首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   126篇
  免费   9篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2024年   1篇
  2022年   3篇
  2021年   2篇
  2020年   2篇
  2019年   3篇
  2018年   6篇
  2017年   3篇
  2016年   3篇
  2015年   3篇
  2014年   5篇
  2013年   6篇
  2011年   1篇
  2010年   4篇
  2009年   11篇
  2008年   8篇
  2007年   6篇
  2006年   4篇
  2005年   8篇
  2004年   4篇
  2003年   2篇
  2002年   1篇
  2001年   3篇
  2000年   1篇
  1999年   2篇
  1998年   3篇
  1997年   1篇
  1996年   1篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   1篇
  1993年   2篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   1篇
  1989年   3篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   5篇
  1985年   1篇
  1983年   4篇
  1982年   2篇
  1979年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   2篇
  1973年   1篇
  1970年   2篇
  1968年   1篇
  1967年   1篇
  1966年   1篇
  1964年   1篇
  1956年   1篇
排序方式: 共有137条查询结果,搜索用时 246 毫秒
31.
In this theoretical and speculative essay, I propose that insights into certain aspects of neural system functions can be gained from viewing brain function in terms of the branch of Statistical Mechanics currently referred to as “Modern Critical Theory” [Stanley, H.E., 1987. Introduction to Phase Transitions and Critical Phenomena. Oxford University Press; Marro, J., Dickman, R., 1999. Nonequilibrium Phase Transitions in Lattice Models. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK]. The application of this framework is here explored in two stages: in the first place, its principles are applied to state transitions in global brain dynamics, with benchmarks of Cognitive Neuroscience providing the relevant empirical reference points. The second stage generalizes to suggest in more detail how the same principles could also apply to the relation between other levels of the structural-functional hierarchy of the nervous system and between neural assemblies. In this view, state transitions resulting from the processing at one level are the input to the next, in the image of a ‘bucket brigade’, with the content of each bucket being passed on along the chain, after having undergone a state transition. The unique features of a process of this kind will be discussed and illustrated.  相似文献   
32.
Hackles have been raised in biosemiotic circles by T. L. Short’s assertion that semiosis, as defined by Peirce, entails “acting for purposes” and therefore is not found below the level of the organism (2007a:174–177). This paper examines Short’s teleology and theory of purposeful behavior and offers a remedy to the disagreement. Remediation becomes possible when the issue is reframed in the terms of the complexity sciences, which allows intentionality to be understood as the interplay between local and global aspects of a system within a system. What is called “acting for purposes” is not itself a type of behavior so much as a relationship between a dynamic system that “exists for a purpose” and its microprocesses that “serve purposes.” The “intentional object” of philosophy is recast here as the holistic self-organized dynamics of a system, which exists for the purpose of self-maintenance, and that constrains the parts’ behaviors, which serve the purpose of forming the system. (A “system” can be any emergent, e.g. an abiotic form, an adapted species, a self, a conditioned response, thought, or a set of ideas.) The self-organized whole, which is represented to the parts in their own constrained behaviors, assumes the guiding function so long attributed to the mysterious “intentional object.” If emergent self-causation is not disallowed, creative originality, as well as directionality, becomes part of the definition of purposeful behavior. Thus, key tools used here, required for understanding emergence, come from poetics rather than semoitics. In the microprocesses of self-organization, I find what I call “accidental” indices and icons — which are poetic in the sense that they involve mere metonymic contiguity and metaphoric similarity — and which are preferentially selected under constrained conditions allowing radically new connections to habituate into an “intentional” self-organized system that, not coincidentally, has some of the emergent characteristics of a conventional symbolic system.
Victoria N. AlexanderEmail:
  相似文献   
33.
34.
Parasitism of the tobacco hornworm, Manducasexta, by the braconid wasp Cotesiacongregata, induces developmental arrest of the host in the larval stage. During the final instar of the host, its juvenile hormone (JH) titer is elevated, preventing host metamorphosis. This study investigated the effects of hormonal manipulation of the host on the parasitoid’s emergence behavior. The second larval ecdysis of the wasps coincides with their emergence from the host, and application of the juvenile hormone analogue methoprene to day 4 fifth instar hosts either delayed or totally suppressed the subsequent emergence of the wasps. Effects of methoprene were dose-dependent and no parasitoids emerged following treatment of host larvae with doses >50 μg. Parasitoids which failed to emerge eventually succumbed as unecydsed pharate third instar larvae in the hemocoel of the host. Effects of host methoprene treatment on parasitoid metamorphosis were also assessed, and metamorphic disruption occurred at much lower dosages compared with doses necessary to suppress parasitoid emergence behavior. The inhibitory effect of methoprene on parasitoid emergence behavior appears to be mediated by effects of this hormone on the synthesis or release of ecdysis-triggering hormone (ETH) in the parasitoid, the proximate endocrine cue which triggers ecdysis behavior in free-living insects. ETH accumulated in the epitracheal Inka cells of parasitoids developing in methoprene-treated hosts, suggestive of a lack of hormone release. Thus, the hormonal modulation of parasitoid emergence behavior appears to be complex, involving a suite of hormones including JH, ecdysteroid, and peptide hormones.  相似文献   
35.
