共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 62 毫秒
1.
本文主要从生物学角度出发,从遗传学、神经内分泌学、行为学和神经解剖学方面综述了同性恋发生的神经内分泌机制。除了遗传和环境影响之外,围产期异常的性激素环境导致了性倾向的异常。在个体发育过程中通过下丘脑-垂体-性腺轴和下丘脑-垂体-肾上腺轴的作用,性腺类固醇激素和多种神经递质相互作用调节个体的性倾向的形成。同时,本文还提出目前该领域的研究进展、有待于解决的问题,以及进一步的研究方向,希望有助于人们正确认识同性恋的发生,关注同性恋生理和心理健康,预防青少年同性恋的发生。 相似文献
2.
长期进化过程中,猎物形成了对捕食者先天的敏感性。捕食风险对小型哺乳动物的影响主要包括行为的改变、神经环路的激活和内分泌的变化。行为的改变包括抑制运动活动、降低非防御行为以及改变生活环境。一些研究用c-fos免疫组化的方法测定了大鼠在暴露于猫的气味后大脑被激活的区域,主要包括中间杏仁核、下丘脑腹内侧区、背内侧区、前乳头状核和导水管周围灰质(periaqueductal gray)。猎物对捕食应激的反应还表现在一些内分泌指标的改变,一些激素如皮质酮(Corticoserone,CORT)和促肾上腺皮质激素(Adrenocorticotropic hormone,ACTH)的含量会显著的增加,同时在一些雄性动物中,睾丸激素的含量会有所降低。捕食者和猎物的关系近年来已经成为很多学科研究的有用工具。 相似文献
3.
白腰文鸟发声行为的神经发育 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
本文研究了 5~ 15 0日龄雄性白腰文鸟 (Lonchurastriataswinhoei)不同年龄段的声谱变化以及这种变化的神经调制机制。结果如下 :(1)HVC、RA和AreaX三个发声核团的神经联系基本接近成年鸟的水平后 ,幼鸟才开始学习鸣叫 (约 45日龄 ) ;(2 )HVC、RA和AreaX达到成年核团体积时 (约 80日龄 ) ,幼鸟才具有成年雄鸟的鸣叫模式 ;(3)发声控制核团的发育与核团间的神经支配有关 ,而基本不受鸣唱行为的影响 ,HVC、RA和AreaX的最快增长时间段各不相同 ,三个核团随年龄增长而呈现体积增长的显著变化 (one wayANOVA ,P <0 0 5 ) ,但各核团在任意两个时间段的体积差异并不都显著。结果提示 :发声行为产生的时间和发展与发声控制核团的发育、核团间的神经联系有关 ,最终的体积发育程度受内在遗传力的作用 ,同时可能还受神经核团建立正常神经联系时间的影响 相似文献
4.
5.
6.
免疫-神经内分泌相互作用郑仲承(中国科学院上海生物化学研究所,上海200031)关键词免疫,神经,内分泌,相互作用我们都知道,“意志可以战胜疾病”,睡眠不足会使机体的免疫能力下降,都说明精神状态影响着健康。精神状态与神经系统有关;健康状态则与内分泌和... 相似文献
7.
8.
神经内分泌讲座(二):———促甲状腺激素分泌的神经内分泌调节 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
甲状腺是人及动物体内十分重要的内分泌器官。它分泌的甲状腺激素调节机体的代谢、生长发育等过程。甲状腺的功能又受到下丘脑-腺垂体-甲状腺轴的调节,而下丘脑又受脑的其它部位的功能控制。因此,通过下丘脑的神经内分泌作用,将神经系统的活动与甲状腺的内分泌功能有... 相似文献
9.
由中国生理学会消化内分泌生殖代谢生理专业委员会、中国神经科学会神经免疫专业委员会和中国解剖学会神经解剖专业委员会联合主办的“2008年神经内分泌暨神经免疫内分泌学术研讨会”于2008年7月20—23日在云南省大理市顺利举行,来自全国11个省市和香港地区的综合大学、医学院校和有关研究机构等近30个单位的80余名代表出席了会议。 相似文献
10.
11.
Puppet‐reared and parent‐reared captive‐bred California condor (Gymnogyps californianus) juveniles were studied before their release into the wild. Behavioral data were collected during social interactions within two cohorts of juveniles (N = 11) and their adult mentors (N = 5). The purposes of this study were to (1) document the social behaviors of mentored juvenile California condors, and (2) compare social behaviors for two different rearing methods (puppet‐reared versus parent‐reared) during two phases of the mentoring process (San Diego Wild Animal Park versus release sites). Of the 17 behaviors examined by 2 × 2 analyses of variance (ANOVAs), two significant interactions between the rearing method and mentoring phase were found: pulls feathers and feeds alone. For both behaviors, parent‐reared condors engaged in these activities more often at the zoo and less often at the release pens than did the puppet‐reared condors. The main effect of rearing was also significant for two behaviors: near others, and receives contact aggression from other. Parent‐reared birds were more likely to be near another bird and receive contact aggression, regardless of mentoring phase, than puppet‐reared birds. The effect size for 16 of the 17 behaviors was greater for the rearing method than for mentoring phase. Rearing method differences may persist long‐term, as parent‐reared adult mentors were significantly more aggressive than puppet‐reared adult mentors. Dominance relations were examined for both cohorts, with the first cohort exhibiting a strong linear relationship (h′ = 0.86, P = 0.018), whereas the second cohort exhibited a moderate but non‐significant linear hierarchy (h′ = 0.63, P = 0.21). The rearing method had no effect on dominance among the juveniles, but adults were probably dominant to juveniles (P = 0.052; the difference was nearly significant). Although social behaviors between the two rearing groups were similar in most respects, this study is the first to document measurable differences between puppet‐ and parent‐reared captive‐bred California condor juveniles. Zoo Biol 27:1–18, 2008. © 2007 Wiley‐Liss, Inc. 相似文献
12.
