首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   428762篇
  免费   49128篇
  国内免费   199篇
  2018年   3658篇
  2017年   3638篇
  2016年   4890篇
  2015年   6480篇
  2014年   7592篇
  2013年   11414篇
  2012年   12657篇
  2011年   12821篇
  2010年   8602篇
  2009年   7983篇
  2008年   11632篇
  2007年   11821篇
  2006年   11366篇
  2005年   10769篇
  2004年   10668篇
  2003年   10348篇
  2002年   10129篇
  2001年   20320篇
  2000年   20480篇
  1999年   16593篇
  1998年   5812篇
  1997年   6080篇
  1996年   5724篇
  1995年   5340篇
  1994年   5458篇
  1993年   5313篇
  1992年   13779篇
  1991年   13187篇
  1990年   13112篇
  1989年   13036篇
  1988年   11900篇
  1987年   11315篇
  1986年   10436篇
  1985年   10477篇
  1984年   8480篇
  1983年   7380篇
  1982年   5588篇
  1981年   4949篇
  1980年   4830篇
  1979年   8111篇
  1978年   6291篇
  1977年   5630篇
  1976年   5401篇
  1975年   5925篇
  1974年   6204篇
  1973年   6050篇
  1972年   5505篇
  1971年   4844篇
  1970年   4262篇
  1969年   3951篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
991.
992.
993.
Current issues in fish welfare   总被引:11,自引:0,他引:11  
Human beings may affect the welfare of fish through fisheries, aquaculture and a number of other activities. There is no agreement on just how to weigh the concern for welfare of fish against the human interests involved, but ethical frameworks exist that suggest how this might be approached. Different definitions of animal welfare focus on an animal's condition, on its subjective experience of that condition and/or on whether it can lead a natural life. These provide different, legitimate, perspectives, but the approach taken in this paper is to focus on welfare as the absence of suffering. An unresolved and controversial issue in discussions about animal welfare is whether non‐human animals exposed to adverse experiences such as physical injury or confinement experience what humans would call suffering. The neocortex, which in humans is an important part of the neural mechanism that generates the subjective experience of suffering, is lacking in fish and non‐mammalian animals, and it has been argued that its absence in fish indicates that fish cannot suffer. A strong alternative view, however, is that complex animals with sophisticated behaviour, such as fish, probably have the capacity for suffering, though this may be different in degree and kind from the human experience of this state. Recent empirical studies support this view and show that painful stimuli are, at least, strongly aversive to fish. Consequently, injury or experience of other harmful conditions is a cause for concern in terms of welfare of individual fish. There is also growing evidence that fish can experience fear‐like states and that they avoid situations in which they have experienced adverse conditions. Human activities that potentially compromise fish welfare include anthropogenic changes to the environment, commercial fisheries, recreational angling, aquaculture, ornamental fish keeping and scientific research. The resulting harm to fish welfare is a cost that must be minimized and weighed against the benefits of the activity concerned. Wild fish naturally experience a variety of adverse conditions, from attack by predators or conspecifics to starvation or exposure to poor environmental conditions. This does not make it acceptable for humans to impose such conditions on fish, but it does suggest that fish will have mechanisms to cope with these conditions and reminds us that pain responses are in some cases adaptive (for example, suppressing feeding when injured). In common with all vertebrates, fish respond to environmental challenges with a series of adaptive neuro‐endocrine adjustments that are collectively termed the stress response. These in turn induce reversible metabolic and behavioural changes that make the fish better able to overcome or avoid the challenge and are undoubtedly beneficial, in the short‐term at least. In contrast, prolonged activation of the stress response is damaging and leads to immuno‐suppression, reduced growth and reproductive dysfunction. Indicators associated with the response to chronic stress (physiological endpoints, disease status and behaviour) provide a potential source of information on the welfare status of a fish. The most reliable assessment of well‐being will be obtained by examining a range of informative measures and statistical techniques are available that enable several such measures to be combined objectively. A growing body of evidence tells us that many human activities can harm fish welfare, but that the effects depend on the species and life‐history stage concerned and are also context‐dependent. For example, in aquaculture, adverse effects related to stocking density may be eliminated if good water quality is maintained. At low densities, bad water quality may be less likely to arise whereas social interactions may cause greater welfare problems. A number of key differences between fish and birds and mammals have important implications for their welfare. Fish do not need to fuel a high body temperature, so the effects of food deprivation on welfare are not so marked. For species that live naturally in large shoals, low rather than high densities may be harmful. On the other hand, fish are in intimate contact with their environment through the huge surface area of their gills, so they are vulnerable to poor water quality and water borne pollutants. Extrapolation between taxa is dangerous and general frameworks for ensuring welfare in other vertebrate animals need to be modified before they can be usefully applied to fish. The scientific study of fish welfare is at an early stage compared with work on other vertebrates and a great deal of what we need to know is yet to be discovered. It is clearly the case that fish, though different from birds and mammals, however, are sophisticated animals, far removed from unfeeling creatures with a 15 s memory of popular misconception. A heightened appreciation of these points in those who exploit fish and in those who seek to protect them would go a long way towards improving fish welfare.  相似文献   
994.
