首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
The hypothesis, partly based on findings in social interactions, that aggressive mice generally adopt an active behavioural strategy (cf. fight-flight) in threatening situations, while non-aggressive ones generally assume a passive strategy (cf. conservation-withdrawal) was tested using a two-way active shock avoidance paradigm. Overall, aggressive mice were found to be better active shock avoiders than non-aggressive animals; a finding that is consistent with our hypothesis. However, within the non-aggressive mice a clear dichotomy in high and low avoidance individuals was found. The high intertrial activity in the superior avoidance groups and the low activity in the poor avoidance group was interpreted as another indication of an active versus passive strategy respectively. Accordingly, it was concluded that not all non-aggressive mice assume a passive strategy, but that some mice adopt an active strategy, like all aggressive males.  相似文献   

2.
Neonatal male rats were injected with 1.25 mg of testosterone propionate (TP) and compared with oil-injected controls on the acquisition of an active and passive avoidance response at 25 days of age. The TP treated animals acquired the active avoidance response significantly faster than controls, but no differences were found between groups tested on the step-down passive avoidance task. The active-avoidance paradigm was repeated at 70 days of age, with experimental and control animals receiving the same neonatal treatment as the prepubescent subjects. Again the TP group showed facilitated acquisition of the active avoidance response. The TP treatment also produced an increase in activity levels and aversion threshold to footshock in the prepubescent animals. Therefore the active avoidance effect may be interpreted more parsimoniously as a reflection of these latter effects, rather than learning per se.  相似文献   

3.
In experiments with male white rats it was shown that 0.001 and 0.01 mg/kg of lysyl vasopressin C-terminal fragment--Pro-Lys-Gly (PLG) being injected intraperitoneally fails to influence the acquisition of active and passive avoidance and also T-maze food rewarded behaviour. The injection of PLG removed by disturbances of active avoidance caused by cyproheptadine and oxytocin. The inhibition of passive avoidance and food-rewarded behaviour was not removed by PLG.  相似文献   

4.
The active avoidance training of rats resulted in a depletion of lipid peroxidation (LPO) products in cerebral cortex. LPO inhibition was also shown in cerebral cortex of "active control" group receiving +non-combined stimuli (the effect of short-term stress). LPO inhibition was more pronounced in rats staining a training criterion compared to rats which received combined stimuli but did not reach the criterion. In the active control group LPO inhibition was accompanied by total phospholipids accumulation and cholesterol depletion in cortical lipid extracts. Irrespective of attaining the criterion in all rats trained for active avoidance the accumulation of cholesterol was seen. Active avoidance training affected also the phospholipid composition of cerebral cortex.  相似文献   

5.
The effects of cholecystokinin octapeptide sulfate ester (CCK-8-SE) and unsulfated cholecystokinin (CCK-8-NS) were studied following intraventricular administration on active avoidance and conditioned feeding behaviour of rats. In the CCK-8-NS and CCK-8-SE treated animals the acquisition of active avoidance and conditioned feeding behaviour were considerably impaired compared to the control; furthermore, these peptides caused a facilitated extinction of active avoidance and conditioned feeding behaviour. The data suggest that cholecystokinin octapeptide is capable of modifying the fear and hunger motivated behaviours of rats.  相似文献   

6.
Effects of pre- and post-natal undernutrition on learning and memory parameters were studied in albino rats. Prenatal undernutrition was induced in rat pups by restricting the mother's diet by 50% during the entire gestation period, whereas postnatal undernutrition was induced in rat pups by restriction of their diet by rotating them between lactating and non-lactating maternalised females for 12 hr each day during suckling period from 2nd day to 18th day after birth. At 2.5 to 3 months of age all the rat offsprings were subjected to (i) original and reversal discrimination learning, (ii) passive avoidance, and (iii) active avoidance and its retention tests. The results indicate that both pre- and post-natal undernutrition in rat pups caused significant deficits in original and reversal discrimination learning, retention of passive avoidance after one week retention interval, and retention of active of avoidance learning. However, both pre- and post-natal undernutrition did not show significant effect on acquisition of active avoidance and retention of passive avoidance after 24 hr retention interval.  相似文献   

7.
In homozygous D.I. rats and in Long-Evans controls the contemporaneous evolution of learning and retention of active and passive avoidance responses was studied by means of the light-dark box test. Passive avoidance performance of homozygous D.I. rats was almost equal to that of control subjects, while their active avoidance performance showed a significant deficit. Also, in homozygous D.I. rats the duration of the first freezing after being placed in the apparatus was consistently longer than in control Ss. The longer duration of this inhibitory behavior has been taken as an indication of a diminished ability of homozygous D.I. rats to respond to stress. The possible role of this diminished response to stress in determining the active avoidance deficit is discussed.  相似文献   

