首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1043篇
  免费   75篇
  国内免费   34篇
  2023年   16篇
  2022年   14篇
  2021年   27篇
  2020年   30篇
  2019年   22篇
  2018年   28篇
  2017年   28篇
  2016年   32篇
  2015年   37篇
  2014年   46篇
  2013年   61篇
  2012年   19篇
  2011年   34篇
  2010年   26篇
  2009年   45篇
  2008年   43篇
  2007年   43篇
  2006年   39篇
  2005年   37篇
  2004年   31篇
  2003年   30篇
  2002年   34篇
  2001年   27篇
  2000年   28篇
  1999年   25篇
  1998年   18篇
  1997年   19篇
  1996年   18篇
  1995年   21篇
  1994年   21篇
  1993年   19篇
  1992年   19篇
  1991年   24篇
  1990年   11篇
  1989年   15篇
  1988年   18篇
  1987年   15篇
  1986年   14篇
  1985年   18篇
  1984年   30篇
  1983年   16篇
  1982年   21篇
  1981年   11篇
  1980年   6篇
  1979年   6篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   2篇
  1972年   2篇
  1958年   1篇
排序方式: 共有1152条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
The amount of light intercepted by vertically oriented, shingle leaves of juvenile Monstera tenuis vines growing in forest understory was compared to the amount of light the leaves would intercept if they were horizontal. Light levels were monitored using quantum sensors and hemispherical photography. Shingle leaves absorb less light than they would if the leaves were horizontal at the same positions, and the difference increases with height in the forest. Modeling based on measured photosynthetic light responses and light interception suggests that at 1 m height, 75% more carbon could be gained if leaves were horizontal instead of vertical. Because the vertical leaf orientation reduces light interception, other selective factors are likely of greater importance in favoring the evolution of the shingle-leaved growth form.  相似文献   
92.
Variation in GC content, GC skew and AT skew along genomic regions was examined at third codon positions in completely sequenced prokaryotes. Eight out of nine eubacteria studied show GC and AT skews that change sign at the origin of replication. The leading strand in DNA replication is G-T rich at codon position 3 in six eubacteria, but C-T rich in two Mycoplasma species. In M. genitalium the AT and GC skews are symmetrical around the origin and terminus of replication, whereas its GC content variation has been shown to have a centre of symmetry elsewhere in the genome. Borrelia burgdorferi and Treponema pallidum show extraordinary extents of base composition skew correlated with direction of DNA replication. Base composition skews measured at third codon positions probably reflect mutational biases, whereas those measured over all bases in a sequence (or at codon positions 1 and 2) can be strongly affected by protein considerations due to the tendency in some bacteria for genes to be transcribed in the same direction that they are replicated. Consequently in some species the direction of skew for total genomic DNA is opposite to that for codon position 3. Received: 2 February 1998 / Accepted: 15 June 1998  相似文献   
93.
Summary Glomerulocyte cellulosic bundles ofPolyzoa vesiculiphora were investigated by microdiffraction and high-resolution electron microscopy. In each bundle, hundreds of cellulose microfibrils, having a rectangular cross-sectional shape, are packed regularly with their 0.6 nm lattice planes parallel to each other. Lattice images reveal that the 0.6 nm plane is parallel to the longer edge of the cross section which is similar to the lattice organization of cellulose with a squarish cross section inValonia spp. More interestingly, all the microfibrils in a bundle have the same directionality of crystallographic c-axis, which suggests that the biosynthesis of the microfibrils within particular bundle occurs unidirectionally.  相似文献   
94.
Search orientation is controlled by information that is stored genetically, idiothetically or through learning and that is gained from internal and external sensory systems. Sources of variation in motor patterns could derive from any of the types of information, or could originate during the execution of the instructions derived from orientation information. Local search after resource utilization in the flies Musca demestica and Drosophila melanogaster can be addressed as an initial increase in turning rate and decrease in locomotory rate, followed by a transition in both measures to a lower turning rate and a higher locomotory rate characteristic of ranging. The search tactic of flies promotes accrual of discrete resources in a patch. A forward-moving tendency, combined with a “noisy” looping motor pattern, enhances resource-finding in both linear and non-linear resource patterns. The period before a fly leaves a patch—its giving-up-time—is a function of current resource density and the slope of the transition from local search to ranging: once a fly switches to the relatively straight path characteristic of ranging, the chances of it leaving the patch depends on whether or not another resource is encountered. Duration of local search is dependent on the genotype of an individual, level of starvation or satiation, resource quality (sucrose concentration) and quantity (patch size) and presence of resource-specific sensory information.  相似文献   
95.
96.
[Correction Notice: An erratum for this article was reported in Vol 20(3) of Dreaming (see record 2010-17362-006). In the article, a third author was not listed in the byline and some acknowledgment information was also missing: Third Co-Author: Anja S. G?ritz, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany Additional Acknowledgment: We thank the operators of the sites www.panopia.de, www.yougov.de, and www.studivz.net for providing participants. This work was in part supported by DFG grant GO 1107/4-1 to G?ritz. The online version of the article has been corrected.] Recently, a large meta-analysis showed that women tend to recall their dreams more often than men. Despite this robust finding, studies focused on explaining the gender difference in dream recall frequency are scarce. The present findings of an online survey indicate that sex role orientation—expressivity/femininity—was related to dream recall frequency but did not fully explain the gender difference in dream recall frequency. Future studies should investigate other variables—in addition to sex role orientation—like verbal memory, recall of emotional experiences, and/or frequency of talking about emotional matters that might play a role in explaining the gender difference in dream recall. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
97.
Reports an error in "Gender, sex role orientation, and dream recall frequency" by Michael Schredl and Olaf Lahl (Dreaming, 2010[Mar], Vol 20[1], 19-24). In the article, a third author was not listed in the byline and some acknowledgment information was also missing: Third Co-Author: Anja S. G?ritz, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany. The name appears in this record. Additional Acknowledgment: We thank the operators of the sites www.panopia.de, www.yougov.de, and www.studivz.net for providing participants. This work was in part supported by DFG grant GO 1107/4-1 to G?ritz. The online version of the article has been corrected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2010-05656-002.) Recently, a large meta-analysis showed that women tend to recall their dreams more often than men. Despite this robust finding, studies focused on explaining the gender difference in dream recall frequency are scarce. The present findings of an online survey indicate that sex role orientation—expressivity/femininity—was related to dream recall frequency but did not fully explain the gender difference in dream recall frequency. Future studies should investigate other variables—in addition to sex role orientation—like verbal memory, recall of emotional experiences, and/or frequency of talking about emotional matters that might play a role in explaining the gender difference in dream recall. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved)  相似文献   
98.
99.
100.
Abstract.  1. Females of the parasitoid fly Emblemasoma auditrix find their host cicadas ( Okanagana rimosa ) using the acoustic signals produced by the host. The phonotactic behaviour of the parasitoid was studied with regard to differently structured habitats.
2. Habitats were modified experimentally within a distance of 2.5 m (approximately the natural range of phonotaxis) from a loudspeaker broadcasting a model of the host calling song.
3. Video analysis showed that in an open habitat (no landmarks) more than 60% of the flies performed a direct flight towards the loudspeaker.
4. In structured habitats (with one to three landmarks) more than 90% of the flies landed on their way to the acoustic target.
5. In about 50% of the landings flies paused for several seconds indicating re-orientation during that time. Several flies included sequences of walking in their approach behaviour, whereby most walking occurred close to the loudspeaker.
6. In summary, the phonotactic approach and host finding depends on the habitat structure.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号