressure, and electrodermal activity were elevated 5--80% with presentation of the stressors, whereas diastolic blood pressure and salivary cortisol were unchanged. The greater levels of cognitive and physiological arousal were associated with reductions in steadiness of a pinch grip for the Shock subjects (approximately 130% reduction from baseline) but not for the subjects in the Math group, who experienced heightened arousal but no change in steadiness (10% reduction from baseline). Although women exhibited more of a reduction in steadiness than men, the effect was largely unrelated to the magnitude of the change in arousal. 相似文献
15.
The aim of the study was to assess if a simple motor task, one that required muscle contractions well below maximum, showed evidence of circadian changes and time-awake. The task consisted of using a larger counter to flick a number of smaller counters to land as near as possible to the center of a target. The closer a counter landed next to the center of the target, the higher the score obtained. Two distances from the target were used (long and short), and 20 counters were flicked at each distance. The task was performed by 72 diurnally active healthy participants at six test sessions distributed every 4 h throughout the day (08:00 h, 12:00 h, ... , 04:00 h), so covering a circadian cycle. When performing the sessions, subjects had been awake for about 1, 4, ... , 20 h. Before each test session, sublingual temperature was measured, and estimates of the individual's fatigue and alertness were made. Clear normally phased circadian rhythms (p<0.0001) in oral temperature and alertness with mean peak time (i.e., acrophases of 17.2 h and 15.9 h, respectively) and fatigue (i.e., mean acrophase of 3.4 h) were detected. The total scores for both the long and short distances also showed circadian rhythms that peaked slightly before the temperature rhythm (by 2.31+/-0.91 h and 1.77+/-0.77 h, mean+/-SE, respectively), and the number of occasions that the target was missed altogether showed rhythms that were in anti-phase (mean acrophases=3.8 h and 4.1 h for the long and short distances, respectively) to that of total scores (mean acrophases=16.0 h and 15.2 h for the long and short distances, respectively). With the long and, particularly, short distances, there were generally significant correlations (r<0.0005) between both the measures of accuracy (total score and number of misses) and body temperature and time-awake. The accuracy of performance at this task seems to show circadian and time-awake effects, and so makes it of potential value in protocols where repetitive measurements during the course of a day are required. 相似文献
16.
17.
Movement-related potentials (MRPs) recorded from the brain are thought to vary during learning of a motor task. However,
since MRPs are recorded at a very low signal-to-noise ratio, it is difficult to measure these variations. In this study we
attempt to remove most of the accompanying noise thus enabling the tracking of transient phenomena in MRPs recorded during
learning of a motor task. Subjects performed a simple motor task which required learning. A modified version of the matching
pursuit algorithm was used in order to remove a significant portion of the electroencephalographic noise overlapping the MRPs
recorded in the experiment. Small groups of MRPs were then averaged according to experimental parameters. Our results show
that the power of the MRPs does not decay uniformly during learning. Instead, there is a significant peak in their power
after 4 or 5 repetitions of the task. This peak is noticeable especially in electrodes placed over the prefrontal region of
the cortex at times subsequent to the actual movement. The observed pattern of activity may indicate problem solving related
to comprehension of the force against which the user performed the task. It is possible that this problem solving occurs in
the prefrontal cortex.
Received: 27 December 2000 / Accepted in revised form: 26 April 2001 相似文献
18.
Separate, causal roles of the caudate in saccadic choice and execution in a perceptual decision task
In contrast to the well-established roles of the striatum in movement generation and value-based decisions, its contributions to perceptual decisions lack direct experimental support. Here, we show that electrical microstimulation in the monkey caudate nucleus influences both choice and saccade response time on a visual motion discrimination task. Within a drift-diffusion framework, these effects consist of two components. The perceptual component biases choices toward ipsilateral targets, away from the neurons' predominantly contralateral response fields. The choice bias is consistent with a nonzero starting value of the diffusion process, which increases and decreases decision times for contralateral and ipsilateral choices, respectively. The nonperceptual component decreases and increases nondecision times toward contralateral and ipsilateral targets, respectively, consistent with the caudate's role in saccade generation. The results imply a causal role for the caudate in perceptual decisions used to select saccades that may be distinct from its role in executing those saccades. VIDEO ABSTRACT: 相似文献
19.
Kasamatsu K Suzuki S Anse M Funada MF Idogawa K Ninomija SP 《Journal of PHYSIOLOGICAL ANTHROPOLOGY and Applied Human Science》2002,21(6):285-290
The purpose of this research was to identify the relationship between task performance and menstrual cycle. The difference of performance on menstrual cycle phase was investigated. The task was the mental arithmetic task which involved the non-sequential and higher order cognitive processes. The duration of the experiment was twenty minutes. Two-way analysis of variance by repeated-measures design was used to examine the differences in task performance between phases and temporal variations. Results showed that there was a significant difference in correct input time during temporal variations though there was no significant difference between phases. Moreover, the relationships between phases and intra-individual variations in task performance were examined using coefficient of variance (CV). CVs were plotted in three dimensions to examine the relationships between intra-individual variations and phases. Based on CVs, the subjects who showed differences were classified into two groups: those with a small difference in three phases and those with a difference every phase. The phase which indicated large CV changed with individuals. 相似文献
20.
1. Recordings were made of the natural dischages of neurones in the supplementary motor area (SMA) of conscious monkeys trained to perform a stereotyped motor task with either hand. 2. Eighty % of the total population of cells showed modulation of their activity during particular movements of either limb. Two thirds of this group had a similar pattern of modulation regardless of whether the contralateral or ipsilateral hand was used. 3. The number of cells whose activity was related to movements of distal joints was approximately equal to that whose discharges occurred with proximal movements. 4. Only 5% of cells tested sent their axons into the pyramidal tract, and only 15% of units investigated showed responses to passive manipulation of the limbs. The effective afferent input usually was of a rather complex kind. 5. The findings suggest that the discharges of a large number of neurones in SMA are changing during particular movements of either arm, and that only a small number of cells receive afferent sensory input. These results contrast with those obtained in the primary motor area and suggest a different role for SMA the control of movement. 相似文献