8.
The effects of peripherally administered cholecystokinin octapeptide sulphate ester and its unsulphated form on the active avoidance behaviour of rats were studied. The acquisition of avoidance behaviour was impaired, while extinction was facilitated, following cholecystokinin octapeptide sulphate ester or unsulphated cholecystokinin octapeptide treatment. These peptides had no action on open-field activity. It is concluded that peripherally administered cholecystokinin octapeptide influences acquisition and extinction of active avoidance behaviour and this effect is unrelated to general motor activity of the animals.  相似文献   

9.
Fear conditioning, escape and active avoidance reactions in two-way avoidance paradigm were compared in rats of different ages. Fear conditioning, but not escape and active avoidance reactions could be acquired on the 16-17th postnatal days, and the acquisition was more effective than in adults. Escape behavior matured beginning from the 18th postnatal day reaching the adult level within the 3d-4th postnatal weeks. Maturation of the mechanisms of Pavlovian (fear reaction) and instrumental (escape reaction) conditioning did not facilitate the acquisition of two-way avoidance until the 4th postnatal week, young animals displayed low acquisition in this period. The maturation of these memory processes is proposed to be related to developmental stages of different mechanisms of hippocampal plasticity.  相似文献   

10.
The effect of active immunization with conjugated serotonin-protein on the "open-field" behaviour and passive avoidance conditioning was studied in genetically different mice strains (C57BL/6 and BALB/c). The obtained immunophysiological effects of serotonin antibodies depended on genetically determined characteristics of animal behaviour. Serotonin antibodies altered rearing and crossings of the "open-field" center and retention of passive avoidance learning in C57BL/6 mice. The active immunization with conjugated serotonin-protein of BALB/c mice resulted in modulation of the "open-field" latency and both training and testing of passive avoidance behaviour.  相似文献   

11.
Rats received intracerebroventricular injections of z-DEVD-FMK (caspase-3 inhibitor) or z-FA-FMK (control peptide) in a dose of 3 nmol. Administration of z-DEVD-FMK significantly decreased the number of avoidance reactions in some blocks of trials in active avoidance (shuttle box) learning. However, only slight effect of the caspase inhibitor across the session was found. Z-DEVD-FMK impaired development of some essential components of the two-way active avoidance performance, such as escape reaction, conditioned fear reaction, and inter-trial crossings. Z-DEVD-FMK did not impair working memory in the spontaneous alternation behavior paradigm. Z-DEVD-FMK affected neither emotionality nor locomotor activity in the open-field test. It also did not influence behavior in the light-dark chamber. Measurement of caspase-3 activity in rat brain regions involved in active avoidance learning revealed z-DEVD-FMK-related inhibition of the enzyme activity most pronounced (about 30%) in the fronto-parietal cortex; a similar effect was close to significant in the hippocampus. The results suggest the involvement of brain caspase-3 in selected forms of learning.  相似文献   

12.
We reported previously that D-Pen2-[D-Pen5]enkephalin (DPDE), a delta-opioid receptor selective analog of Leu-enkephalin, impairs acquisition of an automated jump-up avoidance response in rats and acquisition of a one-way active avoidance response in mice. In the present study we investigated the effects of DPDPE on one-way avoidance conditioning in rats. The rats received two escape-only trials on day 1 and eight additional training trials on day 2. DPDPE (1.16 micrograms/kg IP) administered prior to training on day 2 impaired acquisition of the avoidance response. On the other hand, DPDPE (0.332 microgram/kg IP) administered following presentation of the two escape-only trials on day 1 significantly enhanced retention, as measured by improved one-way active avoidance performance on day 2. These results indicate that activation of delta-opioid receptors by DPDPE has a modulatory effect on acquisition and retention of aversively motivated performance.  相似文献   

13.
The action of cyclic and linear somatostatin on active avoidance and open-field behaviour was investigated in rats. Both peptides inhibited the extinction of the avoidance behaviour and slightly but not significantly increased the intertrial activity (ITR). Both peptides increased ambulation and rearing activity in a dose related fashion, while the grooming and defecation rates were not changed. Since both peptides are capable of increasing the locomotor performance this action might contribute to the effect exerted on the extinction of active avoidance behaviour.  相似文献   

14.
Male Wistar rats sustaining prefrontal cortex aspiration or sham operation at 6 days or 30 days of age were submitted to the following behavioural tests: open-field, acquisition and retention of two-way active as well as passive avoidance tasks. In the open-field the locomotor activity proved enhanced in all the aspirated animals and this enhancement lasted for 30 days. In the two-day active avoidance task, an acquisition deficit was observed in both aspirated groups; but when retrained one month later, they were able to acquire the avoidance task like sham-operated rats and no difference appeared between the groups aspirated at 6 or at 30 days of age. Concerning the passive avoidance task, no difference could be detected between aspirated and sham-operated animals of both groups except that the rats aspirated at an early age (6 days) seemed to display a better avoidance ability in the retention test. These behavioural alterations (hyperactivity and impairment of the acquisition of the 2-way active avoidance) resulted from the prefrontal cortex aspiration, at whatever age this aspiration was performed (6 days or 30 days). They disappeared after a postoperative recovery period of about one month, as evidenced by this longitudinal study.  相似文献   

