首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 125 毫秒
1.
When observers view a grid of mid-gray lines superimposed on a black background, they report seeing illusory dark gray smudges at the grid intersections, an effect known as the Hermann grid illusion. The strength of the illusion is often measured using the cancellation technique: A white disk is placed over one of these intersections and the luminance of the disk is reduced until the disk disappears. Its luminance at this point, i.e., the disk's detection threshold, is taken to be a measure of the strength of the illusion. Our experiments showed that some distortions of the Hermann grid, which were sufficient to completely disrupt the illusion, did not reduce the disk's detection threshold. This showed that the cancellation technique is not a valid method for measuring the strength of the Hermann grid illusion. Those studies that attempted to use this technique inadvertently studied a different effect known as the blanking phenomenon. We conclude by presenting an explanation for the latter effect.  相似文献   

2.
Perceptual multistability is a phenomenon in which alternate interpretations of a fixed stimulus are perceived intermittently. Although correlates between activity in specific cortical areas and perception have been found, the complex patterns of activity and the underlying mechanisms that gate multistable perception are little understood. Here, we present a neural field competition model in which competing states are represented in a continuous feature space. Bifurcation analysis is used to describe the different types of complex spatio-temporal dynamics produced by the model in terms of several parameters and for different inputs. The dynamics of the model was then compared to human perception investigated psychophysically during long presentations of an ambiguous, multistable motion pattern known as the barberpole illusion. In order to do this, the model is operated in a parameter range where known physiological response properties are reproduced whilst also working close to bifurcation. The model accounts for characteristic behaviour from the psychophysical experiments in terms of the type of switching observed and changes in the rate of switching with respect to contrast. In this way, the modelling study sheds light on the underlying mechanisms that drive perceptual switching in different contrast regimes. The general approach presented is applicable to a broad range of perceptual competition problems in which spatial interactions play a role.  相似文献   

3.
郭昆  李朝义 《生理学报》1993,45(6):543-551
用定量的心理物理测量方法,研究了错觉图形组成成分间的亮度对比和颜色对比方位错觉、长度觉及面积错觉幅度的影响。测试结果表明:与通常的错觉效应相比,当错觉图形组成成分间存在亮度对比或颜色对比(等亮度)时,受试者的错觉程度明显降低;其中,当存在颜色对比时,方位错觉的下降幅度更为显著,达到69.3%。此外还观察到,在单纯亮度对比条件下,只需1.8%和5.3%的低对比度即可分别产生轮廓和边缘错觉;但在等亮度  相似文献   

4.
We have utilized a phase-sensitive effect, the square-wave illusion, to investigate the presence and tuning of phase-sensitive mechanisms. Subjects viewed a 1 c/deg triangular-wave grating and reported the presence or absence of the illusion, before and after adaptation to square-wave gratings of various spatial frequencies. Illusion strength declined with adaptation and was a function of spatial frequency. This function may reflect the spatial frequency tuning of a 'phase channel'. We have also measured by adaptation a contrast sensitivity function, maximally sensitive to a frequency of 1 c/deg. When normalized to the same maximal depression from baseline sensitivity, the phase channel and spatial-frequency channel shared the same low frequency tuning, but the phase channel was broader, with extended sensitivity to higher frequencies. These results can be understood if the phase channel is constructed from combinations of phase sensitive spatial-frequency channels.  相似文献   

5.
In the present communication, we have developed a computational model related to the conception of positional coding via centers-of-masses (centroids) of the objects’ luminance distributions. The model predictions have been tested by the results of our psychophysical study of geometrical illusion of extent evoked by a modified Brentano figure consisting of three separate spots clusters. In experiments, the centroids of the clusters were manipulated by varying the positions of additional non-target spots flanking the stimulus terminators. A good correspondence between the model predictions and the illusion magnitude changes provided convincing evidences in favor of “centroid” explanation of origin of the illusion investigated.  相似文献   

