首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
The present findings show the existence of significant differences in latency and amplitude of some waves of ERPs to three subclasses of nouns or verbs. Latency LP3 of ERPs to action verbs was shorter than latency of the same wave to abstract verbs. In nouns the relation was a similar (manipulable objects versus abstract ones). The amplitude of early positive components (P1, P2) and BN3 wave also depended on semantic attributes of nouns and verbs. Some waves of ERPs to motion verbs in our experiment had significantly higher amplitude than the same waves of ERPs to nonmanipulable objects. Also revealed was interaction between some ERP features and the subject's gender. It was primarily the amplitude of BP2, the size of which depended on gender in all cases--BP2 amplitude was significantly higher in females then in males. In the other components (BP1, BP4, BN2 and BN4) there were fewer significant differences and if they do occurred, then their amplitude was higher in males than in females. In some cases, gender also affected latency of waves LP2, LN1 and LN4 of ERPs to the same noun or verb subclass--latencies of these waves were shorter in females.  相似文献   

2.
Event-related potentials (ERPs) to word stimuli were registered in 20 twenty-one-year-old students (7 males and 13 females) of the Faculty of Medicine of Palacky University. In an ideal case in the first 800 ms after the onset of stimulus ERP consists of three clear positive and four negative waves. The amplitude of some waves of ERPs shows a high degree of inter-individual variability. It was also revealed that the latency and amplitude of some ERPs waves to word stimuli depends on the gender and on the site of the registration electrode on the scalp of the subject. Latencies of components LP2 and LN4 in all electrodes are shorter in females than in males, component LN2 behaves reversely--its latency is shorter in males. On the other hand, the amplitude of dependent variables BP2, BN2 and PV1 is in most electrodes higher in females than in males--except for the amplitude of components BP4 and BN1.  相似文献   

3.
Sixty-eight subjects ranging in age from 6 to 23 years were studied in an ‘auditory oddball’ event-related potential (ERP) paradigm. Our results replicate other studies, finding P3 as the most consistent component of ERPs since childhood, although great variability of this component was found in the 6-year-old group. Separate age/ERP component latency and amplitude linear regressions were computed for subjects 6–14 and 6–23 years old. Our data show in both groups a significant negative and positive correlation between age and P3 latency and N1-P2 amplitude respectively. The age/P3 latency slope for the subjects under 15 years old was −19.00 msec/year versus 8.15 msec/year for all subjects (6–23 years old). Our results indicate that P3 latency during childhood decreases with age, reaching an asymptote after or during the second decade of life. No curvilinear relationship between age and P3 latency was found over the child groups, although a significant curvilinear relationship was found over the entire age range.This study showed no significant gender differences in latency at any age group. However, in the adult group females showed significantly larger amplitudes than males.  相似文献   

4.
The paper presents findings of an experiment the participants of which were native Czech speakers (L1 ) possessed of perfect knowledge of English (L2). We assumed that the subjects could process differently each of the three types of English and Czech sentences; the first type represented correct sentences, the other two violated sentences. Violated sentences differed in the precise relationship between the verbs, their preceding noun phrases, and their preceding context. ERPs to critical verbs, the accuracy rate for both English and Czech sentences processing and decision time for each were evaluated. Participants' personality traits were assessed too. It has been shown that our subjects, who learned English after the age of 15, displayed significant differences in all measured parameters. It was especially the longer latency N400, the lower accuracy rate of English sentence processing, the longer decision time, and the same dynamics of all measured ERPs parameters. Possible causes of these differences are discussed.  相似文献   

5.
Monocular pattern-shift visual evoked potentials (PSVEPs) were obtained in 26 neurologically and ophthalmologically normal elderly community volunteers (mean age 59.4, males 15, females 11), and compared with similarly obtained data in 26 sex-matched young subjects (mean age 28.1). Elderly males were age-matched with elderly females, and young males were age-matched with young females. Data analyses at both the midoccipital-linked ears and midoccipital-midcentral derivations revealed that the combined-eye mean P100 latency in the elderly as a whole was significantly longer than the young sex-matched controls (P < 0.02). However, the latency in the elderly males was not significantly different from that of sex-matched young males, or age-matched elderly females. The latency in young female subjects, on the other hand, was significantly shorter both when compared to that of age-matched young males (P < 0.01) and sex-matched elderly females (P < 0.01). The differences in other PSVEP variables, namely, interocular P100 latency differences, P100 amplitudes and interocular P100 amplitude ratios were not significant between the groups and subgroups studied.It is concluded that females account for the major contribution towards the longer P100 latency in the elderly.  相似文献   

