首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Transition from a resting state with eyes closed (REC) to a resting state with eyes open (REO) is associated with visible changes in EEG, which are traditionally considered to be a sign of reorganization of the brain’s activity in response to visual stimuli. The EEGs recorded in the REC and REO states in complete darkness, when the stimulatory effect of light to the eye’s retina was absent, were compared. Thirty healthy subjects participated in the study. EEG in the range of 1.5–50 Hz was recorded from nineteen zones of the head monopolarly. It was found that, under conditions of complete darkness, the REC and REO states significantly differed in their EEG spectral power and coherence in the Δ, θ, α1, α2, β1, β2 and γ frequency bands. Under experimental conditions, these changes in the EEG could not be induced by external influence to the visual system. Therefore, we suppose that they are correlates of the switching of involuntary preliminary attention from internally directed attention specific for the REC state to externally directed attention specific for the REO state.  相似文献   

2.
The brain’s mechanisms of imagination were studied using electroencephalography (EEG) spectral analysis in student actors and student non-actors under three experimental conditions: when they generated coherent stories on the basis of art reproductions (STORY task); listed the details of art reproductions presented (DETAIL task); and performed simple arithmetic calculations while observing a neutral background (COUNT task). Statistical analysis showed that, in α1 (7.5–10 Hz) and α2 (10–12.5 Hz) frequency bands, in both groups, execution of the STORY task, in contrast to the DETAIL task, was accompanied by significantly higher spectral power (synchronization) in most of the cortical areas studied; while, the contrasts STORY-COUNT and DETAILS-COUNT, were associated with a decrease in the EEG’s power (desynchronization) in all of the areas studied. Topographic mapping of the EEG’s power showed that, in both groups, maximal differences between the STORY and DETAILS tasks were related to the central parietal area. Maximal differences between the STORY and COUNT tasks, as well as those between the DETAILS and COUNT tasks, were related mainly to the occipital areas. Based on these findings, we consider parietal areas to be stable elements of integrated brain mechanisms underlying verbal creativity in actors and nonactors. Comparing our data with previous studies, we suggest that the parietal areas are involved in the selective inhibition of visual information processing during the involvement of brain structures in the processes of imagination.  相似文献   

3.
The features of the spatial organization of bioelectric potentials in the cases of successful and unsuccessful creative imagination (refusal or poor quality of the product) under conditions of informational oversaturation. Two groups of subjects were compared: professionals (23 healthy students of the Department of Graphic Arts) and non-professionals (34 persons whose specialties were not linked with systematic visual imagination). During the experiments, the subjects were asked to mentally create a visual image based on two simple graphic elements, a right angle and a diagonal; after recording the EEG, they had to draw the image on paper and give it a title. The total number of elements exceeded 7 ± 2; hence, information processing at the conscious level was impossible, which caused the necessity of involving the mechanisms of unconscious information processing. The quality of the created product was evaluated with regard to the degree of success of performing the task and the features of spatial EEG organization. The EEG was recorded from 24 scalp sites of a subject using a SIT-EEG portable telemetric device. In the case of the successful performance of the task by professionals, the parameters of spatial organization of bioelectric potentials, i.e., spatial synchronization (linear processes) and spatial disorder (non-linear process) were strengthened compared to the base-line level in the frontal temporal areas of the right hemisphere and parietal occipital areas of the left hemisphere. Conversely, in the nonprofessionals, these parameters increased in the frontal temporal areas of the left hemisphere and parietal occipital areas of the right hemisphere. In the case a task was not successfully performed by the professionals, the spatial disorder of bioelectric potentials increased in all the cortical areas; in the nonprofessionals, only weak changes were observed. In all situations, the groups differed also in the parameters of coherence (Coh), spectral power (Sp) of bioelectric potentials, and informational energy, which reflects the level of expenditure of the brain’s energy for information processing. The evidence obtained is interpreted in terms of electroencephalographic correlates of successful task performance by professionals and nonprofessionals, i.e., their creative imagination under the conditions of informational over-saturation.  相似文献   

