共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Hitoshige Hayaki 《Primates; journal of primatology》1985,26(4):343-360
Social play of juvenile and adolescent chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, was studied, by analyzing processes of play and interindividual relationships
in play. The results are discussed in relation to communication mechanisms. Play was initiated in several ways. Communication
about play seems to depend on the receiver's interpretation: They can interpret the sender's behavior as play, referring to
(1) play signals accompanied by the behavior, (2) transformation of the behavior in timing, strength, or rhythm, and (3) situation
of the occurrence. Initiation attempts sometimes failed because one hesitated in playing with the other. Although the stronger
often reduced his/her activity during play, play tended to escalate in activity. Players may enjoy such escalation. Play also
had a mechanism not to escalate into fighting. Play was influenced by individuals other than the players. The third party's
movement often affected the players' interaction. The term play does not indicate a behavior itself but the context of the
behavior: The players interpret their behaviors in their play context. 相似文献
2.
Shigeo Uehara Toshisada Nishida Hiroyuki Takasaki Mahale Mountains Wildlife Kohshi Norikoshi Takahiro Tsukahara Ramadhani Nyundo Miya Hamai 《Primates; journal of primatology》1994,35(3):275-281
K Group, originally one of the two major study groups of chimpanzees since 1965 in the Mahale Mountains National Park, western
Tanzania, was almost extinct by 1983: at most seven individuals remained in the group at the beginning of 1983. K Group continued
to exist for more than four years, but in 1987 a male was left alone at the age of 15 after all the other chimpanzees of the
group emigrated or disappeared. Since then he has been observed sporadically for more than five years only within the former
range of K Group, without having any contact with the many resident chimpanzees of the neighboring M Group, the other major
study group. The present observations reconfirm the strong philopatric tendency of adult male chimpanzees. 相似文献
3.
Michio Nakamura William C. McGrew Linda F. Marchant Toshisada Nishida 《Primates; journal of primatology》2000,41(3):237-248
Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, scratch other individual's bodies while they groom them. This behavioral pattern of “social scratch” is another example of locality-specific social behavior, or custom, as it is not found in the Gombe National Park, Tanzania, about 150 km north of Mahale, nor has it been reported from any other sites of chimpanzee study. Frequency of social scratch was correlated with frequency of social grooming, but not with frequency of self-scratch. Frequencies of social scratch per grooming bout among adult and adoles-cent males, and from lactating females to infants or juveniles, were high, and among males, higher-ranking males especially received more. These facts indicate some social function of the behavior. Social scratch was directed mostly to the dorsal side of the body. However, when lactating females social scratched to infants or juveniles, they scratched other body parts. Social scratch was not lateralized to left or right. We present four hypotheses on the functional origin and on the learning process of this cultural behavioral pattern. 相似文献
4.
The M group chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, began to feed on three agricultural fruit species, guava, mango and lemon. It took them 7–8 years until they began to taste these fruits since the villagers left the park area in 1974. Although adult chimpanzees are conservative in their feeding habits, they are capable of rapidly acquiring new feeding habits, or new traditions, once they notice that the food is suitable. 相似文献
5.
Dominance among male chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park,Tanzania: A preliminary study
Hitoshige Hayaki Michael A. Huffman Toshisada Nishida 《Primates; journal of primatology》1989,30(2):187-197
Dominance relationships among male chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, were analyzed. Although all
adolescent males were unequivocally subordinate to all adult males, dominance relationships within the age classes were much
less clear. Especially among adolescent males, few pant-grunts or agonistic interactions occurred. While adolescent males
frequently pant-grunted at adult males, these latter males, except the alpha and the youngest, rarely pant-grunted to one
another. This suggests that a difference of social status exists between adolescent and adult males. Adult males rarely display
overt dominance to one another probably because the presence of other males affects their interactions. Moreover, they seem
to try to keep their dominance relationship ambiguous when making it overt is not advantageous to them. This may be a political
way for males to coexist with one another in a unit-group. 相似文献
6.