6.
Roncato S  Casco C 《Spatial Vision》2006,19(6):581-603
We previously showed that interpolation between vertically misaligned luminance edges of same polarity of contrast is preferred to that between co-linear edges of opposite polarity of contrast, although it results in illusory tilt (Roncato and Casco, 2003). We here analyze the spatial conditions that produce this illusory binding of vertically misaligned edges of light and dark tiles, alternated in a row, and in counterphase with those in the rows above and below. We find that, independently of scale and number of tiles in a row, the illusion is perceived when the vertical misalignment of more than three tiles is smaller than or equal to 9' and the horizontal separation between co-linear edges is smaller than or equal to 13'. These short distances suggest that the underlying mechanism is local. Both our phenomenological and psychophysical results support the notion of a local short-range association field, selective to contrast polarity, which produces a binding solution different from that of a phase-independent long-range mechanism. We suggest that the occurrence of the illusion at local-level is a result of the activation, within a local short-range association field, of units with orientation different from that stimulated by the physical edges. These units are not inhibited when they are close, iso-oriented and co-linear, and the misaligned edges from which they propagate have the same contrast polarity. We found that horizontal and vertical spatial limits for the interpolation covary but not such that their ratio is fixed, indicating that the two edges can be connected by projections having a relatively wide range of orientations.  相似文献   

7.
In psychophysical experiments, 209 high school students with normal vision, 8 to 16 years old, were examined to study the perception of visual image sizes. Observers assessed the length of linear arrow-like figures (the Müller-Lyer illusion) or the length of single lines without arrow-like ends. Distortion of line size perception by 17–21% was obtained in the Müller-Lyer illusion independent of the age of observers. Distortion of size perception was absent in the case of estimation of the length of single line segments. The size differentiation thresholds gradually decreased in both cases with increasing age of observers and were correlated with the acuity of vision. For single lines, they were, on average, 1.3 times lower than the thresholds in the Müller-Lyer illusion. The probable mechanisms of the Müller-Lyer illusion are discussed. The experimental results demonstrate stability of illusion for observers aged 8–16 years, which may be connected with preferential contribution of the lower levels of the visual system to the appearance of the illusion.  相似文献   

8.
Geier J  Hudák M 《PloS one》2011,6(10):e26062
The Chevreul illusion is a well-known 19th century brightness illusion, comprising adjacent homogeneous grey bands of different luminance, which are perceived as inhomogeneous. It is generally explained by lateral inhibition, according to which brighter areas projected to the retina inhibit the sensitivity of neighbouring retinal areas. Lateral inhibition has been considered the foundation-stone of early vision for a century, upon which several computational models of brightness perception are built. One of the last strongholds of lateral inhibition is the Chevreul illusion, which is often illustrated even in current textbooks. Here we prove that lateral inhibition is insufficient to explain the Chevreul illusion. For this aim, we placed the Chevreul staircase in a luminance ramp background, which noticeably changed the illusion. In our psychophysical experiments, all 23 observers reported a strong illusion, when the direction of the ramp was identical to that of the staircase, and all reported homogeneous steps (no illusion) when its direction was the opposite. When the background of the staircase was uniform, 14 saw the illusion, and 9 saw no illusion. To see whether the change of the entire background area or that of the staircase boundary edges were more important, we placed another ramp around the staircase, whose direction was opposite to that of the original, larger ramp. The result is that though the inner ramp is rather narrow (mean = 0.51 deg, SD = 0.48 deg, N = 23), it still dominates perception. Since all conditions of the lateral inhibition account were untouched within the staircase, lateral inhibition fails to model these perceptual changes. Area ratios seem insignificant; the role of boundary edges seems crucial. We suggest that long range interactions between boundary edges and areas enclosed by them, such that diffusion-based models describe, provide a much more plausible account for these brightness phenomena, and local models are insufficient.  相似文献   

