共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
The M group chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, began to feed on three agricultural fruit species, guava, mango and lemon.
It took them 7–8 years until they began to taste these fruits since the villagers left the park area in 1974. Although adult
chimpanzees are conservative in their feeding habits, they are capable of rapidly acquiring new feeding habits, or new traditions,
once they notice that the food is suitable. 相似文献
2.
Hitoshige Hayaki 《Primates; journal of primatology》1988,29(2):147-161
Association partners of young chimpanzees at the Mahale Mountains National Park were analyzed. Juvenile and adolescent chimpanzees
associated frequently with their mothers, although mother-offspring association decreased as the offspring grew up. Males
tended to leave their mothers and associate with adult males, while females remained frequently associating with their mothers
in early adolescence. In late adolescence and young adulthood, males usually associated with adult males and cycling adult
females. Females may transfer into neighboring unit-groups in this stage. Although an immigrant female tended to be alone
when her estrous cycle stopped, she associated with many individuals, in particular with adult males, when she resumed cycling.
Some orphans were observed to associate frequently with particular adults. The findings were discussed in relation to the
unique characteristics of chimpanzee social system. 相似文献
3.
Hitoshige Hayaki 《Primates; journal of primatology》1985,26(4):343-360
Social play of juvenile and adolescent chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, was studied, by analyzing processes of play and interindividual relationships
in play. The results are discussed in relation to communication mechanisms. Play was initiated in several ways. Communication
about play seems to depend on the receiver's interpretation: They can interpret the sender's behavior as play, referring to
(1) play signals accompanied by the behavior, (2) transformation of the behavior in timing, strength, or rhythm, and (3) situation
of the occurrence. Initiation attempts sometimes failed because one hesitated in playing with the other. Although the stronger
often reduced his/her activity during play, play tended to escalate in activity. Players may enjoy such escalation. Play also
had a mechanism not to escalate into fighting. Play was influenced by individuals other than the players. The third party's
movement often affected the players' interaction. The term play does not indicate a behavior itself but the context of the
behavior: The players interpret their behaviors in their play context. 相似文献
4.
Michael A. Huffman Shunji Gotoh Linda A. Turner Miya Hamai Kozo Yoshida 《Primates; journal of primatology》1997,38(2):111-125
A longitudinal study of nematode infection in chimpanzees was conducted between 1989 and 1994 on the M group chimpanzees of
the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania during two annual dry and rainy season periods and a third rainy season. Chemical
and physical antiparasite properties of medicinal plant use against the strongyle nematodeOesophagostomum stephanostomum have recently been reported at Mahale. Here, the incidence of nematode infections were analyzed for seasonal trends to elucidate
the possible influence of parasite infection on previously reported seasonality of medicinal plant use and to test the hypothesis
that the use of these plants is stimulated byO. stephanostomum. The number of chimpanzees infected byO. stephanostomum was significantly higher in the rainy season than in the dry season of both 1989–1990 and 1991–1992. However, the incidence
ofTrichuris trichura andStrongyloides fuelleborni showed no seasonality. Reinfection of individuals byO. stephanostomum occurred in synchrony with annual variation in rainfall: there was a sharp rise in the occurrence of new infections per individual
within one to two months after the beginning of the first heavy rains of the season. This pattern coincides with the reproductive
cycle of this nematode species.O. stephanostomum (95%) infections were associated significantly more frequently with medicinal plant use than eitherT. trichiura (50%) orS. fuelleborni (40%) infections. These observations are consistent with previous reports for the increased use of these plants during the
rainy season and are consistent the hypothesis that medicinal plant use is stimulated byO. stephanostomum infection. 相似文献
5.
