共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Prescott TJ Diamond ME Wing AM 《Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences》2011,366(1581):2989-2995
Active sensing systems are purposive and information-seeking sensory systems. Active sensing usually entails sensor movement, but more fundamentally, it involves control of the sensor apparatus, in whatever manner best suits the task, so as to maximize information gain. In animals, active sensing is perhaps most evident in the modality of touch. In this theme issue, we look at active touch across a broad range of species from insects, terrestrial and marine mammals, through to humans. In addition to analysing natural touch, we also consider how engineering is beginning to exploit physical analogues of these biological systems so as to endow robots with rich tactile sensing capabilities. The different contributions show not only the varieties of active touch--antennae, whiskers and fingertips--but also their commonalities. They explore how active touch sensing has evolved in different animal lineages, how it serves to provide rapid and reliable cues for controlling ongoing behaviour, and even how it can disintegrate when our brains begin to fail. They demonstrate that research on active touch offers a means both to understand this essential and primary sensory modality, and to investigate how animals, including man, combine movement with sensing so as to make sense of, and act effectively in, the world. 相似文献
2.
The transport of substances across cell membranes may be the most fundamental activity of living things. When the substance transported is any ion there can be a change in the concentration of hydrogen ions on the two sides of the membrane. These hydrogen ion concentration changes are not caused by fluxes of hydrogen ions although fluxes of hydrogen ions may sometimes be involved. The reason for the apparent contradiction is quite simple. All aqueous systems are subject to two constraints: (1) to maintain the charge balance, the sum of the cationic charges must equal the sum of the anionic charges and (2) the product of the molar concentration of H+ and the molar concentration of OH−, established and maintained by the association and the dissociation of water, remains always at 10−14. As a consequence the concentrations of H+ and OH− are determined uniquely by differences between the concentrations of the other cations and anions, with [H+] and [OH−] being dependent variables. Hydrogen ions and hydroxyl ions can be produced or consumed in local reactions whereas any strong ions such as Cl−, Mg2+, or K+ can be neither produced nor consumed in biological reactions. Further consequences of these truisms are outlined here in terms of the chemistry of the kinds of reactions which can lead to pH changes. 相似文献
3.
4.
Comer C Baba Y 《Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences》2011,366(1581):3006-3015
Orthopteroid insects (cockroaches, crickets, locusts and related species) allow examination of active sensory processing in a comparative framework. Some orthopteroids possess long, mobile antennae endowed with many chemo- and mechanoreceptors. When the antennae are touched, an animal's response depends upon the identity of the stimulus. For example, contact with a predator may lead to escape, but contact with a conspecific may usually not. Active touch of an approaching object influences the likelihood that a discrimination of identity will be made. Using cockroaches, we have identified specific descending mechanosensory interneurons that trigger antennal-mediated escape. Crucial sensory input to these cells comes from chordotonal organs within the antennal base. However, information from other receptors on the base or the long antennal flagellum allows active touch to modulate escape probability based on stimulus identity. This is conveyed, at least to some extent, by textural information. Guidance of the antennae in active exploration depends on visual information. Some of the visual interneurons and the motor neurons necessary for visuomotor control have been identified. Comparisons across Orthoptera suggest an evolutionary model where subtle changes in the architecture of interneurons, and of sensorimotor control loops, may explain differing levels of vision-touch interaction in the active guidance of behaviour. 相似文献
5.
Schuster S 《Current biology : CB》2008,18(4):R176-R178
A recent study has shown that, unusually, both the sensory and motor capabilities of an electric fish are omnidirectional. This matching of motor and sensory spaces helps the fish to hunt prey efficiently - particularly important given their energetically costly active sensory system. 相似文献
6.
Tkachenko PV Bobyntsev II 《Rossi?skii fiziologicheski? zhurnal imeni I.M. Sechenova / Rossi?skaia akademiia nauk》2010,96(11):1107-1113
The intrasensory and sensor-effector interactions between inter-spike intervals of acoustic stem-evoked potentials with parameters of hands coordination was found with the help of correlation analysis. Role of excitation of ascending fibres of rostral part of the pons and lateral loop in mechanisms of the auditory-motor coordination was determined. Sex-dependent differences in integration of elements of the sensor-motor system because of lateralization of incoming auditory information and complication of the motor task performed were revealed. In men, mechanisms of auditory-motor coordination have less determined character than in women. That probably determines higher level of bi-manual coordination in men. Optimal correct realization of motor program in men is performed prognostically, while in women--after a mistake has been done. 相似文献
7.
