首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
4.
Research on sensory perception now often considers more than one sense at a time. This approach reflects real-world situations, such as when a visible object touches us. Indeed, vision and touch show great interdependence: the sight of a body part can reduce tactile target detection times [1], visual and tactile attentional systems are spatially linked [2], and the texture of surfaces that are actively touched with the fingertips is perceived using both vision and touch [3]. However, these previous findings might be mediated by spatial attention [1, 2] or by improved guidance of movement [3] via visually enhanced body position sense [4--6]. Here, we investigate the direct effects of viewing the body on passive touch. We measured tactile two-point discrimination thresholds [7] on the forearm while manipulating the visibility of the arm but holding gaze direction constant. The spatial resolution of touch was better when the arm was visible than when it was not. Tactile performance was further improved when the view of the arm was magnified. In contrast, performance was not improved by viewing a neutral object at the arm's location, ruling out improved spatial orienting as a possible account. Controls confirmed that no information about the tactile stimulation was provided by visibility of the arm. This visual enhancement of touch may point to online reorganization of tactile receptive fields.  相似文献   

5.
Touch differs from other exteroceptive senses in that the body itself forms part of the tactile percept. Interactions between proprioception and touch provide a powerful way to investigate the implicit body representation underlying touch. Here, we demonstrate that an intrinsic primary quality of a tactile object, for example its size, is directly affected by the perceived size of the body part touching it. We elicited proprioceptive illusions that the left index finger was either elongating or shrinking by vibrating the biceps or triceps tendon of the right arm while subjects grasped the tip of their left index finger. Subjects estimated the distance between two simultaneous tactile contacts on the left finger during tendon vibration. We found that tactile distances feel bigger when the touched body part feels elongated. Control tests showed that the modulation of touch was linked to the perceived index-finger size induced by tendon vibration. Vibrations that did not produce proprioceptive illusion had no effect on touch. Our results show that the perception of tactile objects is referenced to an implicit body representation and that proprioception contributes to this body representation. We also provide, for the first time, a quantitative, implicit measure of distortions of body size.  相似文献   

6.
Thirty-two healthy human subjects provided thresholds for the perception of slight and moderate heat pain. Four body sites were tested bilaterally: thenar eminence of the hand, plantar surface of the foot, dorsolateral forearm, and lateral calf. Thresholds for the glabrous skin of the hand and foot were significantly greater than thresholds for the hairy skin of the arm and leg, the average difference being 1.3°c. Laterality was not a statistically significant factor. Thresholds increased progressively over 2–4 weeks of repeated testing, resulting in values averaging 0.6°c higher in the later sessions. The difference between moderate and slight pain thresholds averaged 1.1°c, and was consistent across body sites and with repeated testing.

The threshold values were normally distributed across subjects. Considerable intersubject variability was observed for both slight and moderate pain thresholds, more so on glabrous than on hairy skin sites. In comparison, the distribution of right-left difference values was narrower, demonstrating less intrasubject versus intersubject variability.

The highly significant difference in thresholds between glabrous and hairy skin sites demonstrates the importance of skin type for heat pain sensitivity. In contrast, there was no significant difference in heat pain sensitivity between comparable sites on the upper versus lower extremities, or between left and right sides.  相似文献   

7.
The goal of this study was to address some of the factors that contribute to the human ability to detect the presence of weak electric fields generated by direct current (DC) and alternating current (AC) sources. An exposure chamber allowed us to expose a limited surface of the body (forearm and hand) to DC fields of up to 65 kV/m and AC fields up to a maximum of 35 kV/m (frequency 60 Hz). Perception was examined using a staircase procedure and a rating procedure derived from signal detection theory. Sixteen subjects participated in the experiments, and none detected the local DC fields. In contrast, 9/16 subjects were sensitive to local AC electric fields, although detection thresholds (index of sensitivity, d' = 1.0) were widely variable between subjects. When regional exposure was limited to the dorsal forearm, performance was similar to that seen when the forearm and hand were exposed. In contrast, subjects did not reliably detect the AC electric fields when exposure was limited to the hand (either hairy or glabrous skin), although a minority of subjects (3/9) showed some evidence of detecting fields presented to the glabrous palm. Subjects were unable to detect AC electric fields when the hair was removed from the forearm and hand, suggesting that the evoked sensation is mainly dependent on movement of hair located in the exposed region.  相似文献   