Dominance among male chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park,Tanzania: A preliminary study
Hitoshige Hayaki Michael A. Huffman Toshisada Nishida 《Primates; journal of primatology》1989,30(2):187-197
Dominance relationships among male chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, were analyzed. Although all
adolescent males were unequivocally subordinate to all adult males, dominance relationships within the age classes were much
less clear. Especially among adolescent males, few pant-grunts or agonistic interactions occurred. While adolescent males
frequently pant-grunted at adult males, these latter males, except the alpha and the youngest, rarely pant-grunted to one
another. This suggests that a difference of social status exists between adolescent and adult males. Adult males rarely display
overt dominance to one another probably because the presence of other males affects their interactions. Moreover, they seem
to try to keep their dominance relationship ambiguous when making it overt is not advantageous to them. This may be a political
way for males to coexist with one another in a unit-group. 相似文献
6.
Shigeo Uehara 《Primates; journal of primatology》1986,27(1):1-13
Sex differences in animal prey intake were revealed by fecal analysis among wild chimpanzees of the large-sized M-group (ca.
100 members) in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania: prime adult or old males feed more on vertebrates, while adult
females more onCamponotus ants. By contrast, such differences were not obvious in the neighboring, small-sized K-group (ca. 20–30 members), despite
the similar environment in which the two unit-groups lived. Such sex and group differences may be explained in terms of various
factors, either ecological or social, or both, but social factors seem most responsible in particular for the group differences.
It seems likely that increased capture rate of vertebrates per unit-group in the larger-sized M-group results in increased
per capita intake of meat among prime adult or old males. Also, the more frequent interactions among prime adult or old males
of M-group appear to reduce the frequency of theirCamponotus ant-fishing behavior. 相似文献
7.
Hiroyuki Takasaki 《Primates; journal of primatology》1985,26(2):121-129
The mating behavior of receptive females in the M group chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the Mahale Mountains, western Tanzania, was studied by intensive sampling over a period of 5 months. Restrictive matings
were observed only between parous females mostly in the ovulatory stage and prime adult males, in particular the alpha. Young,
nulliparous and/or non-ovulatory females tended to mate with immature or low-ranking adult males. By contrast, older, parous
and/or ovulatory females tended to mate with adult and higher-ranking males. These confirmed the results of a previous extensive
study of the same group. In addition, gradual shift from opportunistic to restrictive matings after inter-unit-group transfer
was revealed for 2 ex-K-group parous females. The findings were discussed in relation to the life history of female chimpanzees.
A National Park since 1984. 相似文献
8.
Detailed observations on the consumption ofVernonia amygdalina (Del.), a naturally occurring plant of known ethnomedicinal value, by an adult female chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of M-group in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania were made. Chewing the pith of several shoots, she sucked out and swallowed
the astringent, bitter tasting juice; spitting out the fibrous remains. The female was followed during this period for 11
hr, over two consecutive days, and was recognized to be in irregular health at the time of consumption, exhibiting signs of
lethargy, lack of appetite, and irregularity of bodily excretions. The low frequency and lack of seasonality in the usage
of this plant suggest that it is sought after for reasons other than as a food source. These factors suggest that for chimpanzees,
the consumption of this plant is primarily medicinal. The symptoms displayed by the female are the same as those experienced
by people throughout tropical Africa who utilize this plant as a medicinal treatment for them. Interactions between the female
and others suggest that they too were aware of her condition and coordinated their activities with the female and her infant. 相似文献
9.
Tool-use for drinking water by immature chimpanzees of Mahale: prevalence of an unessential behavior 总被引:3,自引:3,他引:0
Matsusaka T Nishie H Shimada M Kutsukake N Zamma K Nakamura M Nishida T 《Primates; journal of primatology》2006,47(2):113-122
Use of leaves or sticks for drinking water has only rarely been observed during long-term study of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Mahale. Recently, however, we observed 42 episodes of tool-use for drinking water (73 tools and two cases of using tool-sets) between 1999 and 2004. Interestingly, all of the performers were immature chimpanzees aged from 2 to 10 years. Immature chimpanzees sometimes observed the tool-using performance of others and subsequently reproduced the behavior, while adults usually paid no attention to the performance. This tool-use did not seem to occur out of necessity: (1) chimpanzees often used tools along streams where they could drink water without tools, (2) they used tools for drinking water from tree holes during the wet season when they could easily obtain water from many streams, and (3) the tool-using performance sometimes contained playful aspects. Between-site comparisons revealed that chimpanzees at drier habitats used tools for drinking water more frequently and in a more conventional manner. However, some variations could not be explained by ecological conditions. Such variations and the increase in this tool-use in recent years at Mahale strongly suggest that social learning plays an important role in the process of acquiring the behavior. We should note here that such behaviors that lack obvious benefits or necessity can be prevalent in a group. 相似文献
10.