4.
Features of spatial organization of neocortical potentials during mental creating of the original and standard visual images were studied. Intrahemispheric and interhemispheric coherence in different EEG frequency bands and temporal relations between relative changes in the index of linear (correlation coefficient) and non-linear (multiple entropy) processes in different neocortical areas were analysed. Number of decreased coherence values in the high-frequency alpha subband between remote neocortical points during creating of an original image was significantly lower than during creating of a standard image. During creating of the original image, the number of synchronously functioning pairs of neocortical areas and the mean level of linear correlation between the areas were higher, and correlation by the non-linear index, on the contrary, was lower than during creation of the standard image. The correlation between changes in different neocortical areas for both processes during creating of the original image was only positive, and that during creating of the standard image was both positive and negative. The conclusion was made that creative and reproductive types of visual imagination were different in the spatial distribution of coordinated changes in the linear and non-linear processes, mean magnitudes of these changes, and the character of hemispheric interaction. The data on different interhemispheric relations in diagonal and central bilateral directions raise a question about the radial representation of visual imagination.  相似文献   

5.
The identification of the networks connecting brain areas and the understanding of their role in executing complex tasks is a crucial issue in cognitive neuroscience. In this study, specific visuomotor tasks were devised to reveal the functional network underlying the cooperation process between visual and motor regions. Electroencephalography (EEG) data were recorded from twelve healthy subjects during a combined visuomotor task, which integrated precise grip motor commands with sensory visual feedback (VM). This condition was compared with control tasks involving pure motor action (M), pure visual perception (V) and visuomotor performance without feedback (V + M). Multivariate parametric cross-spectral analysis was applied to ten EEG derivations in each subject to assess changes in the oscillatory activity of the involved cortical regions and quantify their coupling. Spectral decomposition was applied to precisely and objectively determine the power associated with each oscillatory component of the spectrum, while surrogate data analysis was performed to assess the statistical significance of estimated coherence values. A significant decrease of the alpha and/or beta power in EEG spectra with respect to rest values was assumed as indicative of specific cortical area activation during task execution. Indeed alpha band coherence increased in proximity of task-involved areas, while it was suppressed or remained unchanged in other regions, suggesting the activation of a specific network for each task. According to our coherence analysis, a direct link between visual and motor areas was activated during V + M and VM tasks. The effect of visual feedback was evident in the beta band, where the increase of coherence was observed only during the VM task. Multivariate analysis suggested the presence of a functional link between motor and visual cortex subserving sensorimotor integration. Furthermore, network activation was related to the sum of single task (M and V) local effects in the alpha band, and to the presence of visual feedback in the beta band.  相似文献   

6.
This study examined how judging whether the poses of two figures are the same in tasks with delayed and simultaneous image presentation affects the participants’ reaction times and electroencephalograms (EEGs). Eighteen university students performed a delayed task, in which an image of a doll was first presented for 3 sec followed by a second image of the doll, and a simultaneous task, in which images of two dolls were presented on the left and right sides of the monitor at the same time. The dolls were shown from the front and rear angles. The participants were instructed to judge whether the images were the same as accurately and quickly as possible, and the reaction times were recorded. EEG signals were recorded from Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, and P4. The reaction times in the delayed task were found to be shorter then those in the simultaneous task, and that these times for the 0° condition were shorter than for the 180° condition. The amplitudes of EEG responses at Fp1 and Fp2 were larger than those at other electrodes, and the responses in the right hemisphere during the 180° condition and the delayed task within the α1 frequency band were smaller than the responses at other electrodes. These results indicate that cerebral activity in the frontal region of the right hemisphere is associated with the judgment of correspondence or non-correspondence in spatial compatibility tasks.  相似文献   

7.
The effects of neurohumoral status on the EEG α activity were studied in 78 women (18–27 years old) during one or two menstrual cycles using a within-subjects design. The psychometric and electroencephalographic (EEG) indices of α waves, basal body temperature, and salivary progesterone level were monitored every two or three days. The menstrual and follicular recording sessions occurred before the basal temperature rise caused by ovulation, the luteal recording session occurred after the increase in progesterone level by more than 20% compared to the day before, and the premenstrual recording sessions occurred after the decrease in progesterone level by more than 20% compared to the day before. The EEG, electromyographic (EMG) and electrocardiographic (ECG) characteristics of cognitive efficiency and psycho-emotional tension were recorded at rest and during task performance. The experiments were started in the menstrual phase in half the subjects and in the luteal phase in the other half. Psychometric characteristics, EEG α activity, EMG and ECG indices were compared for all the five phases at rest and in response to cognitive task performance. The results have shown that all psychometric and α EEG indices are menstrual-cycle-dependent. The maximum cognitive fluency, α peak frequency, α band width, and power in the α2 frequency band are observed in the luteal phase, while the maximum power in the low-frequency α1 band, as well as visual and cognitive activation calculated from α power reduction, are observed in the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle. The hypothesis that EEG α activity depends on the neurohumoral status is supported by the positive correlation of salivary progesterone level with the α peak frequency and the power in the α2 band and its negative correlation with the power in the α1 band. It is concluded that psycho-physiological recording sessions in women must be carried out with due consideration of the menstrual cycle phase.  相似文献   