The co-evolution of individual behaviors and social institutions 总被引:9,自引:0,他引:9
We present agent-based simulations of a model of a deme-structured population in which group differences in social institutions are culturally transmitted and individual behaviors are genetically transmitted. We use a standard extended fitness accounting framework to identify the parameter space for which this co-evolutionary process generates high levels of group-beneficial behaviors. We show that intergroup conflicts may explain the evolutionary success of both: (a) altruistic forms of human sociality towards unrelated members of one's group; and (b) group-level institutional structures such as food sharing which have emerged and diffused repeatedly in a wide variety of ecologies during the course of human history. Group-beneficial behaviors may evolve if (a) they inflict sufficient fitness costs on outgroup individuals and (b) group-level institutions limit the individual fitness costs of these behaviors and thereby attenuate within-group selection against these behaviors. Thus, the evolutionary success of individually costly but group-beneficial behaviors in the relevant environments during the first 90,000 years of anatomically modern human existence may have been a consequence of distinctive human capacities in social institution building. 相似文献
13.
Michael J. Wade 《Journal of theoretical biology》1982,95(2):351-368
Using a classical population genetic model, the necessary conditions for the spread of genes that determine social behaviors and the rate of spread of these genes are derived. The influence of 1, 2, 3, or k inseminations per female on these conditions is investigated for both diploid and haplodiploid organisms. These results are then extended to a population in which there are arbitrary variations among females in their numbers of mates. These results do not depend upon assuming equal paternity by all inseminating males; the effects of sperm competition and unequal paternity are also derived. The rates and conditions for social evolution in these groups of complex composition are discussed in relation to Hamilton's rule.For all models, the total change in gene frequency, Δq, is partitioned into two components: (1) , the change in gene frequency caused by selection within groups; this component is always negative, illustrating that individual selection always operates against the evolution of social behaviors; and (2) ΔqG, the change in gene frequency caused by selection between groups; this component is generally positive. Hamilton's rule is shown to specify the necessary and sufficient conditions for , that is, for selection among kin groups to over-ride individual selection within kin groups. 相似文献
14.
15.
Background
Microglia, one of the glial cells, play important roles in various brain pathologies including psychiatric disorders. In addition, microglia have recently been proved to monitor synaptic reactions via direct-touching even in normal brain. Human microglia may modulate various social/mental functions, while microglial social/mental roles remain unresolved especially in healthy humans. There is no known drug with the specific effect of modulating microglia. Therefore, using minocycline, a tetracycline antibiotic and the most famous microglial inhibitor, is one of the best alternative approaches to clarify microglial functions on human social/mental activities.Methodology/Principal Findings
We conducted a double-blind randomized trial of trust game, a monetary decision-making experiment, with ninety-nine human adult males who decided how much to trust an anonymous partner after a four-day administration of minocycline. Our previous pilot trial indicated a positive effect of minocycline, while the underlying mechanisms were not clarified. Therefore, in this trial with larger samples, we additionally measured the effects of anxiety and personality. The monetary score in trust game was significantly lower in the minocycline group. Interestingly, participants’ ways of decision-making were significantly shifted; cooperativeness, one component of personality, proved to be the main modulating factor of decision-making in the placebo group, on the other hand, the minocycline group was mainly modulated by state anxiety and trustworthiness.Conclusions/Significance
Our results suggest that minocycline led to more situation-oriented decision-making, possibly by suppressing the effects of personality traits, and furthermore that personality and social behaviors might be modulated by microglia. Early-life events may activate human microglia, establish a certain neuro-synaptic connection, and this formation may determine each human’s personality and personality- oriented social behaviors in later life. To explore these mechanisms, further translational research is needed.Trial Registration
UMIN clinical trial center UMIN000004803 相似文献16.
Julián Hurtado-López David F. Ramirez-Moreno Terrence J. Sejnowski 《Journal of computational neuroscience》2017,43(2):127-142
We propose a mathematical model of a continuous attractor network that controls social behaviors. The model is examined with bifurcation analysis and computer simulations. The results show that the model exhibits stable steady states and thresholds for steady state transitions corresponding to some experimentally observed behaviors, such as aggression control. The performance of the model and the relation with experimental evidence are discussed. 相似文献
17.
18.
19.
Colony defense is a necessary but dangerous task for social insects, and nest defensive behaviors often lead to a premature
death of the actor. As an extreme form of colony defense, self-sacrificial behaviors have evolved by kin selection in various
social insects. Most self-sacrificial defensive mechanisms occur in response to an acute threat to the colony, but some behaviors
are preemptive actions that avert harm to the colony. Self-sacrifice has also been observed as a form of preemptive defense
against parasites and pathogens where individuals will abandon their normal colony function and die in self-exile to reduce
the risk of infecting nestmates. Here, we provide an overview of the self-destructive defense mechanisms that eusocial insects
have evolved and discuss avenues for future research into this form of altruism. 相似文献
20.
Feral cats,Felis catus, showed 2 different defecating behaviors: they buried feces or left then exposed on the ground. We studied the relationship
between the spatial distribution of buried/exposed feces and body weight of the producers, using the bait-marking method.
Disproportionally small numbers of feces were found in the core areas of home ranges. The proportion of exposed feces in the
core area was not different from that outside the core area, either and was not correlated with body weight in either sex.
Heavier male cats buried feces at higher frequencies at sites closer to the centers. Such relationships were not found for
females. These results suggest that social rank influences defecating behaviors in male feral cats.
alphabetical order 相似文献