Ten male and nine female habituated chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Kasoje area of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, were weighed intermittently between December 1973 and March 1980 by luring them up a rope hung on a spring balance: six adult males averaged 42.0 kg and eight adult females 35.2 kg. Seasonal change in body weight was recognized at least partially; body weights tended to decrease in the later part of the wet season presumably because of food shortage in the middle of the wet season. Comparison of body weight among three populations of the same subspecies suggests that adult female chimpanzees of Mahale appear to be heavier than those of the Gombe National Park, Tanzania, and that they seem to be similar to the forest-living counterparts of eastern Zaire. On the other hand, body weights of adult male chimpanzees from the three populations do not show significant differences. Perhaps feeding competition among adult females in a small, isolated habitat is more severe than that among adult males, which may result in the body weight reduction among adult female chimpanzees at Gombe. 相似文献
11.
Hanamura S Kiyono M Lukasik-Braum M Mlengeya T Fujimoto M Nakamura M Nishida T 《Primates; journal of primatology》2008,49(1):77-80
A flu-like disease spread among chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the M group at Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, from June to July 2006. This epizootic or epidemic killed up
to 12 chimpanzees. The obvious evidence of their deaths came from finding the bodies of three infants who had previously shown
some symptoms of the disease. At least one of these infants died of pneumonia. In addition, nine chimpanzees were missing
after the outbreak. These individuals were assumed to have been killed by this epizootic because most of them had contact
with the infected individuals on the last days they were observed. We also found two dead bodies during this period, which
were thought to be those of two missing individuals. We confirmed 23 (35.4%) of 65 individuals of the M group showed some
symptoms of the disease, although most of them (20/23) did not die. More than half of them (14/23) had kin showing symptoms.
Since this epizootic may have been caused by contact with humans, it will be necessary to establish and follow appropriate
protocols for researchers, tourists, and park staff to observe chimpanzees, and to explore the mechanism of disease transmission
from humans to chimpanzees and among chimpanzees. 相似文献
12.
Grips and hand movements of chimpanzees during feeding in Mahale Mountains National Park,Tanzania 下载免费PDF全文
Mary W. Marzke Linda F. Marchant William C. McGrew Sandra P. Reece 《American journal of physical anthropology》2015,156(3):317-326
It has long been assumed that stone tool making was a major factor in the evolution of derived hominin hand morphology. However, stresses on the hand associated with food retrieval and processing also have been recognized as relevant early hominin behaviors that should be investigated. To this end, chimpanzee food manipulation was videotaped in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Grips and hand movements by 39 chimpanzees were analyzed for arboreal and terrestrial feeding involving 10 food‐types and associated vegetation. It was predicted that (1) new grips would be found that had not been observed in captivity, (2) forceful precision grips would be absent from the repertoire, as in captivity, and (3) precision handling would be observed. New grips involving the full thumb and buttressed index finger, and a new integrated pattern of grips and forceful hand movements were discovered, associated with feeding on large fruits and meat. Participation of the full thumb in these grips, rather than the distal thumb and fingers, throws light on feeding behaviors that may have become increasingly significant factors in the evolution of derived hominin thumb morphology. The proximal thumb stabilizes food with the flexed index finger against the pull of the teeth and provides leverage in breaking food into portions. Isolated qualitative observations of possibly forceful pinch by the thumb and side of the index finger highlight the need for comparative quantitative data to test whether humans are unique in forceful precision gripping capability. Precision handling was not seen. Am J Phys Anthropol 156:317–326, 2015. © 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 相似文献
13.