Hanamura S Kiyono M Lukasik-Braum M Mlengeya T Fujimoto M Nakamura M Nishida T 《Primates; journal of primatology》2008,49(1):77-80
A flu-like disease spread among chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the M group at Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania, from June to July 2006. This epizootic or epidemic killed up
to 12 chimpanzees. The obvious evidence of their deaths came from finding the bodies of three infants who had previously shown
some symptoms of the disease. At least one of these infants died of pneumonia. In addition, nine chimpanzees were missing
after the outbreak. These individuals were assumed to have been killed by this epizootic because most of them had contact
with the infected individuals on the last days they were observed. We also found two dead bodies during this period, which
were thought to be those of two missing individuals. We confirmed 23 (35.4%) of 65 individuals of the M group showed some
symptoms of the disease, although most of them (20/23) did not die. More than half of them (14/23) had kin showing symptoms.
Since this epizootic may have been caused by contact with humans, it will be necessary to establish and follow appropriate
protocols for researchers, tourists, and park staff to observe chimpanzees, and to explore the mechanism of disease transmission
from humans to chimpanzees and among chimpanzees. 相似文献
7.
Hiroyuki Takasaki Yukio Takahata Mariko Hiraiwa-Hasegawa Richard W. Byrne Takayoshi Kano 《Primates; journal of primatology》1986,27(4):517-519
A case of unusually early postpartum resumption of estrous cycling (<7 months) was recorded for a young, presumably primiparous
female in the M group of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Mahale Mountains National Park, western Tanzania. The female showed estrous cycling while lactating her infant, and
mated with young and low-ranking males as well as with the alpha male. 相似文献
8.
Hiroyuki Takasaki 《Primates; journal of primatology》1985,26(2):121-129
The mating behavior of receptive females in the M group chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the Mahale Mountains, western Tanzania, was studied by intensive sampling over a period of 5 months. Restrictive matings were observed only between parous females mostly in the ovulatory stage and prime adult males, in particular the alpha. Young, nulliparous and/or non-ovulatory females tended to mate with immature or low-ranking adult males. By contrast, older, parous and/or ovulatory females tended to mate with adult and higher-ranking males. These confirmed the results of a previous extensive study of the same group. In addition, gradual shift from opportunistic to restrictive matings after inter-unit-group transfer was revealed for 2 ex-K-group parous females. The findings were discussed in relation to the life history of female chimpanzees. A National Park since 1984. 相似文献
9.
The copulatory activities of bonobos (Pan paniscus) of Wamba, Zaire, were compared with those of chimpanzees (P. troglodytes schweinfurthii) of Mahale, Tanzania. The copulation rates of adult male bonobos were equal to or lower than those of adult male chimpanzees. The copulation rates of adult female bonobos were approximately equal to those of adult female chimpanzees who were in maximal genital swelling, but it should be much higher than those of the adult female chimpanzees throughout the birth interval. The copulation rates of adolescent male bonobos were lower than those of adolescent male chimpanzees, whereas the copulation rates of adolescent female bonobos were much higher than those of adolescent female chimpanzees. It was suggested that the bonobos of Wamba did not copulate more promiscuously than did the chimpanzees of Mahale. The female bonobos may show “receptivity”, whereas female chimpanzees may show rather “proceptivity”. 相似文献
10.
Tool-use for drinking water by immature chimpanzees of Mahale: prevalence of an unessential behavior
Matsusaka T Nishie H Shimada M Kutsukake N Zamma K Nakamura M Nishida T 《Primates; journal of primatology》2006,47(2):113-122
Use of leaves or sticks for drinking water has only rarely been observed during long-term study of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Mahale. Recently, however, we observed 42 episodes of tool-use for drinking water (73 tools and two cases of using tool-sets) between 1999 and 2004. Interestingly, all of the performers were immature chimpanzees aged from 2 to 10 years. Immature chimpanzees sometimes observed the tool-using performance of others and subsequently reproduced the behavior, while adults usually paid no attention to the performance. This tool-use did not seem to occur out of necessity: (1) chimpanzees often used tools along streams where they could drink water without tools, (2) they used tools for drinking water from tree holes during the wet season when they could easily obtain water from many streams, and (3) the tool-using performance sometimes contained playful aspects. Between-site comparisons revealed that chimpanzees at drier habitats used tools for drinking water more frequently and in a more conventional manner. However, some variations could not be explained by ecological conditions. Such variations and the increase in this tool-use in recent years at Mahale strongly suggest that social learning plays an important role in the process of acquiring the behavior. We should note here that such behaviors that lack obvious benefits or necessity can be prevalent in a group. 相似文献
11.