6.
Baseline auditory ERP data from a larger study of the genetic determinants of the response to alcohol were collected from 59 monozygotic (MZ) twin pairs and from 39 same-sex dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs who drank socially. Three methods for measuring genetic influence on the ERPs were applied. First, based on maximum-likelihood estimates, the heritability of conventional peak amplitude and latency of N1 and P3 components was computed for each of 16 lead locations using tests of the significance of heritability based on intraclass correlations. P3 amplitude provided the strongest results, distributed symmetrically over caudal leads, and implied gene dominance as the mode of genetic transmission for the P3 component. A substantial genetic influence on N1 latency suggested a mixture of additive and dominance effects in the left fronto-temporal region. N1 amplitude measures trended towards significant heritability, but none was observed for P3 latency. The second method used the maximum of the cross-correlation function to compare wave form shape in a lead-by-lead analysis of data from cotwins. Genetic influence was apparent in both target and non-target ERP responses, with a fronto-central topography of significant results. The third method reduced all spatial and temporal ERP differences from a pair of twins to a single scalar number for each response. Distributions of this global measure revealed significant genetic influence on both non-target and target ERPs. A post hoc analysis of the effect of gender on the heritability of N1 or P3 peaks and latencies revealed no statistically significant observations in this sample of young adult twins.  相似文献   

7.
Event-related potentials (ERPs) were elicited with an auditory discrimination paradigm in 20 adult female subjects on the first day of their menstrual cycles and approximately 14 days later. The amplitude and latency of the N1, P2, N2 and P3 (P300) components were measured for the two assessment times. No differences in either amplitude or latency for any of the components were observed as a function of menstrual cycle. Half the subjects who took oral contraceptives were compared to the other half who did not. No differences or interactions between these subgroups were obtained for any component amplitude or latency. It was concluded that menstrual cycle and use of oral contraceptives do not affect the P3 or other ERP components.  相似文献   

8.
Word formation in Arabic has traditionally been assumed to involve interdigitation of a consonantal root with a vocalic pattern. This view is adopted by a large number of modern generative morphologists. More recently, however, several morphologists have argued that words in Semitic are formed from fully vocalised stems. In this paper, I argue that in San'ani (the dialect of San'a), and in some other Arabic dialects, there is a class of verbs that have as part of either their denotations or connotations a diminutive sense. I then consider diminutive nouns in the dialect. On the basis of semantic and phonological relationships between diminutive verbs and their non-diminutive counterparts, as well as native speakers' explanations of certain diminutive verbs, and the relationship between diminutive nouns and their non-diminutive counterparts, I argue that while some derivational processes take the root as the basic morphological unit, phonological and semantic similarities between certain stems can only be accounted for by derivation from a fully vocalised stem. I therefore conclude that both root-based and stem-based types of word formation occur.  相似文献   

9.
Auditory and visual event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were used to complement neuropsychological and medical assessment in neurologically healthy subjects with asymptomatic and symptomatic human immunodeficiency virus type 1 (HIV-1) infection. Auditory and visual ERPs, recorded using standard oddball paradigms, disclosed delays in late waves (N2 and P3) in symptomatic subjects (CDC stage IV) when compared with matched controls. Abnormally delayed P3 waves in at least one modality were recorded in 41% of symptomatics and this was associated with deficits in neuropsychological performance, particularly psychomotor slowing. However, no differences in late wave latencies between asymptomatic and control subjects were found, though asymptomatics showed delays in auditory N1 and P2 latencies. The number of morphological abnormalities, such as indiscernible late waves as well as topographical variability of the P3 wave, was increased in both HIV seropositive groups and possibly indicates a distinct mechanism of impairment, different from latency delay. Whilst P3 delay in symptomatics was not associated with changes in immune function (T4 cells) there was, however, a link with anaemia and subclinical hepatic dysfunction.  相似文献   

