首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 46 毫秒
1.
KT Potkin  WE Bunney 《PloS one》2012,7(8):e42191
Sleep plays an important role in the consolidation of memory. This has been most clearly shown in adults for procedural memory (i.e. skills and procedures) and declarative memory (e.g. recall of facts). The effects of sleep and memory are relatively unstudied in adolescents. Declarative memory is important in school performance and consequent social functioning in adolescents. This is the first study to specifically examine the effects of normal sleep on auditory declarative memory in an early adolescent sample. Given that the majority of adolescents do not obtain the recommended amount of sleep, it is critical to study the cognitive effects of normal sleep. Forty male and female normal, healthy adolescents between the ages of ten and fourteen years old were randomly assigned to sleep and no sleep conditions. Subjects were trained on a paired-associate declarative memory task and a control working memory task at 9am, and tested at night (12 hours later) without sleep. The same number of subjects was trained at 9pm and tested 9am following sleep. An increase of 20.6% in declarative memory, as measured by the number correct in a paired-associate test, following sleep was observed compared to the group which was tested at the same time interval without sleep (p<0.03). The performance on the control working memory task that involved encoding and memoranda manipulation was not affected by time of day or relationship to sleep. Declarative memory is significantly improved by sleep in a sample of normal adolescents.  相似文献   

2.
In adults and seven- to eight-year-old children, event-related potentials (ERPs) were analyzed during quiet observation and detailed paired comparison of visual stimuli. In both age groups, we showed the differences in the initial stages (component N1) of sensory analysis in these situations. In adults, an increase in the negativity during the initial stages of analysis was observed in the caudal and central areas of the cortex during presentation of standard and test stimuli. In the frontal areas of the cortex, an increase in the negative potential was observed only in ERPs induced by the test stimulus. In children, an increase in the negativity at the initial phases of analysis of stimuli in the situation of working memory, as compared to quiet observation, was confined to the caudal areas of the cortex. Differential curves that characterize analysis of standard and test stimuli showed age-related differences in the initial and late phases of information processing under the conditions of working memory. In adults, the differential curves that characterize analysis of the standard stimulus were represented by negative phases, and the curves related to the test stimulus, by positive phases. In children, late phases of analysis of the standard and test stimuli had smaller differences as compared to adults: the late positive wave was predominant in the responses to both standard and test stimulus in the caudal areas of the cortex. In the frontal areas, there was no considerable increase in the amplitude of the late positive wave in response to the test stimulus. This fact, together with the absence of enhancement of initial negativity in the frontal areas, which reflects analysis of the test stimulus, indicates that the prefrontal cortex plays a smaller role in the comparison of the memory trace with the current information in seven- to eight-year-old children. The data obtained suggest that the central executive of working memory is not sufficiently mature in children aged seven to eight years.  相似文献   

3.
AIM: To describe the effects of chronological age and biological age (pubertal development) on serum lipid and lipoprotein levels, body mass index (BMI) and waist circumference in Spanish adolescents. METHODS: A representative Spanish sample of 526 adolescents (254 males and 272 females), were studied. Total cholesterol (TC), high density lipoprotein cholesterol (HDLc), triglycerides, apolipoprotein A1 and B, and lipoprotein(a) were measured, and low density lipoprotein cholesterol (LDLc) was calculated. Additional measurements included BMI and waist circumference. Adolescents were classified according to chronological age, and pubertal development (also age of menarche in females). RESULTS: In males, serum TC levels were lower at late puberty in comparison with early puberty, and serum LDLc levels were lower at late puberty in comparison with mid and early puberty. Serum HDLc levels were lower at mid puberty in comparison with early and late puberty. Serum TC and LDLc levels were not different when analyzed according to chronological age. In females, HDLc levels were lower at late puberty in comparison with early and mid puberty, but no differences were found when HDLc and the other studied lipid and lipoprotein variables were analyzed according to chronological age, or age of menarche. All the observed differences persisted after adjusting for BMI and waist circumference. In female adolescents, both BMI and waist circumference were higher at late puberty in comparison with early and mid puberty, while in males, BMI and waist circumference were different when analyzed according to chronological age. CONCLUSION: The results suggest that the assessment of pubertal development may provide additional valuable information when interpreting lipid profile and body fat in adolescents.  相似文献   

