首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1700篇
  免费   100篇
  2022年   8篇
  2021年   16篇
  2020年   12篇
  2019年   16篇
  2018年   21篇
  2017年   14篇
  2016年   28篇
  2015年   59篇
  2014年   54篇
  2013年   63篇
  2012年   78篇
  2011年   86篇
  2010年   57篇
  2009年   42篇
  2008年   63篇
  2007年   74篇
  2006年   86篇
  2005年   66篇
  2004年   83篇
  2003年   81篇
  2002年   68篇
  2001年   55篇
  2000年   47篇
  1999年   57篇
  1998年   26篇
  1997年   20篇
  1996年   14篇
  1995年   13篇
  1994年   15篇
  1993年   9篇
  1992年   35篇
  1991年   32篇
  1990年   36篇
  1989年   35篇
  1988年   37篇
  1987年   37篇
  1986年   29篇
  1985年   28篇
  1984年   16篇
  1983年   24篇
  1982年   18篇
  1981年   7篇
  1980年   12篇
  1979年   13篇
  1978年   12篇
  1977年   19篇
  1976年   13篇
  1975年   14篇
  1974年   9篇
  1972年   8篇
排序方式: 共有1800条查询结果,搜索用时 78 毫秒
1.
Journal of Ethology - The article Herding mechanisms to maintain the cohesion of a harem group.  相似文献   
2.
This paper has studied the evolution of a predator-prey Volterra-Lotka ecosystem with saturation effect for the general case where both predator and prey evolve. We have interesting results under the evolutional condition, as follows: (1) the predator population and the ratio of predator to prey populations increase; (2) the parameters of the prey drift in the direction of increasing multiplication rate and saturation level, while the parameters of the predator drift in the direction of decreasing death rate.  相似文献   
3.
Sequence analyses of the complete brown bear, Ursus arctos, mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) genome have detected scattered single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) that define distinct mtDNA haplogroups in phylogeographical studies. The degraded DNA in historical samples, such as stuffed or excavated specimens, however, is often not suitable for sequence analyses. To address this problem, we developed an amplified product length polymorphism (APLP) analysis for mtDNA‐haplogrouping U. arctos specimens by detecting haplogroup‐specific SNPs. We verified the validity and utility of this method by analysing up to 170‐year‐old skin samples from U. arctos specimens collected widely across continental Eurasia. We detected some of the same haplogroups as those occurring in eastern Hokkaido (Japan) and eastern Alaska in continental Eurasia (the Altai and the Caucasus). Our results show that U. arctos in eastern Hokkaido and eastern Alaska descended from a common ancestor in continental Eurasia, and suggest that U. arctos occupied several refugia in southern Asia during the Last Glacial Maximum. © 2014 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2014, 111 , 627–635.  相似文献   
4.
5.
6.
Sexually dimorphic glands have convergently appeared in animals and are often responsible for the production of pheromones. In the suborder Laniatores of the order Opiliones (Arachnida), glands of such type are widespread, but there is not a single paper on how they are used. Using Scanning Electron Microscopy and a behavioral approach, we describe glandular openings and how these glands are used, in the harvestmen Gryne perlata and Gryne coccinelloides (Cosmetidae). Males of these two species have glandular openings on the metatarsi of legs I and on the metatarsi IV. Males were shown rubbing the glands of the metatarsi I against their other legs, whereas glands on the metatarsi IV are gently touched on the substrate or rubbed either against other legs, or against the substrate. Not all behaviors were seen in both species.  相似文献   
7.
Human and nonhuman primates comprehend the actions of other individuals by detecting social cues, including others’ goal-directed motor actions and faces. However, little is known about how this information is integrated with action understanding. Here, we present the ontogenetic and evolutionary foundations of this capacity by comparing face-scanning patterns of chimpanzees and humans as they viewed goal-directed human actions within contexts that differ in whether or not the predicted goal is achieved. Human adults and children attend to the actor’s face during action sequences, and this tendency is particularly pronounced in adults when observing that the predicted goal is not achieved. Chimpanzees rarely attend to the actor’s face during the goal-directed action, regardless of whether the predicted action goal is achieved or not. These results suggest that in humans, but not chimpanzees, attention to actor’s faces conveying referential information toward the target object indicates the process of observers making inferences about the intentionality of an action. Furthermore, this remarkable predisposition to observe others’ actions by integrating the prediction of action goals and the actor’s intention is developmentally acquired.  相似文献   
8.
9.
Enzyme(s) capable of decomposing N-acetylglucosaminyl ribitol teichoic acid prepared from the cell wall of Staphylococcus aureus FDA 209 P was obtained from the culture supernatant of a gram-negative, rod-shaped, spore-forming soil bacterium. Properties of the bacterium were very similar to those of Bacillus circulans.  相似文献   
10.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号