首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 718 毫秒
1.
Auditory event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded from 14 subjects with obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) before and after treatment with nasal continuous positive airway pressure (nCPAP). After 2 nights of treatment, there was dramatic improvement in the sleep patterns of the OSA patients, improvements in measures of apnea severity and oxygenation, and decrease in daytime sleepiness. The results of neuropsychological tests of a broad range of cognitive functions failed to confirm the patients' subjective reports of improvement in psychological functioning after treatment. The latencies of the N2 and P3 components were significantly prolonged prior to treatment, and there was a trend towards smaller N2 and P3 amplitude in the apneic subjects. The latency of P3 (but not N2) changed with treatment, decreasing almost to normative values. The results suggest that ERPs may be useful in documenting neural dysfunction in patients with OSA, in evaluating treatment efficacy, and possibly in determining the causes of the daytime symptoms of OSA.  相似文献   

2.
Auditory event-related potentials (ERPs) were assessed in young and elderly subjects when stimulus intensity (40 vs. 60 dB SL) and standard/target tone frequency (250/500 Hz and 1000/2000 Hz) were manipulated to study the effects of these variables on the P3(00) and N1, P2 and N2 components. Auditory thresholds for each stimulus type were obtained, and the stimulus intensity was adjusted to effect perceptually equal intensities across conditions for each subject. Younger subjects demonstrated larger P3 amplitudes and shorter latencies than elderly subjects. The low frequency stimuli produced larger P3 amplitude and shorter latencies than the high frequency stimuli. Low intensity stimuli yielded somewhat smaller P3 amplitudes and longer peak latencies than high intensity stimulus tones. Although additional stimulus intensity and frequency effects were obtained for the N1, P2 and N2 components, these generally differed relatively little with subject age. The findings suggest that auditory stimulus parameters contribute to P3 measures, which are different for young compared to elderly subjects.  相似文献   

3.
Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded in 47 patients with obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) syndrome prior to and after 6 weeks of treatment with continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP). Compared with a control group, the OSA patients showed ERP abnormalities: lengthened P3 latencies and decreased N2-P3 amplitudes. After 6 weeks of CPAP treatment, there was a highly significant improvement in the abnormal ERPs: the P3 and N2 latencies were shortened, but remained longer than in controls, and the N2-P3 and N1-P2 amplitudes were increased. No correlations could be established with various sleep variables. ERPs may be used as an electrophysiological marker of brain dysfunction; treatment of OSA with CPAP is probably responsible for functional brain modifications. On the other hand, possible relationships between the ERP abnormalities and the neuropsychological disorders observed in OSA remain to be established.  相似文献   

4.
Reaction times (RTs) and event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded in middle-aged (MA) and elderly (ELD) subjects performing an auditory selective attention task. Subjets attended to tone bursts of a specified pitch and ear of delivery and responded to occasional longer duration target tones (75 vs. 25 msec). Infrequent novel stimuli (computer synthesized sounds and digitized environmental noises) were also included in the stimulus sequence.No significant age-related differences were found in the speed or accuracy of target detection. However, in both groups, RTs were delayed (by more than 300 msec) to targets that followed novel sounds. The prolongation was greater following novel sounds in the attended ear, particularly in the ELD group.The effects of selective attention on ERPs to standard tones were isolated as negative difference waves (Nds) by subtracting ERPs to non-attended stimuli from ERPs to the same signals when attended. Nds had similar amplitudes, latencies of onset (60 msec), and distributions in ELD and MA groups. In both groups, Nd waves were more prominent following right ear stimulation, reflecting possible hemispheric asymmetries of generators in posterior temporal regions.The mismatch negativity (MMN) was isolated by subtracting ERPs to standard tones from ERPs to deviant stimuli. MMN amplitudes were reduced in the ELD group. There was also a significant change in MMN distribution with age: the MMN was larger over the right hemisphere for MA subjects but larger over the left for ELD subjects. Elderly subjects showed a trend toward smaller P3 amplitudes and delayed P3 latencies, but group differences did not reach statistical significance. ERPs to novel sounds were characterized by centrally distributed N2 and P3a components. Although the novel P3a was enhanced with attention, no novel Nd waves could be isolated. This suggests that novel sounds fell outside the focus of attention.  相似文献   