8.
Functional connectivity between the prefrontal cortex and the temporal and temporo-parieto-occipital cortices in the process of preparing for the recognition of fragmented images were analyzed in adults (n = 26) and seven- to eight-year-old children (n = 20).The evaluations of the imaginary part of the complex-valued coherency for the EEG alpha-rhythm (Jα) were used as an index for the strength of cortico-cortical interactions. The Jα value was analyzed in the following three experimental conditions corresponding to different stages of readiness for visual recognition: (1) nonspecific attention holding in the period preceding a warning stimulus (S1); (2) focused attention in the interval preceding a not-yet-recognized target stimulus (S2) and (3) pretuning preceding a recognized stimulus (S3). Adult subjects tended towards a growing level of functional connectivity in α-rhythm in progressing from attention holding to focused attention preceding the emergence of a target stimulus, but children, on the contrary, demonstrated a decreasing trend. Comparing the Jα values in the subgroups of adults and children who showed the highest recognition scores in the solution of cognitive tasks helped reveal age-specific patterns in the rearrangements of cortico-cortical functional connectivity in α-rhythm in the left and right hemispheres at different stages of readiness for recognizing incomplete images. In adults, the maximal Jα values were found in the left hemisphere in the interval preceding the recognition of a target image. At this stage of pretuning, the Jα values at the leads in the left hemisphere in adults significantly exceeded those in children. The Jα values for the right hemisphere in adults were maximal during focused prestimulus attention when the image was not yet recognized and these values were significantly higher than in children under the same experimental conditions. Children showed maximal Jα values in both hemispheres during nonspecific attention. The specifics of functional connectivity observed between the prefrontal, temporal and temporo-parieto-occipital cortices in seven- to eight-year-old children during functional pretuning to the recognition of fragmented images are considered to reflect the relative immaturity of neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the voluntary attention and working memory in children of this age group.  相似文献   

9.
Students (46 young men) were asked to memorize and reproduce the order and location (on a computer monitor) of signals. Two groups of subjects were formed according to how accurately they reproduced the true location and order of the signals. Subjects of the first group, in contrast to the second group, reproduced the signal location and order at a high accuracy and with few mistakes even in the first trials. EEGs were recorded prior to the test, during memorizing, and after completion of the task. In the initial state and after completion of the task, the two groups did not differ in the EEG θ-rhythm. During memorizing the signal sequence, an increase in the coefficients of coherence was recorded in the EEG θ band of the right brain hemisphere of the first-group of students; this was not characteristic of the second group. Three systems of connection with the foci of activity were determined in the right occipital, central, and frontal cortical areas, where the coherence of the EEG θ band was significantly higher during memorizing in the students that had exhibited a high accuracy of signal reproduction. Since the right hemisphere deals mainly with the perception of visual spatial information and it is more active in processing nonverbal and stereotyped signals, we have suggested that the students of two groups employed different strategies in solving the task during memorizing.  相似文献   

10.
PurposeSkin self-examination (SSE) is one method for identifying atypical nevi among members of the general public. Unfortunately, past research has shown that SSE has low sensitivity in detecting atypical nevi. The current study investigates whether crowdsourcing (collective effort) can improve SSE identification accuracy. Collective effort is potentially useful for improving people's visual identification of atypical nevi during SSE because, even when a single person has low reliability at a task, the pattern of the group can overcome the limitations of each individual.MethodsAdults (N = 500) were recruited from a shopping mall in the Midwest. Participants viewed educational pamphlets about SSE and then completed a mole identification task. For the task, participants were asked to circle mole images that appeared atypical. Forty nevi images were provided; nine of the images were of nevi that were later diagnosed as melanoma.ResultsConsistent with past research, individual effort exhibited modest sensitivity (.58) for identifying atypical nevi in the mole identification task. As predicted, collective effort overcame the limitations of individual effort. Specifically, a 19% collective effort identification threshold exhibited superior sensitivity (.90).ConclusionsThe results of the current study suggest that limitations of SSE can be countered by collective effort, a finding that supports the pursuit of interventions promoting early melanoma detection that contain crowdsourced visual identification components.  相似文献   