4.
In 7–8 and 9–10-years old children, we studied event-related potentials (ERPs) during paired comparison of non-verbalizable visuospatial stimuli presented at an interval of 1.5–1.8 s. Age-related differences were found in the involvement of various cortical areas in the formation and retention of a short-term memory trace of the reference stimulus and during comparison of the short-term trace with the test stimulus presented. In both age groups, working memory was associated with an elevation of the amplitude of the sensory-specific N1 component in the visual cortical areas. Age-related differences in the processing of sensory-specific characteristics of a stimulus were the greatest in the ERPs to the test stimulus: at the age of 9–10, the N1 component amplitude was significantly increased in all caudal leads and, in the occipital and inferior temporal leads, this component was preceded by P1 component. At this age, we observed the early involvement of the inferior frontal cortex, which was not observed at the age of seven. The increase in positivity over that area was observed in the interval of 100–200 ms. Substantial differences between age groups were found in the late ERP component corresponding to cognitive processes. At the age of 7–8, the presentation of both the reference and test stimuli causes the increase in the amplitude of the slow positive complex (SPC) in the caudal liads with the maximum enhancement found in the interval of 300–800 ms in the parietal leads. At the age of 9–10, the SPC increase, much like in adults, was observed in ERP to the test stimulus only. At this age, adult-like specific changes in the late phases of ERPs were observed in the fronto-central regions at the different stages of working memory. They are the increases in the negative N400 wave in the ERP to the reference stimulus and the SPC to the test stimulus. These data show that, at the age of 9–10, the functional organization of working memory of the adult type is formed; however, the extent to which the frontal cortex, and its dorsal regions in particular, is involved into working memory processes does not meet yet a definitive level.  相似文献   

5.
Diurnal and seasonal rhythms of cortisol, testosterone, and DHEA were examined, as little is known about the relationship between these rhythmicities and pubertal development. Salivary samples were obtained from 60 boys and 60 girls at approximately 07∶45, 08∶00, 08∶30, 12∶00, 16∶50, and 21∶00 h. The participants' ages ranged from 8–14 yrs, and each participant was tested three times at six‐month intervals. The study was conducted at a General Clinical Research Center (GCRC) and at the homes of the participants. All hormones showed diurnal fluctuations. The acrophase (peak time) of cortisol occurred earlier than for testosterone or DHEA and showed a seasonal effect, with the acrophase occurring earlier in spring than in summer. The cortisol acrophase also occurred later in the day for boys than for girls during later puberty. Seasonal effects were found only for cortisol with higher concentrations in the spring and summer. Cortisol concentrations were relatively stable across pubertal maturation, but significantly lower concentrations were observed at pubertal stage 3 compared to the other stages. Morning cortisol levels were also higher in boys at pubertal stage 2. Testosterone concentrations were higher in boys at pubertal stages 3 and 4, and DHEA was lower at pubertal stage 1 than 3 and 4 for both boys and girls. For the total sample, there was a positive correlation between DHEA and testosterone during early puberty (stages 1–3) but not later puberty (stages 4–5). Awakening secretory activity correlated with daytime secretory activity for testosterone and DHEA, but not for cortisol. These data provide novel chronobiological information on cortisol, testosterone, and DHEA as it relates to sexual maturation and encourage further study on both normal and abnormal endocrine rhythms.  相似文献   