10.
Recent work in event-related potentials (ERPs) has revealed differences between ERPs elicited during the performance of semantic and non-semantic tasks. This experiment examined age differences in the late components of the ERP as a function of these tasks. Young and elderly males were presented with 5-word strings on each trial. The subject's task was to decide whether or not the fifth word matched the other four. In one condition, the first 4 words were identical to one another and the fifth word was either the same or different. In the other condition, the first 4 words were different from one another but were drawn from from the same semantic category. The fifth word was either from the category or from a different category. Matches occurred in 15% of the trials and mismatches in 85%. The ERP to the fifth word was analyzed. All ERPs evidenced a late positive component (LPC), the latency of which was greater for the Category task and for mismatches. LPC amplitude was greater in the Identity task. In addition, young subjects' LPC amplitudes were greater for matches than for mismatches, whereas the reverse was true for the elderly. In agreement with previous research, our results indicated that an N400 wave is evident only in response to semantic incongruity and that variables affecting late positive activity seem to be independent of those producing N400. The N400 wave was seen at a greater latency in the elderly. Our results extend previous work (e.g., Kutas and Hillyard 1980) by finding an N400 in a semantic context other than sentences, and in concluding that this component is relatively insensitive to stimulus frequency.  相似文献   

11.
This study investigated how both sex and individual differences in a mental rotation test (MRT) influence performance on working memory (WM). To identify the neural substrate supporting these differences, brain electrical activity was measured using the event-related potential technique. No significant sex differences were observed in a test of verbal WM, however males were significantly faster than females to respond to probe stimuli in a test of spatial WM. This difference was no longer significant after controlling for differences in MRT score, suggesting that rotational ability mediates performance in the spatial memory task for both sexes. A posterior P300 was observed in both tasks as participants encoded information into memory, however the amplitude of the P300 correlated with RT in the spatial task but not in the verbal task. Individual differences in the MRT also correlated with RT and with the amplitude of the P300, but again only in the spatial task. After splitting the analysis by sex, partial correlations controlling for MRT revealed that for males, individual differences in rotational ability completely mediated the correlation between the P300 and RT in the spatial task. This mediating effect was not observed for the female participants. The results therefore suggest a relatively stronger association in males between innate mental rotational ability, spatial memory performance, and brain electrophysiological processes supporting spatial memory.  相似文献   

12.
The perception of spatial and successive contexts of auditory information develops during child ontogeny. We compared event-related potentials (ERPs) recorded in 5- to 6-year-old children (N = 15) and adults (N = 15) in response to a digital series with omitted digits to explore age differences in the perception of successive auditory information. In addition, ERPs in response to the sound of a falling drop presented binaurally were obtained to examine the spatial content of auditory information. The ERPs obtained from the omitted digits significantly differed in the amplitude and latency of the N200 and P300 components between adults and children, which supports the hypothesis that the perception of a successive auditory structure is less automatic in children compared to adults. Although no significant differences were found in adults, the sound of a falling drop presented to the left ears of children elicited ERPs with earlier latencies and higher amplitudes of the P300 and N400 components in the right temporal area. Stimulation of the right ear caused an increasing amplitude of the N100 component in children. Thus, the observed differences in the auditory ERPs of children and adults reflect developmental changes in the perception of spatial and successive auditory information.  相似文献   

13.
The examination of the standard waves' amplitude and latency of the brain stem auditory evoked response (BAEP) was performed in 20 guinea pigs (males and females, weighing 250 to 300 g). According with the relative loudness of stimuli (90, 70, 50, 30, 10 dB SPL), the latency of BAEP waves was larger (t1 = 0.2 msec), but the conductance time between P1 to P5 was constant (3.1 to 3.6 msec). The highest wave of BAEP was P2 with an amplitude: 90 dB SPL, U = 6.5 +/- 1.2 microV; 70 dB SPL, U = 4.3 +/- 1.0 microV; 50 dB SPL, U = 3.5 +/- 0.6 microV; 30 dB SPL, U = 2.0 +/- 0.4 microV.  相似文献   