11.
Group 1 (N = 30) and Group 2 (N = 22) of healthy volunteers participated in the experiment. In both groups, the EEG was recorded in the state of rest with the eyes closed (REC); at rest with eyes open (REO); and during passively watching TV channel noises (white noise). EEG was also recorded in the state of forced visual attention: when counting colored symbols appearing on the screen (group 1) and when searching for an image of a real object in the noise (group 2). The averaged values of EEG power in each derivation were calculated for every subject and for each state in the Δ, θ, α1, α2, β1, β2, and γ frequency bands. The results demonstrate that the exposure to unstructured noninformative video noise may lead to significant changes in the EEG power in various frequency bands, with the most prominent changes taking place in the α2 band. These changes are topically wide, reflecting systemic changes in the corresponding brain mechanisms, but are much less intense compared to the difference between the states of rest with the eyes open and closed.  相似文献   

12.
Changes in EEG power and coherence were analyzed in 117 subjects during the performance of the creative task of finishing well-known proverbs in a new way, so as to change, if possible, their meaning, and the noncreative task of recalling the original (well-known) version of the end of each proverb. A characteristic feature of the creative task was that proverbs and sayings are stereotypes fixed in a memory matrix. During the task performance, the EEG was recorded in 19 derivations according to the international 10–20 system. Performance of the creative task was accompanied by a highly significant and reproducible increase in the power of the γ and β2 EEG frequency bands, as well as a less pronounced decrease in the power of the ? band in the central and parietoccipital cortical areas. In addition, the performance of the creative task was also characterized by an increase in the EEG coherence in the α2, β2, and γ bands. No gender-related differences were found in the patterns of EEG changes.  相似文献   

13.
The task of discriminating the motor imagery of different movements within the same limb using electroencephalography (EEG) signals is challenging because these imaginary movements have close spatial representations on the motor cortex area. There is, however, a pressing need to succeed in this task. The reason is that the ability to classify different same-limb imaginary movements could increase the number of control dimensions of a brain-computer interface (BCI). In this paper, we propose a 3-class BCI system that discriminates EEG signals corresponding to rest, imaginary grasp movements, and imaginary elbow movements. Besides, the differences between simple motor imagery and goal-oriented motor imagery in terms of their topographical distributions and classification accuracies are also being investigated. To the best of our knowledge, both problems have not been explored in the literature. Based on the EEG data recorded from 12 able-bodied individuals, we have demonstrated that same-limb motor imagery classification is possible. For the binary classification of imaginary grasp and elbow (goal-oriented) movements, the average accuracy achieved is 66.9%. For the 3-class problem of discriminating rest against imaginary grasp and elbow movements, the average classification accuracy achieved is 60.7%, which is greater than the random classification accuracy of 33.3%. Our results also show that goal-oriented imaginary elbow movements lead to a better classification performance compared to simple imaginary elbow movements. This proposed BCI system could potentially be used in controlling a robotic rehabilitation system, which can assist stroke patients in performing task-specific exercises.  相似文献   

14.
Recent EEG studies have shown that implicit learning involving specific cortical circuits results in an enduring local trace manifested as local changes in spectral power. Here we used a well characterized visual sequence learning task and high density-(hd-)EEG recording to determine whether also declarative learning leaves a post-task, local change in the resting state oscillatory activity in the areas involved in the learning process. Thus, we recorded hd-EEG in normal subjects before, during and after the acquisition of the order of a fixed spatial target sequence (VSEQ) and during the presentation of targets in random order (VRAN). We first determined the temporal evolution of spectral changes during VSEQ and compared it to VRAN. We found significant differences in the alpha and theta bands in three main scalp regions, a right occipito-parietal (ROP), an anterior-frontal (AFr), and a right frontal (RFr) area. The changes in frontal theta power during VSEQ were positively correlated with the learning rate. Further, post-learning EEG recordings during resting state revealed a significant increase in alpha power in ROP relative to a pre-learning baseline. We conclude that declarative learning is associated with alpha and theta changes in frontal and posterior regions that occur during the task, and with an increase of alpha power in the occipito-parietal region after the task. These post-task changes may represent a trace of learning and a hallmark of use-dependent plasticity.  相似文献   

15.
Man perceives the world not only as it really is but also as it might be. In other words, perception is productive as well as reproductive, and the visual system contains mechanisms that generate new images. A study of these mechanisms and the principles of image generation is the subject of a special field in psychology which has recently been given the name "visual thinking" (1).  相似文献   