6.
Diurnal and seasonal rhythms of cortisol, testosterone, and DHEA were examined, as little is known about the relationship between these rhythmicities and pubertal development. Salivary samples were obtained from 60 boys and 60 girls at approximately 07:45, 08:00, 08:30, 12:00, 16:50, and 21:00 h. The participants' ages ranged from 8-14 yrs, and each participant was tested three times at six-month intervals. The study was conducted at a General Clinical Research Center (GCRC) and at the homes of the participants. All hormones showed diurnal fluctuations. The acrophase (peak time) of cortisol occurred earlier than for testosterone or DHEA and showed a seasonal effect, with the acrophase occurring earlier in spring than in summer. The cortisol acrophase also occurred later in the day for boys than for girls during later puberty. Seasonal effects were found only for cortisol with higher concentrations in the spring and summer. Cortisol concentrations were relatively stable across pubertal maturation, but significantly lower concentrations were observed at pubertal stage 3 compared to the other stages. Morning cortisol levels were also higher in boys at pubertal stage 2. Testosterone concentrations were higher in boys at pubertal stages 3 and 4, and DHEA was lower at pubertal stage 1 than 3 and 4 for both boys and girls. For the total sample, there was a positive correlation between DHEA and testosterone during early puberty (stages 1-3) but not later puberty (stages 4-5). Awakening secretory activity correlated with daytime secretory activity for testosterone and DHEA, but not for cortisol. These data provide novel chronobiological information on cortisol, testosterone, and DHEA as it relates to sexual maturation and encourage further study on both normal and abnormal endocrine rhythms.  相似文献   

7.
Puberty forms an important threshold between childhood and adulthood, but this subject has received little attention in bioarchaeology. The new application of clinical methods to assess pubertal stage in adolescent skeletal remains is explored, concentrating on the development of the mandibular canine, hamate, hand phalanges, iliac crest and distal radius. Initial results from the medieval cemetery of St. Peter's Church, Barton‐upon‐Humber, England suggest that application of these methods may provide insights into aspects of adolescent development. This analysis indicates that adolescents from this medieval site were entering the pubertal growth spurt at a similar age to their modern counterparts, but that the later stages of pubertal maturation were being significantly delayed, perhaps due to environmental stress. Continued testing and refinement of these methods on living adolescents is still necessary to improve our understanding of their significance and accuracy in predicting pubertal stages. Am J Phys Anthropol 151:302–310, 2013. © 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.  相似文献   

8.
Functional Organization of the Brain during the Operation of Working Memory   总被引:3,自引:1,他引:2  
Event-related potentials (ERPs) recorded from various cortical areas during matching of two consecutive pictures were analyzed. Reflecting the process of trace fixation, the ERP to the reference stimulus was characterized by an increase in components P150 and P300 in the occipital and temporo-parieto-occipital areas and components N300 and N400 in the precentral areas as compared with the ERP elicited by the warning stimulus. The ERP to the test stimulus, which reflected trace retrieval and matching with current information, was characterized by a generalized increase in the late positive complex in the interval 300–600 ms. Similarity and/or dissimilarity of the test and reference stimuli was reflected in the parameters of the ERP to the test stimulus. The results testify to the difference in functional and topographic organization of the brain cortex at the initial and late stages of operation of the working memory.  相似文献   

9.
The aim of the study was to investigate the relationship between liver transaminase levels and metabolic syndrome (MS) features in obese children and adolescents. A total of 132 children and adolescents (73 males and 59 females) aged 8–16, participated in the study. All were studied at the department of Paediatrics, University Hospital of Zaragoza (Spain). Inclusion criteria were the existence of obesity as defined by body mass index (BMI) according to Cole cut-off values (when BMI was higher than the age and sex specific equivalent to 30 kg/m2). The definition of metabolic syndrome was according to the International Diabetes Federation criteria. Weight (kg), height (cm), waist circumference (cm), blood pressure and BMI were measured. Laboratory determinations after overnight fasting included: transaminases (ALT, AST, GGT), fasting glucose, insulin, triglycerides and HDL-C. The MS was found in 21.6% of the obese children and adolescents and the prevalence was higher in males (25.9%) than in females (15.9%). Serum transaminases (ALT, AST and GGT) mean concentrations were higher in males than in females, and decreased during pubertal development. The obese children and adolescents with the MS did not show higher transaminases concentrations when compared with those without the MS. Some MS manifestations (mainly waist circumference) showed a correlation with ALT, although all transaminases values were normal according to adult references. Liver transaminases, a surrogate marker of NAFLD, did not show an early and consistent manifestation of abnormalities in the obese children and adolescents studied. In order to define the presence of the disease, it would be necessary to obtain aminotransferase reference standards for children and adolescents, considering pubertal stage and gender.  相似文献   