Michio Nakamura William C. McGrew Linda F. Marchant Toshisada Nishida 《Primates; journal of primatology》2000,41(3):237-248
Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, scratch other individual's bodies while they groom them. This behavioral
pattern of “social scratch” is another example of locality-specific social behavior, or custom, as it is not found in the
Gombe National Park, Tanzania, about 150 km north of Mahale, nor has it been reported from any other sites of chimpanzee study.
Frequency of social scratch was correlated with frequency of social grooming, but not with frequency of self-scratch. Frequencies
of social scratch per grooming bout among adult and adoles-cent males, and from lactating females to infants or juveniles,
were high, and among males, higher-ranking males especially received more. These facts indicate some social function of the
behavior. Social scratch was directed mostly to the dorsal side of the body. However, when lactating females social scratched
to infants or juveniles, they scratched other body parts. Social scratch was not lateralized to left or right. We present
four hypotheses on the functional origin and on the learning process of this cultural behavioral pattern. 相似文献
14.
Matsumoto-Oda Akiko Hosaka Kazuhiko Huffman Michael A. Kawanaka Kenji 《International journal of primatology》1998,19(6):999-1011
We studied factors affecting party size and composition of wild chimpanzees at Mahale (M group) over an 11-month period. Parties with 1–5 individuals were most frequent (37.8%; 153/405 parties); they included 94.7% of all male parties (n = 76) and 81.3% of all female parties (n = 75). The median of monthly values was the standard for analysis. We divided the year into four periods based on the median size of monthly bisexual parties (30.9 individuals; includes both males and females): monthly bisexual party sizes were larger in May–June (period II) and October–January (period IV) and smaller in February–April (period I) and July–September (period III). Only bisexual parties changed in size with period. The number of fruit items (=species) eaten was fewer in periods II and IV when abundance per item appeared to be great. The sizes of bisexual parties, which included cycling females with maximal anogenital swelling, were larger, and their representation (%) in all bisexual parties was greater in periods III and IV. The numbers of both cycling females and cycling females with maximal anogenital swelling were also larger in periods III and IV. The percentage of cycling females with maximal anogenital swelling was greater in periods II and III. The results of this study and those of Nishida (1979) suggest that seasonal variation in party size of Mahale chimpanzees maintains a relatively consistent annual cycle. The factors assumed to affect party sizes are fruit availability and the presence of cycling females with maximal anogenital swelling. 相似文献
15.
Wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) used probes made of vegetation to fish for termites (Macrotermes herus) in the Mahale Mountains of western Tanzania. Data on both the artefacts and behavior associated with their use were recorded over a 5-month period. The chimpanzees of one unit group, B Group, were seen four times to use the tools to extract the insects from their mounds. A sample of 290 tools was collected and analyzed in terms of age, seasonality, length, width, class of raw materials, species, methods of making, extent of use, and damage incurred through use. There were differences across groups in tool use in feeding on termites; some could be related to biotic factors, but others appeared to be cultural differences. 相似文献
16.
Feces of wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania, were inspected for intestinal parasites under a compound microscope.
Eggs or larvae ofOesophagostomum, Strongyloides, Trichuris, Prosthenorchis, andBertiella were found. Intestinal nematodes significantly increased in the mid-rainy season. This finding supports (or, at least, is
not in conflict with) the hypothesis thatAspilia leaves which are occasionally swallowed by chimpanzees may function as a vermicide, since ingestion of such leaves also increases
significantly in the mid-rainy season. 相似文献
17.
