共查询到19条相似文献,搜索用时 84 毫秒
1.
当出现在边缘视野的一个物体被周围其他物体包围时,视觉系统对它的识别会很困难,这种现象叫做视觉拥挤效应.研究拥挤效应既有利于理解人类进行客体识别的过程,也对治疗黄斑变性、弱视和阅读障碍等视觉病变有显著的临床意义.自拥挤效应被提出以来,对拥挤效应的特性、神经机制和影响因素等都做了深入地研究.本文将系统地综述拥挤效应的研究进展,包括其特性、现有的理论假设、计算模型、可能涉及的大脑区域以及近年来利用知觉学习消除拥挤效应的一些工作,最后对该领域的未来发展给出建议.尽管在这个领域已经获得了丰富的成果,但在许多问题上仍有争议,未来还需要更为巧妙的设计和精确的技术进一步解决这些问题. 相似文献
2.
3.
根据陈霖的拓扑知觉理论,在视觉感知中,拓扑差异的检测要快于局部特征的检测.本研究组认为,这一现象可能是对有"洞"图形和无"洞"图形的感知差异所造成的.本文旨在检验这两类图形所存在的知觉差异究竟是什么.利用脑电(ERP)技术,测量"洞"和"非洞"图形所引起的视觉诱发电位(VEP),探究"洞"和"非洞"的区别.两类图形引起的VEP区别体现在N170上,"洞"图形的反转使得N170的潜伏期显著变化,而"非洞"图形的反转不会引起N170潜伏期的变化.人脸与非人脸物体的反转引起的N170变化与本结果相似.本研究认为,对"洞"的感知更偏向于整体加工,而对于"非洞"则更偏向于特征加工. 相似文献
4.
视觉感知的一系列研究都支持大范围拓扑感知的理论.拓扑性质作为整体性质,是视觉感知的基础.视觉对图形拓扑特征差异的感知要优先于对局部特征差异的感知.采用Y迷宫研究了小鼠对不同拓扑性质图形的识别.训练小鼠学习识别圆环和实心矩形这一对拓扑性质不同的图形,之后用拓扑特征相同或不同的其他图形测试小鼠,这些图形包括空心矩形、实心圆、缺口的圆环、缺口的空心矩形.实验结果表明,学会识别圆环(奖励)和实心矩形(无奖励)的小鼠无法区分实心圆和实心矩形以及圆环和空心矩形,但是能够分别从缺口圆环、缺口的空心矩形、实心圆与空心矩形组成的图形对中识别出空心矩形.因此证实了小鼠的视觉系统能够感知拓扑特征的差异并且具有对拓扑性质的概括能力.结果为拓扑知觉对视觉系统来说是基本的这一假设提供了证据. 相似文献
5.
对大白鼠组织作DNA拓扑弄构酶Ⅰ(拓扑酶Ⅰ)活力测定,见酶活力出现在胚胎早期,在胚胎发育过程及出生后不同年龄期,酶活力基本稳定;几种成年大鼠组织的酶活力彼此无显著差异;肝细胞再生及癌变,酶活力亦无显著变化。 相似文献
6.
灵长类视觉系统能够非常有效地从环境中提取稳定的不变性特征,而不受各种变换与干扰的影响.这种不变性知觉的时间与动态过程为视觉理论提供了重要的约束.我们开发了一种高速发光二极管(LED)背光速视器,能够短暂呈现1 ms的视觉刺激,并以亚毫秒级精度调整刺激呈现时间.这使得我们可以通过控制视觉输入的可获取时间,估计感觉线索积累的心理物理曲线,从而研究在图形结构优势效应任务中,拓扑、射影、仿射和欧氏不变性的相对加工速度.实验结果表明拓扑不变性所需要的呈现时间最短,这与大范围首先理论的预测相一致. 相似文献
7.
以人视觉诱发电位(VEP)反应为指标,在不同的时间频率下测定了 VEP 对方波光栅刺激的反应幅度与光栅方位的关系。当光栅方位固定时,光栅闪烁的时间频率不同,VEP 反应的波形、潜伏期和反应频率差别很大,但其反应幅度均呈现方位选择性。当光栅的时间频率为9.1Hz时,光栅为垂直和水平方位时引起的 VEP 反应幅度比倾斜方位时都大,对任何光栅方位,VEP反应幅度与光栅对比度的对数呈线性关系;与此相反,当光栅时间频率为0.4Hz 时,光栅为倾斜方位时引起的 VEP 反应幅度比垂直和水平方位时更大。 相似文献
8.
9.