10.
Memory judgments can be based on accurate memory information or on decision bias (the tendency to report that an event is part of episodic memory when one is in fact unsure). Event related potentials (ERP) correlates are important research tools for elucidating the dynamics underlying memory judgments but so far have been established only for investigations of accurate old/new discrimination. To identify the ERP correlates of bias, and observe how these interact with ERP correlates of memory, we conducted three experiments that manipulated decision bias within participants via instructions during recognition memory tests while their ERPs were recorded. In Experiment 1, the bias manipulation was performed between blocks of trials (automatized bias) and compared to trial-by-trial shifts of bias in accord with an external cue (flexibly controlled bias). In Experiment 2, the bias manipulation was performed at two different levels of accurate old/new discrimination as the memory strength of old (studied) items was varied. In Experiment 3, the bias manipulation was added to another, bottom-up driven manipulation of bias induced via familiarity. In the first two Experiments, and in the low familiarity condition of Experiment 3, we found evidence of an early frontocentral ERP component at 320 ms poststimulus (the FN320) that was sensitive to the manipulation of bias via instruction, with more negative amplitudes indexing more liberal bias. By contrast, later during the trial (500–700 ms poststimulus), bias effects interacted with old/new effects across all three experiments. Results suggest that the decision criterion is typically activated early during recognition memory trials, and is integrated with retrieved memory signals and task-specific processing demands later during the trial. More generally, the findings demonstrate how ERPs can help to specify the dynamics of recognition memory processes under top-down and bottom-up controlled retrieval conditions.  相似文献   

11.
We have previously found two stages of amnesia evoked by disruption of memory reconsolidation with MK-801 (NMDA glutamate receptors antagonists) application in food aversion conditioned snails. Repeated conditioning restored the food aversion at early stage of amnesia development (<10 days), whereas repeated conditioning 10 days after MK-801 application did not restore the food aversion. In present work, amnesia was induced with MK-801/reminding 24 hours after food aversion conditioning, and antiamnestic effects of NMDA receptor glycine site agonist d-cycloserine were studied at early (3rd day) or late (12th day) stages of amnesia development. D-cycloserine injection and reminding restored memory only 3 days after amnesia induction whereas d-cycloserine injection without reminding was ineffective. D-cycloserine injection and reminding as well as repeated learning 12 days after amnesia induction were also ineffective in memory restoration. Thus, for the first time, it is revealed that NMDA receptor agonist d-cycloserine influences the memory restoration processes only at early but not the later stages of amnesia development.  相似文献   

12.
The assessment of age‐at‐death in non‐adult skeletal remains is under constant review. However, in many past societies an individual's physical maturation may have been more important in social terms than their exact age, particularly during the period of adolescence. In a recent article (Shapland and Lewis: Am J Phys Anthropol 151 (2013) 302–310) highlighted a set of dental and skeletal indicators that may be useful in mapping the progress of the pubertal growth spurt. This article presents a further skeletal indicator of adolescent development commonly used by modern clinicians: cervical vertebrae maturation (CVM). This method is applied to a collection of 594 adolescents from the medieval cemetery of St. Mary Spital, London. Analysis reveals a potential delay in ages of attainment of the later CVM stages compared with modern adolescents, presumably reflecting negative environmental conditions for growth and development. The data gathered on CVM is compared to other skeletal indicators of pubertal maturity and long bone growth from this site to ascertain the usefulness of this method on archaeological collections. Am J Phys Anthropol 153:144–153, 2014. © 2013 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.  相似文献   

13.
Event-related potentials (ERPs) were used to study working memory operations in young school-children. The type of information has proved to determine the specificity of encoding and retrieval processes in working memory. The formation of the trace and its comparison with new information for phonological stimuli requires involvement of the frontal cortical areas, whereas visual patterns are characterized by an enhanced late positive complex in the caudal areas.  相似文献   