8.
Experiments were conducted in which threshold-frequency characteristics were measured on the hairy skin of the forearm of human observers. Thresholds were measured with two stimulus probe areas (2.9 and 0.008 cm2) at three skin-surface temperatures (15d`, 30d`, and 40d`C). The results suggest that whereas glabrous skin uses four distinct channels of information, only three channels may be involved in mediating the sense of touch for hairy skin. The three channels are defined as Ph (Pacinian, hairy skin), NPh low (non-Pacinian, hairy skin, low frequencies) and NPh mid (non-Pacinian, hairy skin, middle frequencies). In addition, it is proposed that the neural substrates for the three psychophysically characterized channels are, respectively, the Pacinian corpuscle (PC) nerve fibers, the slowly adapting type II (SAII) fibers, and the rapidly adapting (RA) fibers.  相似文献   

9.
It was recently shown that the cutaneous sensitivity to airpuffs is decreased by a low-frequency vibrotactile masker in the hairy skin, and by a low-frequency but especially by a high-frequency masker in the glabrous skin. In the current study, the spatial features of this masking effect were determined in four healthy human subjects, using a reaction time paradigm. The masking effect decreased monotonically with increasing interstimulus distance, and identically in longitudinal and transverse (i.e., lateral) directions in the palm or dorsal surface of the hand. The masking effect was stronger in the glabrous than in the hairy skin, especially in the fingers. In the glabrous skin, the spread of masking effect produced by a high-frequency masker was more extensive than that produced by a low-frequency masker. The mechanical spread of high-frequency vibration was less extensive than that of low-frequency vibration in the skin. In the glabrous skin, a masker applied to the tip of the finger produced a stronger masking effect on sensations in the base of the finger than when the masker was located at the base and the test stimulus was located at the tip. It is concluded that mechanical spread of vibration in the skin is of minor importance in explaining the masking effects. Different peripheral neural mechanisms underlie the airpuff-elicited sensations in the hairy and glabrous skin. The afferent inhibitory mechanisms are stronger for signals coming from the glabrous skin of the fingers than for signals coming from the hairy skin. Furthermore, the peripheral innervation density and size of the cortical representational areas may be of importance in determining the magnitude of the masking effect.  相似文献   

10.
It was recently shown that the cutaneous sensitivity to airpuffs is decreased by a low-frequency vibrotactile masker in the hairy skin, and by a low-frequency but especially by a high-frequency masker in the glabrous skin. In the current study, the spatial features of this masking effect were determined in four healthy human subjects, using a reaction time paradigm. The masking effect decreased monotonically with increasing interstimulus distance, and identically in longitudinal and transverse (i.e., lateral) directions in the palm or dorsal surface of the hand. The masking effect was stronger in the glabrous than in the hairy skin, especially in the fingers. In the glabrous skin, the spread of masking effect produced by a high-frequency masker was more extensive than that produced by a low-frequency masker. The mechanical spread of high-frequency vibration was less extensive than that of low-frequency vibration in the skin. In the glabrous skin, a masker applied to the tip of the finger produced a stronger masking effect on sensations in the base of the finger than when the masker was located at the base and the test stimulus was located at the tip. It is concluded that mechanical spread of vibration in the skin is of minor importance in explaining the masking effects. Different peripheral neural mechanisms underlie the airpuff-elicited sensations in the hairy and glabrous skin. The afferent inhibitory mechanisms are stronger for signals coming from the glabrous skin of the fingers than for signals coming from the hairy skin. Furthermore, the peripheral innervation density and size of the cortical representational areas may be of importance in determining the magnitude of the masking effect.  相似文献   