采用批次培养的方法研究了邻苯二甲酸二丁酯(DBP)对绿色巴夫藻(Pavlova viridis)的生长毒性和干扰效应。实验设置了0 mg·L-1、2.5 mg·L-1、3.0 mg·L-1、3.5 mg·L-1、4.0 mg·L-1、5.0 mg·L-1和7.5 mg·L-1 共7个质量浓度梯度的DBP暴露处理组,测定了绿色巴夫藻的细胞密度、光合色素含量、叶绿素荧光特性及丙二醛(MDA)含量等指标,试图揭示DBP对绿色巴夫藻的生态毒性影响规律及机制。结果表明:DBP暴露对绿色巴夫藻的生长具有显著抑制作用,高质量浓度(5.0 mg·L-1和7.5 mg·L-1)DBP暴露下绿色巴夫藻细胞10 d内全部死亡;随着DBP暴露质量浓度增加,藻细胞叶绿素a和类胡萝卜素含量均显著降低;DBP暴露使光系统Ⅱ(PSⅡ)的最大光能转化效率(Fv/Fm)、潜在光化学活性(Fv/Fo)、实际光能转化效率(Yield)和光合电子传递速率(ETR)等指标也显著降低;DBP暴露还能够使绿色巴夫藻的细胞MDA含量显著增加。该研究进一步证实了DBP污染物对微藻光系统和酶类生理生化过程的干扰作用。 相似文献
10.
11.
12.
大分子拥挤(macromolecular crowding effect)代表了细胞内高度拥挤状态,其源于非特异性容积排斥效应,是细胞内与pH、离子强度等同等重要的生理因素。生物大分子介导的拥挤环境对于DNA-DNA、DNA-蛋白质的相互作用以及DNA高级结构、细胞核或核区结构的稳定具有重要作用。在拥挤环境中,大分子总浓度的增加将增强溶质的浓缩倾向,从而降低溶液的自由能。拥挤效应是胞内大分子环境的总体反映,具有高度的缓冲性,保证了胞内反应的稳定进行及细胞功能的正常行使。 相似文献
13.
知觉物体的定义是认知科学领域富有争议的中心问题之一.本研究通过运动诱发视盲(MIB)现象的研究发现:a.两个图形当它们是连通的,或位于同一个洞中,或是具有相同的洞的个数,更倾向于整体共同消失或重现;b.前景图形和背景图形的拓扑性质差异,较之非拓扑性质差异,显著地削弱MIB现象;c.相对于其他各种几何性质的变化,拓扑性质的变化能更快地被知觉到返回意识状态.这些结果揭示了拓扑性质定义的知觉物体是MIB过程操作的基本单元,提示了拓扑性质改变产生新知觉物体的表征可能是意识下的过程,进一步支持了知觉物体的拓扑学定义. 相似文献
14.
15.
Aurore Avarguès-Weber Adrian G. Dyer Noha Ferrah Martin Giurfa 《Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society》2015,282(1799)
Traditional models of insect vision have assumed that insects are only capable of low-level analysis of local cues and are incapable of global, holistic perception. However, recent studies on honeybee (Apis mellifera) vision have refuted this view by showing that this insect also processes complex visual information by using spatial configurations or relational rules. In the light of these findings, we asked whether bees prioritize global configurations or local cues by setting these two levels of image analysis in competition. We trained individual free-flying honeybees to discriminate hierarchical visual stimuli within a Y-maze and tested bees with novel stimuli in which local and/or global cues were manipulated. We demonstrate that even when local information is accessible, bees prefer global information, thus relying mainly on the object''s spatial configuration rather than on elemental, local information. This preference can be reversed if bees are pre-trained to discriminate isolated local cues. In this case, bees prefer the hierarchical stimuli with the local elements previously primed even if they build an incorrect global configuration. Pre-training with local cues induces a generic attentional bias towards any local elements as local information is prioritized in the test, even if the local cues used in the test are different from the pre-trained ones. Our results thus underline the plasticity of visual processing in insects and provide new insights for the comparative analysis of visual recognition in humans and animals. 相似文献
16.