14.
Objective: To investigate sexual dimorphism and race differences in fat distribution (android/gynoid) before and during puberty. Research Methods and Procedures: Fat distribution was measured by skinfold thickness and DXA in healthy African‐American, Asian, and white subjects (n = 920), divided into pre‐, early, and late pubertal groups. Results: Gynoid fat masses adjusted for covariates were lower in late pubertal compared with prepubertal boys, but were not consistently greater in late pubertal compared with prepubertal girls. Progression of sex‐specific fat distribution with increasing maturation was present in Asians only. Among African‐American and white subjects, early pubertal boys had greater gynoid fat mass compared with the prepubertal group, whereas early pubertal girls had less gynoid fat mass compared with the prepubertal group. Sexual dimorphism in fat distribution was present in all pubertal groups, except among whites at early puberty. Among girls, Asians had lower gynoid fat than whites and African Americans in all pubertal groups. Among boys, Asians had less gynoid fat by DXA in early puberty and late puberty. Discussion: Comparison among races demonstrated differences in sexual dimorphism and sex‐specific fat distribution with progression in pubertal group. However, in all race groups, the fat distribution of late pubertal boys was more “male” or “android” than prepubertal boys, but late pubertal girls did not differ consistently from prepubertal girls. These findings suggested that the greater sexual dimorphism of fat distribution in late puberty compared with prepuberty may be attributable to larger changes in boys with smaller changes in girls.  相似文献   

15.
Growth hormone (GH) was measured in 215 short children (147 males and 68 females, 123 prepubertal, 92 at early pubertal stages), comparing GH responses to classical pharmacologic stimulation tests and spontaneous GH secretion during sleep. GH secretion during sleep, but not GH responses to stimuli, was higher in early pubertal than in prepubertal subjects. The patients were classified into five groups, according to the agreement between GH responses to stimuli and GH secretion during sleep: group I, normal GH-secreting children; group II, completely GH-deficient; group III, partially GH-deficient; group IV, with normal secretion during sleep and low responses to stimuli; group V, with the reverse situation. 30% of the patients were in groups IV and V, both at prepubertal and early pubertal stages. 46 patients of groups II-V were treated with extracted human GH(hGH). The growth rate was enhanced in groups IV and V, to the same extent as in groups II and III. Four points can be concluded: (1) the rise of GH secretion during sleep is an early event at the onset of puberty; (2) the discrepancy between the GH responses to classical stimuli and GH secretion during sleep are of pathological significance; (3) disturbances of GH secretion might be diagnosed by measuring GH secretion during sleep rather than by using conventional stimulation tests; (4) a trial course of hGH treatment could be proposed in patients with both kinds of discrepancies between GH responses to stimuli and GH secretion during sleep.  相似文献   

16.
The variation in the level of angiotensin-converting enzyme in the testes of prepubertal, pubertal and adult cockerels were investigated. Maximum levels of angiotensin-converting enzyme were observed at 8 wk of age, followed by a progressive decrease (P / 0.01) until 14 wk of age. After this stage, however, a rapid growth rate of the testes and comb was noted. The concentration of angiotensin-converting enzyme in pubertal and adult birds remained at a significantly lower level than in the prepubertal birds. The level of activity of this enzyme was significantly but negatively correlated with age and with the weights of the testes and comb. The high level of this enzyme found in prepubertal cockerels suggests it may be more highly involved in the early rather than later stages of spermatogenesis.  相似文献   

17.
The relationship between noise and human performance is a crucial topic in ergonomic research. However, the brain dynamics of the emotional arousal effects of background noises are still unclear. The current study employed meaningless speech noises in the n-back working memory task to explore the changes of event-related potentials (ERPs) elicited by the noises with low arousal level vs. high arousal level. We found that the memory performance in low arousal condition were improved compared with the silent and the high arousal conditions; participants responded more quickly and had larger P2 and P3 amplitudes in low arousal condition while the performance and ERP components showed no significant difference between high arousal and silent conditions. These findings suggested that the emotional arousal dimension of background noises had a significant influence on human working memory performance, and that this effect was independent of the acoustic characteristics of noises (e.g., intensity) and the meaning of speech materials. The current findings improve our understanding of background noise effects on human performance and lay the ground for the investigation of patients with attention deficits.  相似文献   