5.
Recognition of joy, anger, and fear by face expression in humans   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Behavioral and neurophysiological characteristics of a visual recognition of emotions of joy, anger, and fear were studied in 9 young healthy men and 10 women. It was shown that these emotions were identified by subjects with different rate and accuracy; significant gender differences in recognition of anger and fear were found. Recording of visual evoked potentials (VEP) from the occipital (O1/2), medial temporal (T3/4), inferior temporal (T5/6), and frontal (F3/4) areas revealed differences (related with the type of emotion) in the latencies of P150, N180, P250, and N350 waves and in the amplitude of VEP components with the latencies longer than 250 ms. These differences were maximally expressed in T3/4 derivation. The subjects could be divided in two groups. The first group was characterized by increased VEP latencies and higher amplitudes of VEP components later than 250 ms in response to anger (in comparison with other types of emotions). These phenomena were observed in all the derivations but were most pronounced in T3/4. In the second group, only late P250 and N350 components had shorter latencies during recognition of fear. VEP amplitude variations related with the type of emotions were insignificant and were recorded in the occipital and frontal areas. The two groups of subjects also differed in psychoemotional personality characteristics. It is suggested that primary recognition of facial expression takes place in the temporal cortical areas. A possible correlation of electrophysiological indices of emotion recognition with personality traits is discussed.  相似文献   

6.

Background

A paradoxical enhancement of the magnitude of the N1 wave of the auditory event-related potential (ERP) has been described when auditory stimuli are presented at very short (<400 ms) inter-stimulus intervals (ISI). Here, we examined whether this enhancement is specific for the auditory system, or whether it also affects ERPs elicited by stimuli belonging to other sensory modalities.

Methodology and Principal Findings

We recorded ERPs elicited by auditory and somatosensory stimuli in 13 healthy subjects. For each sensory modality, 4800 stimuli were presented. Auditory stimuli consisted in brief tones presented binaurally, and somatosensory stimuli consisted in constant-current electrical pulses applied to the right median nerve. Stimuli were delivered continuously, and the ISI was varied randomly between 100 and 1000 ms. We found that the ISI had a similar effect on both auditory and somatosensory ERPs. In both sensory modalities, ISI had an opposite effect on the magnitude of the N1 and P2 waves: the magnitude of the auditory and the somatosensory N1 was significantly increased at ISI≤200 ms, while the magnitude of the auditory and the somatosensory P2 was significantly decreased at ISI≤200 ms.

Conclusion and Significance

The observation that both the auditory and the somatosensory N1 are enhanced at short ISIs indicates that this phenomenon reflects a physiological property that is common across sensory systems, rather than, as previously suggested, unique for the auditory system. Two of the hypotheses most frequently put forward to explain this observation, namely (i) the decreased contribution of inhibitory postsynaptic potentials to the recorded scalp ERPs and (ii) the decreased contribution of ‘latent inhibition’, are discussed. Because neither of these two hypotheses can satisfactorily account for the concomitant reduction of the auditory and the somatosensory P2, we propose a third, novel hypothesis, consisting in the modulation of a single neural component contributing to both the N1 and the P2 waves.  相似文献   

7.
The aim of this research is to study the impact of extreme prematurity on the cognitive development of the child as assessed at age 5 years 9 months. Our samples include 15 healthy prematures born between 25 and 28 weeks of gestational age carefully matched with 15 full-term controls. In the first experiment, two different auditory stimuli were presented to the subjects who listened passively without instruction. The second experiment consisted of a standard visual oddball task in which the subjects were instructed to `catch' two different animals, by pushing a left or right button for a moose (n=120) or a raccoon (n=40), respectively. In the auditory task, 3 ERP peaks were analyzed (frontal N100 and P3a, temporal P2). All premature children demonstrated normal early frontal N100 and temporal P2 responses. The group differences were apparent in the late positivity (P3a) where controls showed a larger amplitude to the rare tones applied evenly to both ears. In contrast, the prematures did not show sensitivity to rare tones but showed a larger P3a upon left ear stimulation, when compared to the right. Also, the ERPs to the visual oddball task showed normal early positivities (P250–300) in the premature group. Once again, deviations from the normal were evident in late waves. The ERPs recorded from prematures showed a more diffuse topography especially between 500 and 600 ms post-stimulus and around the posterior area (P550). The succeeding negativity (SW) was not altered in the premature group. The ERP data suggest that premature children, even without clinically apparent problems, convey specific ERP singularity when engaged in a task that involves complex processing.  相似文献   