15.
Effects of L-tryptophan on learning and behavior were studied in male rats with a deficit or excess of thyroid hormones. Learning was assessed in active avoidance paradigm, and behavior was estimated in the "open-field" test. It was found that L-tryptophan restored the capability of thyreoidectomized rats for acquisition and reproduction of the active avoidance reaction and increased the exploratory behavior of these rats in the open-field. In triiodothyronine treated rats, L-tryptophan eliminated a light stimulatory effects of thyroid hormones on the processes of formations and retention of the active avoidance reaction, increased the exploratory activity, but decreased grooming in the open-field.  相似文献   

16.
The method of immunization with hippocampal tissue cytosol was employed for functional disturbance of hippocampus in newborn and mature rats. This influence resulted in behavioral disorders resembling the hippocampectomy consequences. There were ++non-decremental motor activity and rearing in "open field" test and an improvement in acquisition of active avoidance response in adult rats (250-300 g). In young animals the decreased levels of motor and exploratory activity in "open field" and difficulties in active avoidance learning (in 40 days) were the main features of hippocampal disturbance. In case of immunization with neocortical cytosol no significant alterations in behavior were revealed.  相似文献   

17.
A study was made of the influence of stress (swinging) applied on the first to the fifth day of life of newborn rats, on acquistion of active unilateral avoidance, passive avoidance of a dark "dangerous" compartment, the relative weight of the brain and absolute increase in the weight of the body at an early age. It was shown that in four-week old rats the stress significantly worsens active avoidance acquisition. In two-month old animals avoidance of a dark "dangerous" compartment, i.e. "passive" avoidance, tested immediately after application of electric current is more pronounced in stressed rats. No difference was found in the relative weight of the brain and the absolute increase in the weight of the body between control and stress groups of the animals.  相似文献   

18.
It has been reported that a nonapeptide (Val-Pro-Val-Glu-Ala-Val-Asp-Pro-Met) called V-9-M is produced from procholecystokinin in the brain. Since this peptide is particularly abundant in the hippocampus, septum, and amygdala. V-9-M may be involved in memory processes. The present study was attempted to observe the effect of V-9-M on memory processes of rat performing a one-trial passive avoidance task and a platform jumping active avoidance task. The results indicate that injection of V-9-M into the lateral ventricle of the rat prevents experimental amnesia induced by electroconvulsive shock in passive avoidance testing, and that this effect is not significantly affected by cholecystokinin-8 antagonists. V-9-M also causes a long-lasting enhancement of memory in the active avoidance task. These results suggest that V-9-M may participate in the facilitation of memory.  相似文献   

19.
Stable co-existence of similar species should be facilitated by mechanisms impairing, besides exploitative, interference competition. We investigated avoidance of intra- and interspecific conflicts in a four-species community of shrews [Sorex minutus Linnaeus, 1766,S. araneus Linnaeus, 1758,Neomys anomalus Cabrera, 1907, andN. fodiens (Pennant, 1771)], using the method of dyadic encounters in a neutral arena. We tested whether the use of passive (habituation, reduction of mobility, increase of inter-individual distance, and stillness) and active (‘to-and-fro’ and ‘keeping distance’ behaviours) forms of conflict avoidance depends on species, size or domination rank. The duration of conflicts was positively correlated with mobility and negatively with inter-individual distance, whereas it was unrelated to time of stillness and the active forms. The repertoire of conflict avoidance mechanisms was not species-specific and the display of these mechanisms depended rather on the size and domination rank of animals participating in a given interaction. In contrast to rodents, shrews did not avoid conflicts by the most passive forms: freeze and stillness reactions. All other forms were used with a higher or lower efficiency by all species. However, consistent with our predictions, large shrews (asN. fodiens) used mainly the passive mechanisms of conflicts avoidance (‘wait-and-see’ strategy), whereas small shrews (asS. minutus) invest proportionally more time in active forms (‘escape’ strategy).  相似文献   

20.
The open-field behaviour of rats selected for high and low active avoidance level was studied. The data obtained showed the correlated response of two selected lines for avoidance ability with the locomotor activity and emotional reactivity in the open-field situation. The line of a low avoidance value had the low activity level and high defecation score in comparison to the alternative one and to progenitory Krushinsky--Molodkina strain. The correspondence of learning ability and emotionality-motivation performance has been discussed.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号