18.
Emotion effects on cognition have often been reported. However, only few studies investigated emotional effects on subsequent language processing, and in most cases these effects were induced by non-linguistic stimuli such as films, faces, or pictures. Here, we investigated how a paragraph of positive, negative, or neutral emotional valence affects the processing of a subsequent emotionally neutral sentence, which contained either semantic, syntactic, or no violation, respectively, by means of event-related brain potentials (ERPs). Behavioral data revealed strong effects of emotion; error rates and reaction times increased significantly in sentences preceded by a positive paragraph relative to negative and neutral ones. In ERPs, the N400 to semantic violations was not affected by emotion. In the syntactic experiment, however, clear emotion effects were observed on ERPs. The left anterior negativity (LAN) to syntactic violations, which was not visible in the neutral condition, was present in the negative and positive conditions. This is interpreted as reflecting modulatory effects of prior emotions on syntactic processing, which is discussed in the light of three alternative or complementary explanations based on emotion-induced cognitive styles, working memory, and arousal models. The present effects of emotion on the LAN are especially remarkable considering that syntactic processing has often been regarded as encapsulated and autonomous.  相似文献   

19.
Cognitive theory posits association by juxtaposition or by fusion. We employed the measurement of event-related brain potentials (ERPs) to a concept fusion task in order to explore memory encoding of these two types of associations between word pairs, followed by a memory test for original pair order. Encoding processes were isolated by subtracting fusion task ERPs corresponding to pairs later retrieved quickly from ERPs corresponding to pairs later retrieved slowly, separately for pairs fused successfully and unsuccessfully (i.e., juxtaposed). Analyses revealed that the encoding of these two types of associations yields different ERP voltage polarities, scalp topographies, and brain sources extending over the entire time course of processing.  相似文献   

20.
The potential role of transforming growth factor-alpha (TGF-alpha) as a mediator of cell-cell interactions in the growth and development of the testis was examined. Developing rat testes were collected, and preparations of mesenchymal-derived peritubular cells and epithelial-like Sertoli cells were isolated from prepubertal, midpubertal, and late pubertal rat testes. The developmental expression of TGF-alpha and its receptor, the epidermal growth factor receptor (EGFR), in whole testis and isolated cell types was determined using a nuclease protection assay. TGF-alpha and EGFR gene expression were predominant early in testis development and decreased during pubertal development. TGF-alpha expression was greatest in prepubertal peritubular cells. Sertoli cell TGF-alpha expression remained relatively constant during development, with a slight decline at the later pubertal stages. EGFR gene expression was predominant in peritublar cells throughout development. A low level of EGFR expression was detected in Sertoli cells. Scatchard analysis confirmed the presence of high affinity receptors on peritubular cells; however, no functional receptors were detected on Sertoli cells from any stage of development examined. Interestingly, low-level EGFR gene expression was also detected in pachytene spermatocytes and round spermatids. TGF-alpha was found to stimulate [3H] thymidine incorporation into DNA and increase cellular proliferation of peritubular cells from each developmental stage, while having no effect on Sertoli cells. The in vivo physiological significance of TGF-alpha was evaluated in a line of transgenic mice which overexpress TGF-alpha in the mature testis. These transgenic animals had no abnormal testicular morphology or alterations in spermatogenesis. Observations demonstrate that gene expression of TGF-alpha and its receptor is high during early pubertal stages when somatic cell growth is predominant and low at late pubertal stages when somatic cell proliferation is reduced. TGF-alpha can act as an autocrine/paracrine mitogen for the mesenchymal-derived peritubular cell, while actions on the Sertoli cell population are not evident. The observation that spermatogenic cells express the EGFR gene, although the protein remains to be identified, implies that TGF-alpha may potentially mediate Sertoli-germinal cell interactions.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号