8.
A delay in the movement initiation was shown to entail an augmentation of the N2 wave ascending front and a delay in the ERPs P3 wave in frontal leads in healthy subjects and in parkinsonian patients. The modulation of the N2 wave seems to reflect inhibitory processes in frontal areas of the brain.  相似文献   

9.
Twenty-two right-handed subjects were asked either to identify sad, neutral, or laughing faces presented on a computer monitor or to view passively the same pictures without classification. Visual evoked potentials were recorded from F3/4, C3/4, P3/4, O1/2, and T5/6 derivations. In comparison with the passive viewing, the emotion recognition was characterized by the higher level of cortical activity reflected in the higher N1, N2, N3 amplitudes and shortened latencies of N1, P2, and N2 waves. In contrast, the latencies of the later P3 and N3 waves were longer. During emotion recognition, the dynamical brain mapping technique revealed symmetrical activation of the frontocentral areas and only the right-side activation during the passive perception. Factor analysis demonstrated a complication of N2 structure in the task of face emotion recognition. A principal component corresponding to the descending part of the N2 wave was revealed, which probably reflected the stage of image classification.  相似文献   

10.
Neuronal pathways for the lingual reflex in the Japanese toad   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
1. Anuran tongue is controlled by visual stimuli for releasing the prey-catching behavior ('snapping') and also by the intra-oral stimuli for eliciting the lingual reflex. To elucidate the neural mechanisms controlling tongue movements, we analyzed the neuronal pathways from the glossopharyngeal (IX) afferents to the hypoglossal (XII) tongue-muscle motoneurons. 2. Field potentials were recorded from the bulbar dorsal surface over the fasciculus solitarius (fsol) to the electrical stimulation of the ipsilateral IX nerve. They were composed of three successive negative waves: S1, S2 and N wave. The S1 and S2 waves followed successive stimuli applied at short intervals (10 ms or less), whereas the N wave was strongly suppressed at intervals shorter than 500 ms. Furthermore, the S1 wave had lower threshold than the S2 wave. 3. Orthodromic action potentials were intra-axonally recorded from IX afferent fibers in the fsol to the ipsilateral IX nerve stimuli. Two peaks found in the latency distribution histogram of these action potentials well coincided with the negative peaks of the S1 and the S2 waves of the simultaneously recorded field potentials. Therefore, the S1 and S2 waves should represent the compound action potentials of two groups of the IX afferent fibers with different conduction velocities. 4. Ipsilateral IX nerve stimuli elicited excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) in the tongue-protractor motoneurons (PMNs) and the tongue-retractor motoneurons (RMNs). Inhibitory postsynaptic potentials were not observed. 5. The EPSPs recorded in PMNs had mean onset latencies of 6.4 ms measured from the negative peaks of the S1 wave. The EPSPs were facilitated when paired submaximal stimuli were applied at intervals shorter than 20 ms, but were suppressed at intervals longer than 30 ms. Furthermore, the EPSPs were spatially facilitated when peripherally split two bundles of the IX nerve were simultaneously stimulated. 6. On the other hand, the EPSPs recorded in RMNs had shorter onset latencies, averaging 2.5 ms. In 14 of 43 RMNs, early and late EPSP components could be reliably discriminated. The thresholds for the early EPSP components were as low as those for the S1 waves, whereas for the late EPSP components the thresholds were usually higher than those for the S2 waves.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)  相似文献   

11.
Auditory event-related potentials (ERP) were registered to the dichotically presented white noise stimuli (duration 1500 ms, band 150-1200 Hz). Abrupt or gradual change ofinteraural time difference in the middle of stimuli (750 ms after sound offset) was perceived as an apparent auditory image (AI) instant relocation or motion from the midline to one of the ears. In responses these stimuli two ERPs were observed: one to the sound onset, and second--to the onset of motion or AI relocation. ERPs to AI relocation differed from those to sound onset in longer components latencies (123 ms versus 105 ms for N 1,227 ms versus 190 ms for P2). In responses to AI motion component latencies were even longer (N1: 137 ms, P2: 240 ms); N1 amplitude was greater at sites contralateral to the AI motion direction.  相似文献   