11.
We have used the quinacrine labeling technique and electron microscopy to study the development of the Merkel cell population in the skin of the rat and how this is affected by denervation produced at birth and at various times thereafter. An unexpected difference was found between the Merkel cells of glabrous and hairy skin. In the paw pads of rats aged 1 day or older the Merkel cells differentiated normally and survived quantitatively in the absence of their nerves. In the touch domes however, denervation at 1-4 days prevented the differentiation of the normal Merkel cell population and led to the disappearance of all or most of the Merkel cells that were already present. The Merkel cells in touch domes of the lower leg were affected by denervation like those of the back skin, differing strikingly from the Merkel cells of the footpads, even though the hairy skin of the leg and the glabrous skin of the foot are innervated by the same anatomical nerve. In adult rats, axons regenerating to denervated paws reinnervated epidermal Merkel cells of the pads and restored essentially normal mechanosensitivity to them; thus the Merkel cells of mammalian glabrous skin, like their counterparts in the wholly glabrous skin of lower vertebrates (S. A. Scott, E. Cooper, and J. Diamond, 1981, Proc. R. Soc. London B211, 455-470; K. M. Mearow and J. Diamond, 1988, Neuroscience 26, 695-708), can act as targets for ingrowing nerves. However, even though the differentiation of Merkel cells in hairy skin is nerve dependent, they probably have in common with the Merkel cells of glabrous skin the role of acting as final targets for nerves during development and regeneration.  相似文献   

12.
The sense of touch provides fundamental information about the surrounding world, and feedback about our own actions. Although touch is very important during the earliest stages of life, to date no study has investigated infants’ abilities to process visual stimuli implying touch. This study explores the developmental origins of the ability to visually recognize touching gestures involving others. Looking times and orienting responses were measured in a visual preference task, in which participants were simultaneously presented with two videos depicting a touching and a no-touching gesture involving human body parts (face, hand) and/or an object (spoon). In Experiment 1, 2-day-old newborns and 3-month-old infants viewed two videos: in one video a moving hand touched a static face, in the other the moving hand stopped before touching it. Results showed that only 3-month-olds, but not newborns, differentiated the touching from the no-touching gesture, displaying a preference for the former over the latter. To test whether newborns could manifest a preferential visual response when the touched body part is different from the face, in Experiment 2 newborns were presented with touching/no-touching gestures in which a hand or an inanimate object—i.e., a spoon- moved towards a static hand. Newborns were able to discriminate a hand-to-hand touching gesture, but they did not manifest any preference for the object-to-hand touch. The present findings speak in favour of an early ability to visually recognize touching gestures involving the interaction between human body parts.  相似文献   

13.
Subjects perceived touch sensations as arising from a table (or a rubber hand) when both the table (or the rubber hand) and their own real hand were repeatedly tapped and stroked in synchrony with the real hand hidden from view. If the table or rubber hand was then 'injured', subjects displayed a strong skin conductance response (SCR) even though nothing was done to the real hand. Sensations could even be projected to anatomically impossible locations. The illusion was much less vivid, as indicated by subjective reports and SCR, if the real hand was simultaneously visible during stroking, or if the real hand was hidden but touched asynchronously. The fact that the illusion could be significantly diminished when the real hand was simultaneously visible suggests that the illusion and associated SCRs were due to perceptual assimilation of the table (or rubber hand) into one's body image rather than associative conditioning. These experiments demonstrate the malleability of body image and the brain's remarkable capacity for detecting statistical correlations in the sensory input.  相似文献   