Bruce C. Jayne Jeffrey P. Olberding Dilip Athreya Michael A. Riley 《Journal of comparative physiology. A, Neuroethology, sensory, neural, and behavioral physiology》2012,198(12):905-913
Movement and searching behaviors at diverse spatial scales are important for understanding how animals interact with their environment. Although the shapes of branches and the voids in arboreal habitats seem likely to affect searching behaviors, their influence is poorly understood. To gain insights into how both environmental structure and the attributes of an animal may affect movement and searching, we compared the three-dimensional exploratory movements of snakes in the dark on two simulated arboreal surfaces (disc and horizontal cylinder). Most of the exploratory movements of snakes in the dark were a small fraction of the distances they could reach while bridging gaps in the light. The snakes extended farther away from the edge of the supporting surface at the ends of the cylinder than from the sides of the cylinder or from any direction from the surface of the disc. The exploratory movements were not random, and the surface shape and three-dimensional directions had significant interactive effects on how the movements were structured in time. Thus, the physical capacity for reaching did not limit the area that was explored, but the shape of the supporting surface and the orientation relative to gravity did create biased searching patterns. 相似文献
8.
An experimental system incorporating a computerized micro-impedance unit has been used to make direct measurements of the activity of copepod cephalic appendages. As the appendages are used to both propel the copepod through water and handle particles, it follows that appendage activity reflects feeding behaviour.To investigate the sensory feeding behaviour of copepods, their activity was recorded with food stimuli varying in size and chemical composition. Sample impedance traces are given for the appendage movements of Temora longicornis in the presence of: 1 — filtered seawater; 2 — beads; 3 — phytoplankton cells; 4 — dissolved free amino acids. The normal appendage movements shown in filtered seawater were modified when copepods were offered particles and dissolved chemicals. Results show that chemical and mechanical stimuli are responsible for the recognition and selection of food. Impedance traces distinguish between behavioural responses such as: antennule flicks, leg kicks, combing, handling and rejection of particles. Spectral analyses of traces have demonstrated that differences in beat pattern are significant.Paper presentation, at the Third International Conference on Copepoda. British Museum (Natural History), London, U. K., 10–14 August, 1987. 相似文献
9.
Spudich JL 《Biophysical chemistry》1995,56(1-2):165-169
Sensory rhodopsin I (SR-I lambda(max) 587 nm) is a phototaxis receptor in the archaeon Halobacterium salinarium. Photoisomerization of retinal in SR-I generates a long-lived intermediate with lambda(max) 373 nm which transmits a signal to the membrane-bound transducer protein HtrI. Although SR-I is structurally similar to the electrogenic proton pump bacteriorhodopsin (BR), early studies showed its photoreactions do not pump protons, nor result in membrane hyperpolarization. These studies used functionally active SR-I, that is, SR-I complexed with its transducer HtrI. Using recombinant DNA methods we have expressed SR-I protein containing mutations in ionizable residues near the protonated Schiff base, and studied wild-type and site-specifically mutated SR-I in the presence and absence of the transducer protein. UV-Vis kinetic absorption spectroscopy, FT-IR, and pH and membrane potential probes reveal transducer-free SR-I photoreactions result in vectorial proton translocation across the membrane in the same direction as that of BR. This proton pumping is suppressed by interaction with transducer which diverts the proton movements into an electroneutral path. A key step in this diversion is that transducer interaction raises the pK(a) of the aspartyl residue in SR-I (Asp76) which corresponds to the primary proton-accepting residue in the BR pump (Asp85). In transducer-free SR-I, our evidence indicates the pK(a) of Asp76 is 7.2, and ionized Asp76 functions as the Schiff base proton acceptor in the SR-I pump. In the SR-I/HtrI complex, the pK(a) of Asp76 is 8.5, and therefore at physiological pH (7.4) Asp76 is neutral. Protonation changes on Asp76 are clearly not required for signaling since the SR-I mutants D76N and D76A are active in phototaxis. The latent proton-translocation potential of SR-I may reflect the evolution of the SR-I sensory signaling mechanism from the proton pumping mechanism of BR. 相似文献
10.