12.
Parameters of visual search (reaction time, mistakes) were investigated in 11 young healthy volunteers, under changing parameters of target symbol (form, color and location) in surroundings of white distracters (flankers). In 6 cortical leads: P3, P4, T3, T4, T5, T6, monopolar event-related potentials (ERPs) were registered and analyzed for the late endogenous components: N2, P3, because these very components have changed under verifying of search (so called late selection). In the hard search (similar target and distractors) the increase of search time was accompanied by the delay of P3 component and diminishing of its amplitude. The definite (the knowledge of) target position resulted by the decrease search time and decrease of P3 wave latency as compared with situation of uncertainty in respect to the position of target. Alteration of the target stimulus colour were led to abolishment (cancel) of inhibition effect of distracters: parameters of IRP have not differed from parameters of ERPs to solid target stimulus without the flankers. A high correlation of search time and parameters of P3 wave was revealed. Changes of IRPs in different kinds of search were similar in parietal and temporal leads. This suggests that the parietal and temporal cortical areas function as united system, under search of target in environment.  相似文献   

13.
The effects of spatial selective attention upon ERPs associated with the processing of word stimuli were investigated. While subjects maintained central eye fixation, ERPs were recorded to words presented to the left and right visual fields. In each of 6 runs, subjects focussed attention to alternate fields to perform a category-detection task. Pairs of semantically related and repeated words were embedded in the word lists presented to the attended and unattended visual fields. Consistent with prior studies, the P1-N1 visual ERP was larger when elicited by words in attended spatial locations. A large negative slow wave identified as N400 was elicited by attended, but not unattended, words. For attended words, N400 was smaller for semantically primed or repeated words. We concluded that spatial selective attention can modulate the degree to which words are processed, and that the cognitive processes associated with N400 are not automatic.  相似文献   

14.
Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded from 74 subjects (45 men) between 18 and 82 years of age in a simple visual detection task. On each trial the subject reported the location of a triangular flash of light presented briefly 20° laterally to the left or right visual field or to both fields simultaneously. ERPs to targets exhibited a similar morphology including P1, N1, P2, N2, and P3 components across all age groups. The principal effects of advancing age were (1) a marked reduction in amplitude of the posterior P1 component (75–150 latency) together with an amplitude increase of an anterior positivity at the same latency; (2) an increase in amplitude of the P3 component that was most prominent over frontal scalp areas; and (3) a linear increase in P3 peak latency. These results extend the findings of age-related changes in P3 peak latency and distribution to a non-oddball task in the visual modality and raise the possibility that short-latency ERPs may index changes in visual attention in the elderly.  相似文献   

15.
Event-related Potential Study of Novelty Processing Abnormalities in Autism   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
To better understand visual processing abnormalities in autism we studied the attention orienting related frontal event potentials (ERP) and the sustained attention related centro-parietal ERPs in a three stimulus oddball experiment. The three stimulus oddball paradigm was aimed to test the hypothesis that individuals with autism abnormally orient their attention to novel distracters as compared to controls. A dense-array 128 channel EGI electroencephalographic (EEG) system was used on 11 high-functioning children and young adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and 11 age-matched, typically developing control subjects. Patients with ASD showed slower reaction times but did not differ in response accuracy. At the anterior (frontal) topography the ASD group showed significantly higher amplitudes and longer latencies of early ERP components (e.g., P100, N100) to novel distracter stimuli in both hemispheres. The ASD group also showed prolonged latencies of late ERP components (e.g., P2a, N200, P3a) to novel distracter stimuli in both hemispheres. However, differences were more profound in the right hemisphere for both early and late ERP components. Our results indicate augmented and prolonged early frontal potentials and a delayed P3a component to novel stimuli, which suggest low selectivity in pre-processing and later-stage under-activation of integrative regions in the prefrontal cortices. Also, at the posterior (centro-parietal) topography the ASD group showed significantly prolonged N100 latencies and reduced amplitudes of the N2b component to target stimuli. In addition, the latency of the P3b component was prolonged to novel distracters in the ASD group. In general, the autistic group showed prolonged latencies to novel stimuli especially in the right hemisphere. These results suggest that individuals with autism over-process information needed for the successful differentiation of target and novel stimuli. We propose the potential application of ERP evaluations in a novelty task as outcome measurements in the biobehavioral treatment (e.g., EEG biofeedback, TMS) of autism.  相似文献   