14.
Cutaneous mechanoreceptors are localized in the various layers of the skin where they detect a wide range of mechanical stimuli, including light brush, stretch, vibration and noxious pressure. This variety of stimuli is matched by a diverse array of specialized mechanoreceptors that respond to cutaneous deformation in a specific way and relay these stimuli to higher brain structures. Studies across mechanoreceptors and genetically tractable sensory nerve endings are beginning to uncover touch sensation mechanisms. Work in this field has provided researchers with a more thorough understanding of the circuit organization underlying the perception of touch. Novel ion channels have emerged as candidates for transduction molecules and properties of mechanically gated currents improved our understanding of the mechanisms of adaptation to tactile stimuli. This review highlights the progress made in characterizing functional properties of mechanoreceptors in hairy and glabrous skin and ion channels that detect mechanical inputs and shape mechanoreceptor adaptation.  相似文献   

15.
Touch sense     
Cutaneous mechanoreceptors are localized in the various layers of the skin where they detect a wide range of mechanical stimuli, including light brush, stretch, vibration and noxious pressure. This variety of stimuli is matched by a diverse array of specialized mechanoreceptors that respond to cutaneous deformation in a specific way and relay these stimuli to higher brain structures. Studies across mechanoreceptors and genetically tractable sensory nerve endings are beginning to uncover touch sensation mechanisms. Work in this field has provided researchers with a more thorough understanding of the circuit organization underlying the perception of touch. Novel ion channels have emerged as candidates for transduction molecules and properties of mechanically gated currents improved our understanding of the mechanisms of adaptation to tactile stimuli. This review highlights the progress made in characterizing functional properties of mechanoreceptors in hairy and glabrous skin and ion channels that detect mechanical inputs and shape mechanoreceptor adaptation.  相似文献   

16.
Watching the movie scene in which a tarantula crawls on James Bond's chest can make us literally shiver--as if the spider crawled on our own chest. What neural mechanisms are responsible for this "tactile empathy"? The observation of the actions of others activates the premotor cortex normally involved in the execution of the same actions. If a similar mechanism applies to the sight of touch, movies depicting touch should automatically activate the somatosensory cortex of the observer. Here we found using fMRI that the secondary but not the primary somatosensory cortex is activated both when the participants were touched and when they observed someone or something else getting touched by objects. The neural mechanisms enabling our own sensation of touch may therefore be a window also to our understanding of touch.  相似文献   

17.
Factors involved in the outcome of regeneration of the saphenous nerve after a cut or crush lesion were studied in adult rats with electrophysiological recordings of low-threshold mechanoreceptor activity and plasma extravasation of Evans blue after electrical nerve stimulation that activated C fibers.

In the first series of experiments, saphenous and sciatic nerve section was combined with anastomosis of the transected proximal end of the saphenous nerve to the distal end of the cut tibial nerve. Regeneration of saphenous nerve fibers involved in plasma extravasation and low-threshold mechanoreceptor activity in the glabrous skin was observed 13 weeks after nerve anastomosis. Substance P-, calcitonin gene-related peptide-, and protein gene product 9.5 (PGP-9.5)-immunoreactive (IR) thin epidermal and dermal nerve endings, as well as coarse dermal PGP-9.5-IR nerve fibers and Meissner corpuscles and Merkel cell-neurite-like complexes, were observed in the reinnervated glabrous skin at this time.

In a second series of experiments, the time course of the regeneration of saphenous nerve axons to the permanently sciatic-nerve-denervated foot sole was examined. Saphenous-nerve-induced plasma extravasation and low-threshold mechanoreceptor activity in the saphenous nerve were found in the normal saphenous nerve territory 2, 3, 4, and 6 weeks after sciatic nerve cut combined with saphenous nerve crush in the left hindlimb. Saphenous-nerve-induced plasma extravasation was also present in the glabrous skin normally innervated by the sciatic nerve 3, 4, and 6 weeks after the sciatic cut/saphenous crush lesion. However, no low-threshold mechanoreceptor activity was detected in the saphenous nerve when the glabrous skin area was stimulated.

In a third series of experiments, the fate of the expansion of the saphenous nerve territory after saphenous nerve crush was examined when the crushed sciatic nerve had been allowed to regenerate. Nerve fibers involved in plasma extravasation were observed in the glabrous skin of the hindpaw after saphenous nerve, as well as after tibial nerve, C-fiber stimulation 3, 12, and 43 weeks after the saphenous crush/sciatic crush lesion.

