首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
相似文献
 共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 46 毫秒
1.
A. Vermeglio  J.M. Carrier 《BBA》1984,764(2):233-238
The inhibition of respiration by continuous or flashing light has been studied in intact cells of different species of photosynthetic bacteria. For Rhodopseudomonas palustris, Rhodopseudomonas sphaeroides and Rhodopseudomonas capsulata, the inhibition by short actinic flashes shows a remarkable periodicity of two: each flash induces an inhibition of respiration, but a stimulation is observed after an even number of flashes. On the other hand, no oscillation is observed for Rhodospirillum rubrum and Rhodopseudomonas viridis cells. These different behaviours are explained by a difference in the redox state of the secondary electron acceptor as shown by the effect of ortho-phenanthroline on the amperometric signal. Addition of uncouplers (carbonyl cyanide m-chlorophenylhydrazone) or of an ATPase inhibitor (tri-N-butyl tin), has little effect on the oscillatory pattern induced by flash excitation. However, inhibition of respiration by continuous light is suppressed in the presence of carbonyl cyanide m-chlorophenylhydrazone. In the presence of tri-N-butyl tin the steady-state level is reached more rapidly than in the control experiment for a given light intensity. These results are interpreted as evidence of two modes of light inhibition of respiration in photosynthetic bacteria. A first type of inhibition, clearly shown under flash excitation, is due to interaction between respiratory and photosynthetic chains at the level of electron carriers. After each flash, an electron is diverted from the respiratory chain to the photooxidized reaction center. Because of the gating mechanism at the level of the secondary acceptor, the respiration is stimulated after an even number of flashes. The second mode of inhibition prevails under continuous illumination. Under these conditions, the rate of respiration is controlled essentially by the photoinduced proton electrochemical gradient.  相似文献   

2.
The electrophysiology of extraocular photoreception in the myopsidsquid, Loligo forbesi Steenstrup 1856 has been examined. Extracellulargenerator potentials were evoked by white light flashes. Intracellularrecordings from extraocular photoreceptor cells in the parolfactorybodies of the squid demonstrated that they had resting potentialsaround –40 mV, and were depolarised by flashes of white,but not red light (>650 nm). The evoked depolarisation consistedof a transient component, followed by a steady plateau component.The amplitude of depolarisation increased with the logarithmof the light intensity and was maintained for the duration ofthe light stimulus. Action potentials were seen in some recordingsand these increased in frequency during light flash stimulation. (Received 11 February 1997; accepted 10 May 1997)  相似文献   

3.
Steering their swimming direction toward the light is crucial for the viability of Volvox colonies, the larger members of the volvocine algae. While it is known that this phototactic steering is achieved by a difference in behavior of the flagella on the illuminated and shaded sides, conflicting reports suggest that this asymmetry arises either from a change in beating direction or a change in beating frequency. Here, we report direct observations of the flagellar behavior of various Volvox species with different phyletic origin in response to light intensity changes and thereby resolve this controversy: Volvox barberi W. Shaw from the section Volvox sensu Nozaki (2003) changes the direction of the flagellar beating plane, while species encompassed in the group Eudorina (Volvox carteri F. Stein, Volvox aureus Ehrenb., and Volvox tertius Art. Mey.) decrease the flagellar beating frequency, sometimes down to flagellar arrest.  相似文献   

4.
Using high-speed microcinematography flagellar shock responses of a great number of Chlamydomonas cells, free-swimming as well as immobilized on micropipettes, were investigated in this study. Responses were elicited by flashes, by blue, red or white light steps or occurred “spontaneously”. A large variety of shock responses has been found, in part due to various kinds of flagellar deactivations. Typical courses of flagellar responses are described in detail. The major part of the analyzed responses consists of a transition back from undulatory beats, characteristic for shock responses, to the normal breaststroke beats, probably as a result of a decreasing Ca++ concentration at the axoneme. It is known that undulatory beats are triggered by a transient strong influx of Ca++ ions into the flagella. Responses are initiated simultaneously in the two flagella but are finished independently. Differences in cis (= next to the stigma) and trans (= far from it) flagella were observed but were not consistent. The origin of the deactivations during the shock responses is discussed, as well as an involvement of basal body-associated structures in flagellar beating and in the change between the two beating modes. The comparison of the two fundamentally different types of beating and a close study of transitional beats may convey insight into the complexity of flagellar beating in Chlamydomonas.  相似文献   