The use of perforating sticks and flexible stalks in combination for termite fishing and a complex tool-set of three components
used sequentially (stout chiel, bodkin, and dip-stick) to penetrate melipone and ground-dwelling bee hives byPan troglodytes troglodytes are documented or, inferred from circumstantial evidence. Functionally, termite extraction tools were similar to other locations
in west and central Africa, but the plants and the number of raw material species used were different. Tools varied in the
degree of modification (fraying ends). Chimpanzees in the Lossi forest seem to be able to use the tools not in a stereotyped
fashion, but in a flexible, insightful way. The extraction of Melipone honey using large pieces of wood as pounding tools
has rarely been recorded elsewhere. The most impressive technological solution to the honey-getting problem by wild chimpanzees
was shown by this study. This is the only known, use of a tool-set of three components in sequence to extract honey by wild
chimpanzees. 相似文献
18.
Terence L. Marsh Charles Hagedorn Stephen R. Werre Taranjit Kaur 《American journal of primatology》2010,72(7):566-574
Although the intestinal flora of chimpanzees has not been studied, insight into this dynamic environment can be obtained through studies on their feces. We analyzed fecal samples from human‐habituated, wild chimpanzees at Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, and compared microbial community profiles to determine if members of the same social group were similar. Between July and December 2007, we collected fresh fecal samples from 12 individuals: four juveniles, four adolescents, and four adults, including three parent–offspring pairs. Each sample was analyzed using Terminal‐Restriction Fragment Length Polymorphism of amplified 16S rRNA genes. Twelve different profiles were generated, having between 1 and 15 Terminal‐Restriction Fragments (T‐RFs). Overall, a total of 23 different T‐RFs were produced. Putative assignments of T‐RFs corresponded to the phyla Firmicutes (Clostridia, Bacilli, and Lactobacilli), Bacteroidetes, Tenericutes (Mollicutes Class), Actinobacteria, and Proteobacteria, as well as to uncultured or unidentified organisms. Firmicutes and Bacteroidetes phyla and Mollicutes Class were the most commonly assigned in 11, 8, and 8 of the samples, respectively, with this being the first report of Mollicutes in wild chimpanzees. Principal Components Analysis (PCA) revealed clustering of nine samples, and 80.5% of the diversity was accounted for by three samples. Morisita indices of community similarity ranged between 0.00 and 0.89, with dissimiliarity (<0.5) between most samples when compared two at a time. Our findings suggest that, although phylotypes are common among individuals, profiles among members of the same social group are host‐specific. We conclude that factors other than social group, such as kinship and age, may influence fecal bacterial profiles of wild chimpanzees, and recommend that additional studies be conducted. Am. J. Primatol. 72:566–574, 2010. © 2010 Wiley‐Liss, Inc. 相似文献
19.
Hiroyuki Takasaki Yukio Takahata Mariko Hiraiwa-Hasegawa Richard W. Byrne Takayoshi Kano 《Primates; journal of primatology》1986,27(4):517-519
A case of unusually early postpartum resumption of estrous cycling (<7 months) was recorded for a young, presumably primiparous
female in the M group of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Mahale Mountains National Park, western Tanzania. The female showed estrous cycling while lactating her infant, and
mated with young and low-ranking males as well as with the alpha male. 相似文献
20.
Shigeo Uehara Toshisada Nishida Hiroyuki Takasaki Mahale Mountains Wildlife Kohshi Norikoshi Takahiro Tsukahara Ramadhani Nyundo Miya Hamai 《Primates; journal of primatology》1994,35(3):275-281
K Group, originally one of the two major study groups of chimpanzees since 1965 in the Mahale Mountains National Park, western
Tanzania, was almost extinct by 1983: at most seven individuals remained in the group at the beginning of 1983. K Group continued
to exist for more than four years, but in 1987 a male was left alone at the age of 15 after all the other chimpanzees of the
group emigrated or disappeared. Since then he has been observed sporadically for more than five years only within the former
range of K Group, without having any contact with the many resident chimpanzees of the neighboring M Group, the other major
study group. The present observations reconfirm the strong philopatric tendency of adult male chimpanzees. 相似文献