Low-threshold mechanoreceptors from the regenerated saphenous nerve, which primarily innervates hairy skin, seem to be functional in the glabrous skin if the axons are guided by the transected tibial nerve by anastomosis. Furthermore, the results indicate that fibers from the regenerating saphenous nerve that have extended into denervated glabrous skin areas can exist even if sciatic nerve axons are allowed to grow back to their original territory.  相似文献   

18.
The purpose of this project was to test the hypothesis that, independent of neural control, glabrous and nonglabrous cutaneous vasculature is capable of autoregulating blood flow. In 10 subjects, spectral and transfer function analyses of arterial pressure and skin blood flow (laser-Doppler flowmetry) from glabrous (palm) and nonglabrous (forearm) regions were performed under three conditions: baseline, ganglionic blockade via intravenous trimethaphan administration, and trimethaphan plus oscillatory lower body negative pressure (LBNP; -5 to -10 mmHg) from 0.05 to 0.07 Hz. Oscillatory LBNP was applied to regenerate mean arterial pressure variability that was abolished by ganglionic blockade. Ganglionic blockade was verified by an absence of a heart rate response to a Valsalva maneuver. Spectral power and transfer function gain between blood pressure and skin blood flow were calculated in this oscillatory frequency range (0.05-0.07 Hz). Within this frequency range, ganglionic blockade significantly decreased spectral power of blood flow in both the forearm and palm, whereas regeneration of arterial blood pressure oscillations significantly increased spectral power of forearm blood flow but not palm blood flow. During oscillatory LBNP, transfer function gain between blood pressure and skin blood flow was significantly elevated at the forearm (0.28 +/- 0.03 to 0.53 +/- 0.02 flux units/mmHg; P < 0.05) but was reduced at the palm (4.7 +/- 0.5 to 1.2 +/- 0.1 flux units/mmHg; P < 0.05). These data show that independent of neural control of blood flow, glabrous skin has the ability to buffer blood pressure oscillations and demonstrates a degree of dynamic autoregulation. Conversely, these data suggest that nonglabrous skin has diminished dynamic autoregulatory capabilities.  相似文献   

19.
Immunoregulation of passively induced allergic encephalomyelitis   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Experimental allergic encephalomyelitis (EAE) can be readily induced passively by transfer of lymphocytes from neuroantigen immunized rats to naive recipients. This passively induced disease runs an acute, monophasic, self-limiting course, much the same as is usually seen in actively induced diseases. Here we examine the mechanisms regulating passive EAE. We report that splenectomy, thymectomy, and increasing age of recipients, manipulations which have been reported to influence recovery from actively induced EAE, have no effect on passively induced disease. EAE effector cells are not inactivated when transferred into recipients that have been actively sensitized and are beginning their recovery from clinical signs; this being a time when recovery associated suppressor cells are thought to be present. Finally, in the absence of suppressor T cells in both the recipient and in the transfer cell population, recovery from passive EAE still occurs. We conclude that suppressor T cells play no role in regulating passively induced EAE.  相似文献   

20.

Background

It has been reported that participants judge an object to be closer after a stick has been used to touch it than after touching it with the hand. In this study we try to find out why this is so.

Methodology

We showed six participants a cylindrical object on a table. On separate trials (randomly intermixed) participants either estimated verbally how far the object is from their body or they touched a remembered location. Touching was done either with the hand or with a stick (in separate blocks). In three different sessions, participants touched either the object location or the location halfway to the object location. Verbal judgments were given either in centimeters or in terms of whether the object would be reachable with the hand. No differences in verbal distance judgments or touching responses were found between the blocks in which the stick or the hand was used.

Conclusion

Instead of finding out why the judged distance changes when using a tool, we found that using a stick does not necessarily alter judged distances or judgments about the reachability of objects.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号