5.
E M Holland  H Harz  R Uhl    P Hegemann 《Biophysical journal》1997,73(3):1395-1401
Both phototactic and photophobic responses of Chlamydomonas are mediated by a visual system comprising a rhodopsin photoreceptor. Suction pipette recordings have revealed that flash stimulation causes calcium currents into the eyespot and the flagella. These photocurrents have been suggested to be the trigger for all behavioral light responses of the cell. But this has never been shown experimentally. Here we describe a detection technique that combines electrical and optical measurements from individual algae held in a suction pipette. Thus it is possible to record photocurrents and flagellar beating simultaneously and establish a direct link between the two. We demonstrate that in Chlamydomonas only the photoreceptor current in conjuction with a fast flagellar current constitutes the trigger for photophobic responses. Within the time of the action-potential-like flagellar current, the flagella switch from forward to backward swimming, which constitutes the beginning of the photoshock reaction. The switch is accompanied by a complex frequency change and beating pattern modulation. The results are interpreted in terms of a general model for phototransduction in green algae (Chlorophyceae).  相似文献   

6.
Light pulses were used to mimic dinoflagellate bioluminescence and test its effects on the swimming behavior of Acartia hudsonica (Pinhey). The horizontal swimming patterns of the copepod were tracked and described using a video-computer system. Single flashes of light of 60 ms duration, with a wavelength of peak emission of 475 nm and an intensity of 2 μE · m?2 · s?1 caused a “startle” response consisting of a short burst of high speed swimming. A series of these flashes repeated every 5 s resulted in higher average swimming speed, more swimming speed bursts, and straighter paths. These behavioral changes are similar to those previously found for A. hudsonica in the presence of bioluminescent dinoflagellates. The effects of altering the intensity, duration, and color of the simulated dinoflagellate flash were also tested. Our results support the hypothesis that dinoflagellate bioluminescence is a highly evolved adaptation for repelling nocturnal grazers.  相似文献   

7.
Discrete potential waves can be recorded from cells in the eye of Limulus both in darkness and in dim illumination. With constant illumination the frequency of these waves is linearly related to light intensity and the distribution of intervals between waves follows an exponential function. The latency of waves evoked by short flashes of light is usually long and variable and the number of waves evoked by a flash varies randomly, obeying approximately a Poisson distribution. The results of experiments with flashes of light have been compared with the predictions derived from the hypotheses that one, two, or three quanta of light are required for production of one wave. The agreement of the data with the theory can be considered acceptable for the "one quantum" hypothesis, is less satisfactory for the "two quanta" hypothesis, and is very poor for the "three quanta" hypothesis.  相似文献   

8.
We explored O2 flash yield in two cyanophytes, Anacystis nidulans and Agmenellum quadruplicatum. On a rate-measuring electrode, a single flash gave a contour of O2 evolution with a peak at about 10 ms which was maximum (100) for 680 nm background light. On 625 nm illumination the peak was smaller (62) but was followed by an increased tail of O2 attributed to enhancement of the background. After a period of darkness, repetitive flashes (5 Hz) gave a highly damped initial oscillation in individual flash yields which finally reached steady state at 94% of the yield for 680 nm illumination. When O2 of repetitive flashes was measured as an integrated flash yield the results was distinctive and similar to that for a continuous light 1 (680 nm). An apparent inhibition of respiration which persisted into the following dark period was taken as evidence for the Kok effect. With a concentration-measuring electrode, integrated flash yield vs. flash rate showed the same nonlinear behavior as O2 rate vs. intensity of light 1. We draw three conclusions about the two cyanophytes. (a) The plastoquinone pool is substantially reduced in darkness. (b) Because of a high ratio of reaction centers, reaction center 1 / reaction center 2, for the two photoreactions, saturating flashes behave as light 1. (c) Because repetitive flashes are light 1, they also give a Kok effect which must be guarded against in measurements designed to count reaction centers.  相似文献   

9.
Patterns of oxygen evolution in flashing light for the blue-green alga Anacystis nidulans are compared with those for broken spinach chloroplasts and whole cells of the green alga Chlorella pyrenoidosa. The oscillations of oxygen yield with flash number that occur in both Anacystis and Chlorella, display a greater degree of damping than do those of isolated spinach chloroplasts. The increase in damping results from a two- to threefold increase in the fraction (α) of reaction centers “missed” by a flash. The increase in α cannot be explained by non-saturating flash intensities or by the dark reduction of the oxidized intermediates formed by the flash. Anaerobic conditions markedly increase α in Anacystis and Chlorella but have no effect on α in broken spinach chloroplasts. The results signify that the mechanism of charge separation and water oxidation involved in all three organisms is the same, but that the pool of secondary electron acceptors between Photosystem II and Photosystem I is more reduced in the dark, in the algal cells, than in the isolated spinach chloroplasts.Oxygen evolution in flashing light for Anacystis and Chlorella show light saturation curves for the oxygen yield of the third flash (Y3) that differ markedly from those of the steady-state flashes (Ys). In experiments in which all flashes are uniformly attenuated, Y3 requires nearly twice as much light as Ys to reach half-saturation. Under these conditions Y3 has a sigmoidal dependence on intensity, while that of Ys is hyperbolic. These differences depend on the number of flashes attenuated. When any one of the first three flashes is attenuated, the variation of Y3 with intensity resembles that of Ys. When two of the first three flashes are attenuated, Y3 is intermediate in shape between the two extremes. A quantitative interpretation of these results based on the model of Kok et al. (Kok, B., Forbush, B. and McGloin, M. (1970) Photochem. Photobiol. 11, 457–475, and Forbush, B., Kok, B. and McGloin, M. P. (1971) Photochem. Photobiol. 14, 307–321) fits the experimental data.  相似文献   