16.
Brain neurodynamics was studied by the EEG method during the performance of a task for figurative (or imaginative) creativity. The EEG was recorded in 19 standard derivations according to the international 10–20 system in 30 subjects. The following creative tasks were presented to subjects to involve them in the creative process: (Crl) thinking up and drawing an original picture; (Cr2) drawing a face, a house, and a clown in an original manner; (C1) drawing a picture from memory; and (C2) drawing geometric figures without any system. All the tasks had to be performed using a given set of geometric figures (a circle, semicircle, triangle, and rectangle). Statistical analysis of the EEG coherence function in these states for the frequency bands Δ, θ, α1, α2, β1, β2, and γ showed that the performance of creative and control tasks was associated with significant coherence changes in all the EEG frequency bands. As compared to the control tasks, performance of creative tasks caused an increase in the coherence of the α1-and α2 bands, more pronounced when creative tasks were compared with the second control task. In addition, the performance of the creative tasks (as compared to the control tasks) was accompanied by a decrease in the interhemispheric coherence of high-frequency rhythms (β2 and γ) and an increase in the intrahemispheric coherence of these rhythms. The findings are compared to the results of previous EEG studies on creative activity.  相似文献   

17.
We compared the alpha band EEG depression (event-related desynchnization, ERD) level in two tasks, involving activation of different attentional processes: visual search for a deviant relevant stimulus among many similar ones and visual oddball. Control data for the visual search task consisted of simple viewing of several stimuli being of the same shape as the relevant stimulus in the search trials. Gaze position was verified by eye tracking method. We interpreted alpha band ERD as a correlate of activation of attentional processes. Fixating the target in visual search task caused a significantly larger ERD than fixating the same stimuli in control trials over all leads. We suppose this to be related with task and visual environment complexities. The frontal ERD domination may indicate attentional control over voluntary movements execution (top-down attention). The caudal ERD may be related with updating of visual information as a result of search process (bottom-up attention). Both relevant and irrelevant stimuli in the oddball task also induced alpha band ERD, but it was larger in response to relevant one and reached maximum level over occipital leads. Domination of caudal ERD in oddball task is supposed to indicate bottom-up attention processes.  相似文献   

18.
The EEG was recorded in 19 standard derivations according to the international 10–20 system in 30 subjects during a test for figurative creativity. To involve subjects in the creative process, they were presented with the following creative tasks: (Cr1) thinking up and drawing an original picture; (Cr2) drawing a face, a house, and a clown in an original manner; (C1) drawing a picture from memory; and (C2) drawing geometric figures without any system. All the tasks had to be performed using a given set of geometric figures (a circle, semicircle, triangle, and rectangle). Statistical analysis of the EEG spectral power for the frequency bands α1, α2, β1, β2, and γ showed that the creative task performance was characterized by an increase in the EEG power in the β2 and γ bands and single differently directed power changes in the α1, α2, and β1 bands. The findings were compared to previous results of EEG studies on creative activity.  相似文献   

19.
The ideas of fractals and fractal dimension are here translated into the realm of visual psychophysics. Borders between two fields of different luminance were used. Because of the finite grain of the visual system, fractal dimension need be defined only within a certain size range. For a fractal dimension of 1.15, the just-detectable difference in fractal dimension was found to be about 0.0085, rising to about 0.015 for a fractal dimension of 1.25. Reducing exposure duration from 1 s to 0.33 s decreases sensitivity to differences in fractal dimension, but there was no gain in increasing the exposure duration. Good visual observers who are naive to the task require some training before reaching optimal performance. The ability to discriminate fractal dimension differs between fractal edges of the same fractal dimension that were generated with differing statistical programs. Even after considerable training, an observer makes 29% errors when asked to distinguish a fractal edge generated with a Gaussian random walk from one with a rectangular random walk. Gaussian random walk fractals can be more easily distinguished from Poissonian and Cauchy ones.  相似文献   

20.
Evoked, motor and final potentials and some other EEG phenomena are suggested as additional components accompanying movements, external stimuli, imagination etc. Obviously, the EEG reactions are not restricted to them. On the basis of the method of synchronic averaging a way for detecting the amplitude-frequency modulation (AFM) related to the repeated movements is proposed. This method permitted to reliably single out the EEG effects which sometimes were detected by visual analysis. As the experiments showed the depth of AFM EEG accompanying the movements was about 3-6% (in this case spontaneous AFM plays the role of noise and equals 50% or more). Relations between changes in AFM for EEG recorded from various points, as well as for EEG rhythms were investigated.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号