9.
To test the hypothesis that Hermann grid illusory lines are most clear when the grid is presented at a 45 deg, each of 20 participants underwent 10 trials in each of two conditions ('make the lines least clear' and 'make the lines most clear') which were run using a method of adjustment. A matched-pairs t-test applied to the means of the two conditions was significant beyond the 0.01 level. One-group t-tests supported the claim that the illusory lines are least clear for a vertical/horizontal orientation of the grid and most clear when it is rotated 45 deg. The results substantiate, in a systematic manner, 70 years of claims about the effects of grid orientation on illusory line clarity.  相似文献   

10.
The sequential analysis of information in a coarse-to-fine manner is a fundamental mode of processing in the visual pathway. Spatial frequency (SF) tuning, arguably the most fundamental feature of spatial vision, provides particular intuition within the coarse-to-fine framework: low spatial frequencies convey global information about an image (e.g., general orientation), while high spatial frequencies carry more detailed information (e.g., edges). In this paper, we study the development of cortical spatial frequency tuning. As feedforward input from the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) has been shown to have significant influence on cortical coarse-to-fine processing, we present a firing-rate based thalamocortical model which includes both feedforward and feedback components. We analyze the relationship between various model parameters (including cortical feedback strength) and responses. We confirm the importance of the antagonistic relationship between the center and surround responses in thalamic relay cell receptive fields (RFs), and further characterize how specific structural LGN RF parameters affect cortical coarse-to-fine processing. Our results also indicate that the effect of cortical feedback on spatial frequency tuning is age-dependent: in particular, cortical feedback more strongly affects coarse-to-fine processing in kittens than in adults. We use our results to propose an experimentally testable hypothesis for the function of the extensive feedback in the corticothalamic circuit.  相似文献   

11.
Within a wide class of multichannel models of the visual system it is suggested that spatial distributions of luminance are processed by the independent activation of grating detectors, or spatial frequency channels. Probability summation is often described in terms of Quick's nonlinear pooling model [Quick RF (1974) Kybernetik 16:65–67]. Using this model, we find evidence for the existence of different kinds of nonlinear summation at threshold; for compound gratings with well-separated spatial frequency components, the threshold functions indicate nonlinear summation which is not compatible with probability summation, while for line patterns well separated in the spatial domain the probability summation rule proves compatible with the data. Received: 24 June 1998 / Accepted in revised form: 16 March 1999  相似文献   

12.
Although the human visual system can accurately estimate the reflectance (or lightness) of surfaces under enormous variations in illumination, two equiluminant grey regions can be induced to appear quite different simply by placing a light-dark luminance transition between them. This illusion, the Craik-Cornsweet-O'Brien (CCOB) effect, has been taken as evidence for a low-level 'filling-in' mechanism subserving lightness perception. Here, we present evidence that the mechanism responsible for the CCOB effect operates not via propagation of a neural signal across space but by amplification of the low spatial frequency (SF) structure of the image. We develop a simple computational model that relies on the statistics of natural scenes actively to reconstruct the image that is most likely to have caused an observed series of responses across SF channels. This principle is tested psychophysically by deriving classification images (CIs) for subjects' discrimination of the contrast polarity of CCOB stimuli masked with noise. CIs resemble 'filled-in' stimuli; i.e. observers rely on portions of the stimuli that contain no information per se but that correspond closely to the reported perceptual completion. As predicted by the model, the filling-in process is contingent on the presence of appropriate low SF structure.  相似文献   

13.
Contrast adaptation and contrast masking in human vision.   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
After a preliminary study of visual evoked potentials (VEPS) to a test grating seen in the presence of masks at different orientations, psychophysical data are presented showing the effects of adaptation and of masking on thresholds for detecting the same test grating. The test is a vertical grating of spatial frequency 2 cycles per degree; adapting and masking gratings differ from the test either in orientation or in spatial frequency. The effects of adaptation and masking are explained by a single mechanism model that assumes: (i) adaptation and masking both alter the contrast response (or transducer) function of the mechanism that detects the test; (ii) masks, but not adaptors, stimulate the mechanism that detects the test; and (iii) a test is detectable when it raises response level by a constant amount. The model incorporates two distinct tuning functions, a broad adaptive contrast function and a narrow effective contrast function. It accounts adequately for all the data, including the location and size of the facilitative dip found in some masking functions, the constant slopes of the threshold elevation segments of adaptation functions and the varying slopes of masking functions. It also predicts the sometimes surprising joint effects of adaptation followed by masking and of two masks operating simultaneously.  相似文献   