Matsumoto-Oda Akiko Hosaka Kazuhiko Huffman Michael A. Kawanaka Kenji 《International journal of primatology》1998,19(6):999-1011
We studied factors affecting party size and composition of wild chimpanzees at Mahale (M group) over an 11-month period. Parties with 1–5 individuals were most frequent (37.8%; 153/405 parties); they included 94.7% of all male parties (n = 76) and 81.3% of all female parties (n = 75). The median of monthly values was the standard for analysis. We divided the year into four periods based on the median size of monthly bisexual parties (30.9 individuals; includes both males and females): monthly bisexual party sizes were larger in May–June (period II) and October–January (period IV) and smaller in February–April (period I) and July–September (period III). Only bisexual parties changed in size with period. The number of fruit items (=species) eaten was fewer in periods II and IV when abundance per item appeared to be great. The sizes of bisexual parties, which included cycling females with maximal anogenital swelling, were larger, and their representation (%) in all bisexual parties was greater in periods III and IV. The numbers of both cycling females and cycling females with maximal anogenital swelling were also larger in periods III and IV. The percentage of cycling females with maximal anogenital swelling was greater in periods II and III. The results of this study and those of Nishida (1979) suggest that seasonal variation in party size of Mahale chimpanzees maintains a relatively consistent annual cycle. The factors assumed to affect party sizes are fruit availability and the presence of cycling females with maximal anogenital swelling. 相似文献
12.
Shigeo Uehara Mariko Hiraiwa-Hasegawa Kazuhiko Hosaka Miya Hamai 《Primates; journal of primatology》1994,35(1):49-55
Usurpation of alpha status by males is a relatively rare event among wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, western Tanzania, where long-term field studies have been carried out since 1965. Here we report three such cases and compare the fate of those defeated alpha males. The formation of effective alliances with other adult males is considered to be crucial in determining what will happen to the defeated alpha male in the rank order after the loss of his status. 相似文献
13.
Katharin Pieta 《International journal of primatology》2008,29(4):845-864
Studies of reproduction among chimpanzees traditionally have focused on the mating strategies of males. However, less is known
about the mating strategies of female chimpanzees and whether they demonstrate mate choice. I investigated sexual behavior
and female mate preference in the chimpanzees of the Kanyawara community. To estimate mate preferences, I analyzed female
proceptivity and resistance rates of 6 estrous females toward a total of 13 males as well as male solicitation and aggression
rates toward females. Males solicited some females more often than others for mating and preferred them throughout estrus,
not only during the periovulatory period (POP), when conception was most likely. In contrast, though females had strong mate
preferences in both non-POP and POP, their mate preferences were not consistent between the 2 phases. The shift in mate preferences
is evidence of a promiscuous yet tactical mating strategy to confuse paternity. Further, females were more proceptive and
generally less resistant toward eschewed males in non-POP and more proceptive and less resistant toward preferred males in
POP. Hence, the results indicate that females attempted to mate selectively during the fertile phase. Kanyawara female chimpanzees
appear to change their mating strategies and selectivity during estrus and thus may pursue a mixed reproductive strategy.
The tactic may allow females to deceive males, indicating that promiscuity among chimpanzee females may be more strategic
than previously thought. 相似文献
14.
Michael A. Huffman Shunji Gotoh Linda A. Turner Miya Hamai Kozo Yoshida 《Primates; journal of primatology》1997,38(2):111-125
A longitudinal study of nematode infection in chimpanzees was conducted between 1989 and 1994 on the M group chimpanzees of
the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania during two annual dry and rainy season periods and a third rainy season. Chemical
and physical antiparasite properties of medicinal plant use against the strongyle nematodeOesophagostomum stephanostomum have recently been reported at Mahale. Here, the incidence of nematode infections were analyzed for seasonal trends to elucidate
the possible influence of parasite infection on previously reported seasonality of medicinal plant use and to test the hypothesis
that the use of these plants is stimulated byO. stephanostomum. The number of chimpanzees infected byO. stephanostomum was significantly higher in the rainy season than in the dry season of both 1989–1990 and 1991–1992. However, the incidence
ofTrichuris trichura andStrongyloides fuelleborni showed no seasonality. Reinfection of individuals byO. stephanostomum occurred in synchrony with annual variation in rainfall: there was a sharp rise in the occurrence of new infections per individual
within one to two months after the beginning of the first heavy rains of the season. This pattern coincides with the reproductive
cycle of this nematode species.O. stephanostomum (95%) infections were associated significantly more frequently with medicinal plant use than eitherT. trichiura (50%) orS. fuelleborni (40%) infections. These observations are consistent with previous reports for the increased use of these plants during the
rainy season and are consistent the hypothesis that medicinal plant use is stimulated byO. stephanostomum infection. 相似文献
15.