11.
Voluntary motor commands produce two kinds of consequences. Initially, a sensory consequence is observed in terms of activity in our primary sensory organs (e.g., vision, proprioception). Subsequently, the brain evaluates the sensory feedback and produces a subjective measure of utility or usefulness of the motor commands (e.g., reward). As a result, comparisons between predicted and observed consequences of motor commands produce two forms of prediction error. How do these errors contribute to changes in motor commands? Here, we considered a reach adaptation protocol and found that when high quality sensory feedback was available, adaptation of motor commands was driven almost exclusively by sensory prediction errors. This form of learning had a distinct signature: as motor commands adapted, the subjects altered their predictions regarding sensory consequences of motor commands, and generalized this learning broadly to neighboring motor commands. In contrast, as the quality of the sensory feedback degraded, adaptation of motor commands became more dependent on reward prediction errors. Reward prediction errors produced comparable changes in the motor commands, but produced no change in the predicted sensory consequences of motor commands, and generalized only locally. Because we found that there was a within subject correlation between generalization patterns and sensory remapping, it is plausible that during adaptation an individual''s relative reliance on sensory vs. reward prediction errors could be inferred. We suggest that while motor commands change because of sensory and reward prediction errors, only sensory prediction errors produce a change in the neural system that predicts sensory consequences of motor commands. 相似文献
12.
《Current biology : CB》2023,33(4):780-789.e4
- Download : Download high-res image (127KB)
- Download : Download full-size image
13.
Hooded rats were reared under identical conditions until 3 months old. They were then exposed for 5 weeks to either a sensorily enriched or an impoverished environment. Each subject was then observed for 15 min in a novel Y-maze. Animals exposed to the enriched environment showed the strongest exploration tendency, particularly early in the trial. The results are discussed in relation to the emphasis placed by earlier workers upon the role of early stimulation in setting adult levels of exploratory behaviour. 相似文献
14.
It was previously found that the exploratory activity of adult Wistar rats with their vibrissae cut in the period from 9 to 20 postnatal days was characterized by lower intragroup variability in comparison with control rats [3]. The present study has shown that the earlier limitation of species-specific afferentation (whisker trimming on postnatal days 2–9) does not induce such changes. We conclude that high plasticity of the brain during the early postnatal period provides better adaptation to the deficit of sensory information. 相似文献
15.
16.
Sensory-motor learning is commonly considered as a mapping process, whereby sensory information is transformed into the motor commands that drive actions. However, this directional mapping, from inputs to outputs, is part of a loop; sensory stimuli cause actions and vice versa. Here, we explore whether actions affect the understanding of the sensory input that they cause. Using a visuo-motor task in humans, we demonstrate two types of learning-related behavioral effects. Stimulus-dependent effects reflect stimulus-response learning, while action-dependent effects reflect a distinct learning component, allowing the brain to predict the forthcoming sensory outcome of actions. Together, the stimulus-dependent and the action-dependent learning components allow the brain to construct a complete internal representation of the sensory-motor loop. 相似文献
17.
Mastronarde DN 《Journal of structural biology》2005,152(1):36-51
A new method was developed to acquire images automatically at a series of specimen tilts, as required for tomographic reconstruction. The method uses changes in specimen position at previous tilt angles to predict the position at the current tilt angle. Actual measurement of the position or focus is skipped if the statistical error of the prediction is low enough. This method allows a tilt series to be acquired rapidly when conditions are good but falls back toward the traditional approach of taking focusing and tracking images when necessary. The method has been implemented in a program, SerialEM, that provides an efficient environment for data acquisition. This program includes control of an energy filter as well as a low-dose imaging mode, in which tracking and focusing occur away from the area of interest. The program can automatically acquire a montage of overlapping frames, allowing tomography of areas larger than the field of the CCD camera. It also includes tools for navigating between specimen positions and finding regions of interest. 相似文献
18.