14.
图形简单几何特征的分辨及其相关错觉的定量研究   总被引:2,自引:1,他引:1  
李朝义  郭昆 《生理学报》1993,45(5):486-497
本实验用定量的心理物理测量方法,研究了在亮度对比与等亮度颜色对比的条件下,受试者分辨图形简单几何特征的能力以及与这些图形特征有关的错觉效应(Zoellner错觉、Muller-Lyer错觉、Ponzo错觉和Delboeuf错觉)。实验结果表明:受试者在亮度对比与颜色对比的条件下对线段平行度,线段长度与图形面积的分辨能力相同,与这些几何特征有关的错觉幅度也没有显著差异。但是,在等亮度颜色对比条件下,  相似文献   

15.
Visual channels are defined psychophysically; stimuli that interact share information in the same channel, and those that do not interact are processed in different channels. Channels are often investigated by means of adaptation to one stimulus, testing contrast threshold elevation with one (or more) others. Much recent work has tested the tuning of channels for orientation and spatial frequency, using simple line gratings. This study examined the pattern-specificity of such adaptation, testing the hypothesis that the fundamental operators of the Lie Transformation Group Theory of Neuropsychology (LTG/NP) define psychophysical channels. In Experiment I the three basic pattern pairs of LTG/NP were used as adaptation and test stimuli in a conventional contrast threshold-elevation experiment. Threshold elevation was pattern-specific, thus supporting the hypothesis. In subsequent experiments various 'fractured' patterns, and patterns generated by combinations of Lie operators were used both for adaptation and test. The results were mixed; some supported the original hypothesis, but many did not. Relations between local contour orientations in adaptation and test patterns could explain some results, but not all. The hypothesis that adaptation occurs to the oriented spatial-frequency components of the test patterns, on the other hand, gave a good fit to the data. It is concluded that there is pattern-specificity in contrast threshold elevation, but it is a form of specificity that can be explained without recourse to a model of geometrical pattern processing, at least for the simple patterns used here.  相似文献   

16.
This paper reports the research and structuring of a classification system for the effects of psychophysical variables on the color attributes. A basic role of color science is to psychophysically specify color appearance. An early stage is to specify the effects of the psychophysical variables (as singles, pairs, etc) on the color attributes (as singles, pairs, etc), for example to model color appearance. Current data on effects are often scarce or conflicting. Few effects are well understood, and the practice of naming effects after their discoverer(s) is inadequate and can be confusing. The number and types of possible effects have never been systematically analyzed and categorized. We propose a simple and rigorous system of classification including nomenclature. The total range of effects is computed from the possible combinations of three psychophysical variables (luminance, dominant wavelength, purity) and six color attributes (lightness, brightness, hue, chroma, colorfulness, saturation) in all modes of appearance. Omitting those effects that are normally impossible to perceive at any one time (such as four- or five-dimensional colors), the total number perceivable is 161 types of effects for all modes of appearance. The type of effect is named after the psychophysical stimulus (or stimuli) and the relevant color attribute(s), e.g., Luminance-on-hue effect (traditionally known as Bezold-Brucke effect). Each type of effect may include slightly different effects with infinite variations depending on experimental parameters.  相似文献   