Koichi Koshimizu Hajime Ohigashi Michael A. Huffman Toshisada Nishida Hiroyuki Takasaki 《International journal of primatology》1993,14(2):345-356
Potential medicinal plants for wild chimpanzees have been studied in order to discover their physiologically active compounds. Tests of the physiological activity of 3 plant species—Vernonia amygdalina, Aspilia mossambicensis, andFicus exasperata—indicate that they contain a variety of active compounds. From one species,V. amygdalina, an antitumor agent and 2 possible antitumor promoters are identified. Furthermore, steroid glucosides were isolated as the bitter substances. These structurally new compounds are expected to exhibit a number of significant physiological activities. The chemical investigation of possible medicinal plants used by chimpanzees should be helpful in recovering naturally occurring compounds of medicinal significance for human use. 相似文献
16.
17.
18.
Cranial capacity measurements of 18 individuals (9 ♂, 9 ♀) of a total collection of 277Pan troglodytes verus skulls were taken using four different methods:- two conventional filling methods and two mathematical methods. The two mathematical
analyses normally used in hominids, were for the first time specifically modified forPan. The results probably present a more accurate estimation of total cranial capacity inPan and specifically inPan troglodytes verus (352 cm3) than previously available. Sexual differences related to this trait were also measured. Regardles of the methods used,Pan troglodytes verus seems to manifest the smallest cranial capacity of all subspecies ofPan. 相似文献
19.
Shigeo Uehara 《Primates; journal of primatology》1986,27(1):1-13
Sex differences in animal prey intake were revealed by fecal analysis among wild chimpanzees of the large-sized M-group (ca.
100 members) in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania: prime adult or old males feed more on vertebrates, while adult
females more onCamponotus ants. By contrast, such differences were not obvious in the neighboring, small-sized K-group (ca. 20–30 members), despite
the similar environment in which the two unit-groups lived. Such sex and group differences may be explained in terms of various
factors, either ecological or social, or both, but social factors seem most responsible in particular for the group differences.
It seems likely that increased capture rate of vertebrates per unit-group in the larger-sized M-group results in increased
per capita intake of meat among prime adult or old males. Also, the more frequent interactions among prime adult or old males
of M-group appear to reduce the frequency of theirCamponotus ant-fishing behavior. 相似文献
20.
Monica L. Wakefield 《International journal of primatology》2008,29(4):907-929
In mammals, access to mates is probably the most important influence on male reproductive success, whereas foraging efficiency is probably the most important influence on female reproductive success Emlen and Oring (Science 197:215–223, 1977). Male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are highly gregarious and form cooperative relationships with other males. In contrast, female social relationships vary within and between populations. Females in most East African populations, e.g., Gombe, Mahale, Kibale-Kanyawara, are less gregarious than males and spend most of their time alone or with only their dependent offspring. Researchers have attributed low female gregariousness to the high potential for feeding competition. I provide the first data on association patterns and agonistic interactions of female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) from the unusually large Ngogo community, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Ngogo females were less gregarious than males, but spent a mean of 64% of their time in association with ≥1 other females and as much time in all-female parties as they did alone. Further, female dyads associated nonrandomly and they formed associative cliques. Association levels within cliques were similar to those among the relatively gregarious West African chimpanzee females at Taï (Pan troglodytes verus) and among bonobo (P. paniscus) females. Agonistic interfemale interactions were extremely rare, and monthly mean party size and the numbers of anestrous females per party do not correlate significantly with fruit availability. Thus, Ngogo females maintained relatively high levels of gregariousness, but avoided detrimental feeding competition by preferentially associating with a small subset of other community females. 相似文献