F. Menu 《Oecologia》1993,96(3):383-390
In the chestnut weevil Curculio elephas, adult emergences spread over 3 or 4 years due to prolonged larval diapause in some individuals. Weevils with an extended diapause emerge, on the average, 1–10 days before those with simple diapause, but whatever the age of insects, emergences occur always from mid-August to early October. When the summer is dry, some adults cannot emerge because of the hardness of the soil. Emergence sucess of adults is smaller in females than in males. The result is that the sex ratio is female-biased before emergence and male-biased after. Summer drought cannot be predicted by the chestnut weevil, and when the soil is dry 27–78% of females cannot emerge and do not reproduce. The year after a summer drought, many reproducing females may emerge from larvae with prolonged diapause. These results suggest an evolutionary influence on the variability in diapause duration. Computer simulations and observations do not support the hypothesis that the main cause of variation in diapause length is the existence of several distinct genotypes within populations. On the contrary, our data strengthen the hypothesis for coin-flipping plasticity discussed in a previous paper.  相似文献   
36.
An atypical brook charr (Salvelinus fontinalis) spawning area   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
Synopsis Brook charr (Salvelinus fontinalis) spawned successfully on a 0.3 m thick aggregation of waterlogged sticks, woodchips and debris overlying the soft ooze bottom of a small Precambrian Shield lake. Brook charr were apparently attracted to groundwater seeping up through the aggregation and utilized the tangle of various sized sticks as a spawning substrate. Eggs were deposited in late October and alevins emerged from the area in late March, periods that coincide with the conventional spawning of native brook charr on gravel areas in nearby lakes. Our observations support the contention that upwellin, water is more important than bottom type in stimulating brook charr to select a spawning site.  相似文献   
37.
38.
We have produced computational simulations of multi-agent systems in which wolf agents chase prey agents. We show that two simple decentralized rules controlling the movement of each wolf are enough to reproduce the main features of the wolf-pack hunting behavior: tracking the prey, carrying out the pursuit, and encircling the prey until it stops moving. The rules are (1) move towards the prey until a minimum safe distance to the prey is reached, and (2) when close enough to the prey, move away from the other wolves that are close to the safe distance to the prey. The hunting agents are autonomous, interchangeable and indistinguishable; the only information each agent needs is the position of the other agents. Our results suggest that wolf-pack hunting is an emergent collective behavior which does not necessarily rely on the presence of effective communication between the individuals participating in the hunt, and that no hierarchy is needed in the group to achieve the task properly.  相似文献   
39.
Reservoir interactions and disease emergence   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Animal populations act as reservoirs for emerging diseases. In order for transmission to be self-sustaining, a pathogen must have a basic reproduction number R0>1. Following a founding transmission event from an animal reservoir to humans, a pathogen has not yet adapted to its new environment and is likely to have an R0<1. However, subsequent evolution may rescue the pathogen from extinction in its new host. Recent applications of branching process theory investigate how the emergence of a novel pathogen is influenced by the number and rates of intermediate evolutionary steps. In addition, repeated contacts between human and reservoir populations may promote pathogen emergence. This article extends a stepping-stone model of pathogen evolution to include reservoir interactions. We demonstrate that the probability of a founding event culminating in an emerged pathogen can be significantly influenced by ongoing reservoir interactions. While infrequent reservoir interactions do not change the probability of disease emergence, moderately frequent interactions can promote emergence by facilitating adaptation to humans. Frequent reservoir interactions promote emergence even with minimal adaptation to humans. Thus, these results warn against perpetuated interaction between humans and animal reservoirs, as occurs when there are ecological or environmental changes that bring humans into more frequent contact with animal reservoirs.  相似文献   
40.
Tanimoto J 《Bio Systems》2008,92(1):82-90
This paper reports an intelligent agent equipped with two-layer finite state machines (FSMs) that can communicate by turning lighting on and off, leading to social cooperation that solves the dilemma situation, modeled by a one-shot 2x2 game. This communication between two gaming agents can be observed in hero- and leader-type dilemma games, where alternating reciprocity, repeating cooperation (C)-defeat (D) after D-C, is the equal pareto optimum instead of a sequence of mutual cooperation that is the equal pareto optimum for a prisoner's dilemma (PD) game.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号