Schaefer H. Martin; Levey Douglas J.; Schaefer Veronika; Avery Michael L. 《Behavioral ecology》2006,17(5):784-789
Fruit color changes during ripening are typically viewed asan adaptation to increase signal efficacy to seed dispersers.Plants can increase signal efficacy by enhancing chromatic (wavelengthrelated) and/or achromatic (intensity related) contrasts betweenfruit and background. To assess how these contrasts determinethe detectability of fruit signals, we conducted 2 experimentswith free-flying crows (Corvus ossifragus) under seminaturalconditions in a 2025 m2 aviary. Crows searched first for artificialred and black fruits and detected red fruits from a larger distance.Because artificial red fruits had higher chromatic and lowerachromatic contrasts against foliage than artificial black fruits,crows apparently prioritized chromatic contrasts. Thus, thecommon change in fruit color from red to black during ripeningdoes not increase signal efficacy to crows. In a second trial,crows searched for UV-reflecting and black blueberries (Vaccinummyrtillus) against backgrounds of foliage and sand. Againstfoliage, UV-reflecting berries had higher chromatic and achromaticcontrasts than black berries, and crows detected them from alarger distance. Against sand, UV-reflecting berries had lowachromatic contrasts and black berries low chromatic contrasts.Crows detected both fruit types equally, suggesting that theyused chromatic contrasts to detect UV-reflecting berries andachromatic contrasts to detect black berries. Birds prioritizedchromatic contrasts when searching for artificial red fruitsin foliage but not when searching for blueberries on sand. Wesuggest that the relative importance of chromatic and achromaticcontrasts is contingent on the chromatic and achromatic varianceof the background. Models of signal perception can be improvedby incorporating background-specific effects. 相似文献
17.
《Somatosensory & motor research》2013,30(3):100-105
We investigated the effect of adapting speed, duration, and distance on the frequency of occurrence, duration, and vividness of the tactile motion aftereffect (tMAE). Using a cylindrical drum with a patterned surface we adapted the glabrous surface of the right hand at two speeds (14 and 28 cm/s) and three durations (60, 120, and 240 s). Distance was explored in the interaction of adapting speed and duration. The results showed that the frequency of occurrence, duration, and vividness of the tMAE increased with adapting speed. There was also a positive relationship between adapting duration and the frequency of occurrence, but not the duration or vividness, of the illusion. Distance was only a factor when it came to the duration of the tMAE. Taken together, these results show the importance of adapting parameters, particularly speed, on the tMAE. 相似文献
18.
PETER I. MACREADIE ROD M. CONNOLLY MICHAEL J. KEOUGH GREGORY P. JENKINS JEREMY S. HINDELL 《Austral ecology》2010,35(5):515-521
Ecological theory predicts that habitat growth and loss will have different effects on community structure, even if they produce patches of the same size. Despite this, studies on the effects of patchiness are often performed without prior knowledge of the processes responsible for the patchiness. We manipulated artificial seagrass habitat in temperate Australia to test whether fish and crustacean assemblages differed between habitats that formed via habitat loss and habitat growth. Habitat loss treatments (originally 16 m2) and habitat growth treatments (originally 0 m2) were manipulated over 1 week until each reached a final patch size of 4 m2. At this size, each was compared through time (0–14 days after manipulation) with control patches (4 m2 throughout the experiment). Assemblages differed significantly among treatments at 0 and 1 day after manipulation, with differences between growth and loss treatments contributing to most of the dissimilarity. Immediately after the final manipulation, total abundance in habitat loss treatments was 46% and 62% higher than controls and habitat growth treatments, respectively, which suggests that animals crowded into patches after habitat loss. In contrast to terrestrial systems, crowding effects were brief (≤1 day), signifying high connectivity in marine systems. Growth treatments were no different to controls, despite the lower probability of animals encountering patches during the growth phase. Our study shows that habitat growth and loss can cause short‐term differences in animal abundance and assemblage structure, even if they produce patches of the same size. 相似文献
19.
Cui Ye Qian Chai Meng Zhong Yinan Wei 《Protein science : a publication of the Protein Society》2013,22(2):239-245
Folding of outer membrane proteins (OMPs) has been studied extensively in vitro. However, most of these studies have been conducted in dilute buffer solution, which is different from the crowded environment in the cell periplasm, where the folding and membrane insertion of OMPs actually occur. Using OmpA and OmpT as model proteins and Ficoll 70 as the crowding agent, here we investigated the effect of the macromolecular crowding condition on OMP membrane insertion. We found that the presence of Ficoll 70 significantly slowed down the rate of membrane insertion of OmpA while had little effect on those of OmpT. To investigate if the soluble domain of OmpA slowed down membrane insertion in the presence of the crowding agent, we created a truncated OmpA construct that contains only the transmembrane domain (OmpA171). In the absence of crowding agent, OmpA171 refolded at a similar rate as OmpA, although with decreased efficiency. However, under the crowding condition, OmpA171 refolded significantly faster than OmpA. Our results suggest that the periplasmic domain slows down the rate, while improves the efficiency, of OmpA folding and membrane insertion under the crowding condition. Such an effect was not obvious when refolding was studied in buffer solution in the absence of crowding. 相似文献