Delusions are currently characterised as false beliefs produced by incorrect inference about external reality (DSM IV). This inferential conception has proved hard to link to explanations pitched at the level of neurobiology and neuroanatomy. This paper provides that link via a neurocomputational theory, based on evolutionary considerations, of the role of the prefrontal cortex in regulating offline cognition. When pathologically neuromodulated the prefrontal cortex produces hypersalient experiences which monopolise offline cognition. The result is characteristic psychotic experiences and patterns of thought. This bottom-up account uses neural network theory to integrate recent theories of the role of dopamine in delusion with the insights of inferential accounts. It also provides a general model for evolutionary psychiatry which avoids theoretical problems imported from evolutionary psychology.  相似文献   
995.
The cortical response in Xenopus laevis ova   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
A dependence on extracellular calcium has been demonstrated for fertilization and the cortical response to pricking in Xenopus ova. Neither event occurred in calcium-free solutions or in the presence of divalent cation chelating agents. The calcium-sensitive phase of the cortical response to pricking in dejellied eggs was restricted to the 5–10 sec immediately following the activation stimulus; the initial phase of activation was not calcium dependent. In contrast, the cortical response in dejellied Xenopus ova exposed to the chemical activating agents, urethan or methyl urethan, was independent of extracellular calcium. Experimental evidence was presented for the involvement of a direct, nonpropagated cortical reaction in response to urethan stimulation as opposed to a propagated reaction in response to pricking. A cortical response in dejellied eggs was not induced spontaneously by high concentrations of potassium, and the prick response was unaffected by inhibitors of energy transfer processes. Molecular mechanisms operative in the initiation and propagation of the cortical response in animal eggs have been discussed.  相似文献   
996.
997.
998.
Cationic amphiphile-mediated delivery of plasmid DNA is the non-viral gene transfer method most often used. In the present work, we considered a new cysteine-detergent, ornithinyl-cysteinyl-tetradecylamide (C(14)-CO), able to convert itself, via oxidative dimerization, into a cationic cystine-lipid. By using fluorescence techniques, we first characterized the structure of complexes of plasmid DNA with C(14)-CO molecules either kept as monomers, or oxidized into dimers. Both forms are able to condense DNA, with the formation of hydrophobic micelle-like domains along the DNA chain. Domains with a larger molecular order were obtained with dimeric C(14)-CO/DNA complexes. In a second step, the interactions of these complexes with lipid vesicles considered as membrane models were investigated. In the presence of vesicles, we observed a decondensation of the DNA involved in complexes obtained with C(14)-CO monomers. With anionic vesicles, the DNA is released into the bulk solution, while with neutral vesicles, it remains bound to the vesicles via electrostatic interactions with inserted C(14)-CO molecules. In sharp contrast, the complexes with C(14)-CO dimers are unaffected by the addition of either neutral or anionic vesicles and show no interaction with them. These results may partly explain the low transfection efficiency of these complexes at the +/-charge ratios used in this study.  相似文献   
999.
We describe a new tracer method to measure unidirectional fluxes of Li+, despite the lack of any utilizable radioisotope of lithium. This method uses the purified stable isotopes, 6Li and 7Li, detected with an ion-probe microanalyser. The accuracy is comparable to that obtained for other ions (e.g., Na+) with radiotracers.The method has been applied to frog skin with both faces bathed in a 20% lithium/80% sodium medium. Sodium and lithium unidirectional fluxes have been measured simultaneously. The results are consistent with lithium being actively pumped, the outflux of lithium being, however, much larger than that of sodium.  相似文献   
1000.
We have earlier demonstrated that a mixed population of immunologically specific killer cells, including cytotoxic T lymphocytes, non-T (“B”) lymphocytes and monocytes, infiltrate “sponge matrix” allografts at the peak of rejection on Day 8 after transplantation. We have now performed a sequential study covering both early and late stages of the rejection response. We demonstrate that the early infiltrating killer cells are sensitive to anti-Ø and anti-T cell serum plus complement treatment but the late killer cells are not. This finding indicates that the first cytotoxic host cells infiltrating the allograft are predominantly T lymphocytes, whereas as the rejection process proceeds also cytotoxic non-T (“B”) lymphocytes and monocytes are recruited to the site of inflammation.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号