Cutaneous overexpression of NT-3 increases sensory and sympathetic neuron number and enhances touch dome and hair follicle innervation 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
下载免费PDF全文

《The Journal of cell biology》1996,134(2):487-497
Target-derived influences of nerve growth factor on neuronal survival and differentiation are well documented, though effects of other neurotrophins are less clear. To examine the influence of NT-3 neurotrophin overexpression in a target tissue of sensory and sympathetic neurons, transgenic mice were isolated that overexpress NT- 3 in the epidermis. Overexpression of NT-3 led to a 42% increase in the number of dorsal root ganglia sensory neurons, a 70% increase in the number of trigeminal sensory neurons, and a 32% increase in sympathetic neurons. Elevated NT-3 also caused enlargement of touch dome mechanoreceptor units, sensory end organs innervated by slowly adapting type 1 (SA1) neurons. The enlarged touch dome units of the transgenics had an increased number of associated Merkel cells, cells at which SA1s terminate. An additional alteration of skin innervation in NT-3 transgenics was an increased density of myelinated circular endings associated with the piloneural complex. The enhancement of innervation to the skin was accompanied by a doubling in the number of sensory neurons expressing trkC. In addition, measures of nerve fibers in cross- sectional profiles of cutaneous saphenous nerves of transgenics showed a 60% increase in myelinated fibers. These results indicate that in vivo overexpression of NT-3 by the epidermis enhances the number of sensory and sympathetic neurons and the development of selected sensory endings of the skin. 相似文献
19.
The sensory weighting model is a general model of sensory integration that consists of three processing layers. First, each
sensor provides the central nervous system (CNS) with information regarding a specific physical variable. Due to sensor dynamics,
this measure is only reliable for the frequency range over which the sensor is accurate. Therefore, we hypothesize that the
CNS improves on the reliability of the individual sensor outside this frequency range by using information from other sensors,
a process referred to as “frequency completion.” Frequency completion uses internal models of sensory dynamics. This “improved”
sensory signal is designated as the “sensory estimate” of the physical variable. Second, before being combined, information
with different physical meanings is first transformed into a common representation; sensory estimates are converted to intermediate
estimates. This conversion uses internal models of body dynamics and physical relationships. Third, several sensory systems
may provide information about the same physical variable (e.g., semicircular canals and vision both measure self-rotation).
Therefore, we hypothesize that the “central estimate” of a physical variable is computed as a weighted sum of all available
intermediate estimates of this physical variable, a process referred to as “multicue weighted averaging.” The resulting central
estimate is fed back to the first two layers. The sensory weighting model is applied to three-dimensional (3D) visual–vestibular
interactions and their associated eye movements and perceptual responses. The model inputs are 3D angular and translational
stimuli. The sensory inputs are the 3D sensory signals coming from the semicircular canals, otolith organs, and the visual
system. The angular and translational components of visual movement are assumed to be available as separate stimuli measured
by the visual system using retinal slip and image deformation. In addition, both tonic (“regular”) and phasic (“irregular”)
otolithic afferents are implemented. Whereas neither tonic nor phasic otolithic afferents distinguish gravity from linear
acceleration, the model uses tonic afferents to estimate gravity and phasic afferents to estimate linear acceleration. The
model outputs are the internal estimates of physical motion variables and 3D slow-phase eye movements. The model also includes
a smooth pursuit module. The model matches eye responses and perceptual effects measured during various motion paradigms in
darkness (e.g., centered and eccentric yaw rotation about an earth-vertical axis, yaw rotation about an earth-horizontal axis)
and with visual cues (e.g., stabilized visual stimulation or optokinetic stimulation).
Received: 20 September 2000 / Accepted in revised form: 28 September 2001 相似文献
20.
Kagan I 《Current biology : CB》2012,22(6):R186-R188
The significance of the miniature eye movements that we make during visual fixation has been intensely debated for the last 80 years. Recent studies have revealed that these motions of the eyes fulfill an important functional role: helping to extract useful information from natural scenes. 相似文献