17.
Along with physical luminance, the perceived brightness is known to depend on the spatial structure of the stimulus. Often it is assumed that neural computation of the brightness is based on the analysis of luminance borders of the stimulus. However, this has not been tested directly. We introduce a new variant of the psychophysical reverse-correlation or classification image method to estimate and localize the physical features of the stimuli which correlate with the perceived brightness, using a brightness-matching task. We derive classification images for the illusory Craik-O''Brien-Cornsweet stimulus and a “real” uniform step stimulus. For both stimuli, classification images reveal a positive peak at the stimulus border, along with a negative peak at the background, but are flat at the center of the stimulus, suggesting that brightness is determined solely by the border information. Features in the perceptually completed area in the Craik-O''Brien-Cornsweet do not contribute to its brightness, nor could we see low-frequency boosting, which has been offered as an explanation for the illusion. Tuning of the classification image profiles changes remarkably little with stimulus size. This supports the idea that only certain spatial scales are used for computing the brightness of a surface.  相似文献   

18.
Binocular responses have been recorded extra-cellularly at 58 sites in the barn owl's (Tyto alba) visual Wulst. Neurons showed disparity tuning to stimulation with moving bars, moving sinewave gratings and a moving visual-noise stimulus. Responses to sinewave gratings as a function of disparity were cyclic, with the period of a cycle of the response being correlated to one cycle of the stimulus. Cyclic responses were also found when bars or noise were used as a stimulus, but, especially in response to visual noise, one response peak, the main peak, was different from the other peaks, the sidepeaks: usually, the main peak was either higher or narrower or both higher and narrower than the sidepeaks. When the responses to different spatial frequencies were compared, response maxima coincided at the main peak, but not at the other peaks. In analogy to auditory physiology the disparity at which the frequency-independent peak occurs is termed characteristic disparity. Spatial-frequency tuning revealed broad tuning, ranging from 1 to more than 3 octaves at 50% of the maximal response. Disparity tuning was broad at the onset of the response and sharpened later. The data are discussed within the framework of a model for the neural representation of visual disparity that was derived from a model proposed earlier for the representation of interaural time difference, the main cue for encoding sound-source azimuths in the barn owl.Abbreviations ITD interaural time difference - CD characteristic delay - RF receptive field  相似文献   

19.
We quantified texture segregation by measuring psychophysically the percentage correct detection scores for each of a set of 10 texture-defined (TD) letters using the temporal two-alternative forced choice method, and at the same time quantified spatial discrimination of the TD form of measuring psychophysically the percentage correct letter recognition scores for the 10 letters. Ten levels of task difficulty were created by adding noise dots to the texture patterns. The resulting psychophysical data were used to test and compare models of the detection and recognition of texture-defined letters. Each model comprised a sequence of physiologically plausible stages in early visual processing. Each had the same first, second and third stages, namely linear orientation-tuned spatial filters followed by rectification and smoothing. Model 1 had only one non-linear stage. Model 2 had two non-linear stages. In model 2 the second non-linear stage was cross-orientation inhibition. This second non-linear stage enhanced the texture borders by, in effect, comparing textures at different locations in the texture pattern. In both models, the last stage modelled either letter detection or letter recognition. Letter recognition was modelled as follows. We passed a given letter stimulus through the first several stages of a model and, in 10 separate calculations, cross-correlated the output with a template of each of the 10 letters. From these 10 correlations we obtained a predicted percentage correct letter recognition score for the given letter stimulus. The predicted recognition scores closely agreed with the experimental data at all 10 levels of task difficulty for model 2, but not for model 1. We conclude that a borderenhancing algorithm is necessary to model letter recognition. The letter-detection algorithm modelled detection of part of a letter (a single letter stroke) in terms of the signal-to-noise ratio of a letter-segment detector. The predicted letter detection scores fitted the data closely for both models.  相似文献   

20.
感受野是视觉系统信息处理的基本结构和功能单元。X、Y细胞是两类主要的视网膜神经节细胞。生理实验发现,在经典感受野之外还存在一个大范围的在周边去抑制区。文中采用周边去抑制区对经典外周的去抑制非线性使用方式,建立一个二维的与实验结果联系紧密的X、Y细胞统一的复合感受野模型。该模型不仅能模拟X细胞的null-test反应和Y细胞的on-off反应,还模拟了Y细胞在低空频刺激时的信频反应、圆面积空间的倍频  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号