16.
Auditory nerve brain-stem (ABR) and somatosensory evoked responses (SER) were recorded in cats as body temperature was uniformly lowered from 37 to 27°C. Analysis of the results showed that the alterations in the evoked responses were due to disturbances induced both in axonal propagation and synaptic transmission by the hypothermia. By studying the first wave of the SER, which is solely an axonal event, and by assuming reasonable values for the total synaptic delay and axonal propagation times along the ABR pathway, it was concluded that this lesion model induced an effect on synaptic transmission 1.3–1.7 times greater than that on axonal propagation. There was a strong inverse correlation between wave latency and body temperature, with slightly steeper slopes for the longer latency waves. Wave amplitudes were not correlated with temperature. Furthermore, the wave latencies and amplitudes were generally not dependent on stimulus rate.  相似文献   

17.
ERPs were recorded from subjects who were required to push a button in response to a given conjunction of spatial frequency and orientation (target), and to ignore conjunctions sharing with the target only frequency (frequency-relevant), only orientation (orientation-relevant), or neither (irrelevant). Differences between ERPs to irrelevant and frequency-relevant stimuli were identified as frontal selection positivity (Fz, 150–200 msec post-stimulus), selection negativity (Oz, just after 200 msec), and vertex N2b (200–250 msec). For relevant orientations, the selection negativity and the N2b were also found, but the frontal selection positivity was elicited only when the spatial frequency was relevant as well. Neither of these 3 waves was sensitive to spatial frequency or orientation per se. Other waves (N80, N180) did show such sensitivity, but this was independent of stimulus relevance. Hence, direct evidence for the neural-specificity theory, stating that task-related selective ERP responses reflect differential enhancement of activity in stimulus-specific pathways, was not obtained. It is further concluded that ERPs associated with selection of frequency and orientation are highly comparable to those associated with other visual dimensions (e.g., color), and that selection of multiple visual dimensions may be hierarchical at one level of processing and independent at another.  相似文献   

18.
The different processing of global and local properties of compound visual stimuli was studied with event-related brain potentials (ERPs) in the present experiment. It was found that, compared with the identification of global properties, the discrimination of local properties elicited longer RTs, lower accuracies, increased amplitudes of P1, decreased amplitudes of N1, and longer latencies of N2 and P3. The conflict of global and local properties increased the amplitudes of P2, decreased the amplitudes of P3, and prolonged latencies of N2 and P3. These results indicated that the advantage of global processing occurs at an early perceptual stage, and the attentional mechanisms for global and local processing may be different.  相似文献   

19.
Perception and discrimination of auditory and speech stimuli in children aged 7-9 years with either receptive (n=6) or expressive (n=5) type of special language impairment and 7 healthy age-matched controls was investigated using evoked potential technique. The measurements were performed with a 32-channel Neuroscan electroencephalographic system. Two types of stimuli were applied, pure tones (1 kHz and 2 kHz) and double syllabi consisting of one consonant and one vocal characteristic of Croatian language. The stimuli were presented in an oddball paradigm, requiring a conscious reaction for the subjects. Latencies and amplitudes of P1, N1, P2, N2, P3, N4, and SW waves were analized, as well as the reaction time and number of responses. There were found no statistically significant difference between children with special language impairment and the control group in average response time and number of responses to tone burst or double syllable. Analysis of variance of all used variables showed a statistically significant difference in P3 and Sw wave latencies after double syllable stimulation, P3 and N4 waves latencies after target stimulation, P2 and Sw wave amplitude; and in N1 wave amplitude after pure tone stimulation. Our study showed that children with speech and language disorder take longer time to perceive and discriminate between either tonal or speech auditory stimuli than children with typical speech and language development.  相似文献   

20.
The P3(00) event-related potentials (ERPs) elicited by visual stimuli in two visual tasks were assessed in depressed patients (12 patients with major depression and 11 patients with bipolar disorder) and compared with those of 20 age-matched normal controls. At remission, the ERPs from 18 of the depressed patients were again recorded. The visual oddball (VO) paradigm presented both target and standard visual stimuli and the simple visual (SV) paradigm presented a target but no standard visual stimulus. Subjects performed the VO task significantly less accurately than the SV task, as reflected by the behavioral measures (reaction-time and task accuracy). Depressed patients of the bipolar group showed longer P3 peak latency for the VO task and no change in P3 amplitude. No significant differences were found in any other ERP component between the groups. During remission, slowing RTs and reduced P3 peak latencies were observed for both major depression and bipolar disorder groups. Thus, the P3 ERP may be an index of the contribution of the slowed central processing to psychomotor retardation in clinically homogenous samples of depressive patients performing an appropriately demanding task.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号