14.
Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded from 74 subjects (45 men) between 18 and 82 years of age in a simple visual detection task. On each trial the subject reported the location of a triangular flash of light presented briefly 20° laterally to the left or right visual field or to both fields simultaneously. ERPs to targets exhibited a similar morphology including P1, N1, P2, N2, and P3 components across all age groups. The principal effects of advancing age were (1) a marked reduction in amplitude of the posterior P1 component (75–150 latency) together with an amplitude increase of an anterior positivity at the same latency; (2) an increase in amplitude of the P3 component that was most prominent over frontal scalp areas; and (3) a linear increase in P3 peak latency. These results extend the findings of age-related changes in P3 peak latency and distribution to a non-oddball task in the visual modality and raise the possibility that short-latency ERPs may index changes in visual attention in the elderly.  相似文献   

15.
In most species of small mammals, males are exposed to higher levels of risk than females because they compete for mates, travel greater distances to find and procure mates, and/or defend a territory. This suggests that males and females might have different responses to risky situations, such as the presence of a predator. We tested responses to a visual predator cue (an owl silhouette) in male and female golden hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus). In a laboratory arena, there was no significant sex difference in the latency to enter the burrow or time spent in the burrow immediately after exposure to the owl silhouette. Males, however, were less likely to be active during the 3-min period following the animal’s exposure to the silhouette, indicating that male golden hamsters are more wary after exposure to an aerial predator cue than females. Most studies of responses to predators or predator cues have not considered sex differences, but our results show that males and females may have quite different responses to predator cues. Further work should be done to characterize and quantify sex differences in response to predators or predator cues.  相似文献   

16.
名词和动词加工的认知神经机制   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
方燕红  张积家 《现代生物医学进展》2007,7(10):1557-1559,1549
关于名词和动词加工的认知神经机制,神经心理学和脑功能成像研究得出两种不同的结果:前者主张名词和动词认知加工的神经机制专门化,两者的神经机制分离;后者认为名词和动词认知加工的神经机制不分离。对此,语言表征的神经生物学及语义联想学分别给出了不同的解释。  相似文献   

17.
18.
Visual event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded during a simple response task (SRT) and a discriminative response task (DRT) in remitted schizophrenic outpatients and age-matched controls to examine 2 endogenous negative potentials: NA and N2c. The NA potentials were derived by subtracting the ERPs for SRT from those for non-target stimuli in DRT. Other subtracting wave forms, N2c potentials, were calculated as the difference between ERPs for target and non-target stimuli in DRT. Schizophrenics showed retardation in NA and N2c peaks and degradation in N2c amplitude relative to controls. The NA peak latency increased as much as the latencies of N2c and reaction time for DRT in schizophrenia. The NA peak emerged prior to the N2c peak, while the NA peak latency correlated closely with the N2c latency. These results indicate that the retarded NA peak latency may serve as a physiological marker for neurobiological vulnerability of schizophrenia.  相似文献   

19.
Twenty healthy volunteers aged 21-48 years (10 males, 10 females) were submitted to pattern reversal visual evoked potentials with 15' and 30' checks. The recordings were repeated after 7 days to assess reliability and upper normal variability limits of the following parameters: latencies of N70, P100, N140 and peak-to peak amplitudes of N70-P100, P100-N140. Reliability was tested with intraclass correlation coefficient, which was excellent or good for all parameters. Test-retest variability limits were computed with = 0.01 for absolute latency differences and relative amplitude differences.  相似文献   

20.
Brain-stem auditory evoked potentials (BAEPs) were performed on 30 male and 30 female young normal Oriental subjects, using both condensation and rarefaction stimulation. The effects of sex and click polarity on the BAEP latencies and amplitudes were studied. Females had shorter absolute and interpeak latencies and higher absolute amplitudes than the males. These sex-related BAEP differences were independent of the click polarity. Rarefaction clicks produced shorter wave I latency and longer I–III interpeak latency, but the differences were significant in the female only. The polarity-related BAEP amplitude differences were essentially independent of the sex. BAEPs performed on 60 sex- and age-matched young Caucasian subjects produced similar results. The importance of establishing control BAEP values according to the sex and click polarity is emphasised.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号