10.
1. Photographic records of impulses from single ganglion cells in the cat's retina were made while the retina was stimulated by flashes occurring once a second. Ten flashes at each of several intensities near threshold were used. 2. For the purpose of statistical analysis, the number of impulses (x) falling within a critical period following each flash was used as an index of the response. Histograms of x were plotted and used to calculate rates of transfer of information by the ganglion cell for the case of an ideal experiment, the yes-no choice, in which flashes of intensity I and blanks are to be distinguished. 3. The information rate increased (a) with increasing stimulus intensity and (b) with the number of identical flashes or blanks presented successively in a block. The intensity chosen as threshold by the experimenter, who observed the impulses visually and aurally, corresponded to an average information rate for single flashes of 0.7 bit/flash, compared to the maximum possible rate of 1 bit/flash. A threshold intensity giving 0.4 or more bit/flash, if presented in blocks of six identical flashes, corresponded to 0.95 or more bit/block, or near certainty. Thus the calculation of information rates using the index x provides an estimate of threshold at least as sensitive as those obtained during an experiment, which were made only after observing the responses to five to ten flashes of the same intensity. 4. The index x has statistical properties similar to those of the "index of neural activity" used by Tanner and Swets (1954) in their statistical model of human vision, and represents a possible physical interpretation of their index. However, x gave values (0.5 to 1.5) of the parameter called the slope which were consistently smaller than their values (2.1 to 3.1).  相似文献   

11.
The response properties of jittery movement fibers (JMF) in the crayfish optic tract reacting to a non-moving temporally patterned light were analyzed. The JMFs usually show no response during the regular flickering of stationary light with a flash duration of less than 50 msec when the stimulus frequency is between 4 and 20 per second; however they do respond when the flickering stops if a certain number of flashes have been given. The response appears about 50 msec after the first missing flash, i.e., the latency of the response after the last flash of the train changed from 100 to 300 msec. Thus, the “off” response at the end of the flicker is entrained to the stimulus repetition interval and locked onto the time of the first missing flash. The response of a sustaining fiber to an identical stimulus has quite different features as illustrated in Fig. 2. Some of the fibers show responses to the beginning part of the flicker but not necessarily to each flash, and habituate after several flashes. When a single flash longer than 250 msec is given, the fiber shows an “off” response with about 50 msec latency, as it does to sustained light. Some fibers show a double burst of “off” discharge to single flashes; the first at 50 msec is followed after 120 msec by the second one. However, when the flash duration is between 250 and 50 msec, a single flash elicits little or no response. The latency of the “off” response is as much as 300 msec for short single flashes less than 50 msec. An “on” response to flashes of light is observed when the inter-stimulus interval is more than 5 sec. The responses to the beginning part of flicker train are not simply locked to the just preceding flash except the “on” response to the very first one, but they can be the long latency responses to the flash before that. This response is modified in latency by the succeeding flashes in flicker trains and becomes entrained to the missing flash. Four types of entrainment are classified on the basis of the change in latency from the missing flash with regard to the number of flashes in a train. In most cases, 10 flashes are sufficient to entrain the response to the first missing flash. Non-resposiveness, i.e., habituation, during a regular flicker, may be due to an active inhibitory process, initiated by each succeeding light pulse. The response to the missing flash, therefore results from a disinhibited modified response to the last flash. Some JMFs continue to respond to the flicker even after a considerable number of flashes but only when the repetition interval is about 120 msec corresponding well to the interval of the double burst “off” discharge, thus the JMF has a resonant frequency of about 8 Hz. The JMFs appear to be acting as an irregularity detector in temporal sequence.  相似文献   

12.
Slow fluorescence transients in Chlamydomonas reinhardi arise after transitions from high light intensities to low light or dark conditions. Characteristics of the newly described transient phenomena include: (a) A slow biphasic decrease in fluorescence yield occurs in the dark, followed by an even slower, hour long, increase in fluorescence. (b) A similar, but faster, fluorescence yield decrease and subsequent increase also occurs during low intensity illumination periods separating high light intervals, or after transitions from high intensity to low intensity light. (c) Short (several seconds) flashes of light given during a dark period have no effect on the dark fluorescence decay, regardless of the flash frequency. Such flash regimes accurately monitor the dark decline of the M2 level by tracing the parallel decay of flash-generated P2 (Kautsky) peaks. However, flashes during a low light illumination period do influence the decay kinetics. Frequent flashes allow decay similar to that occurring in dark, but less frequent flashes inhibit the decrease in fluorescence yield.  相似文献   

13.
Four experiments examined the ability of respondents to identify letters that were displayed on an LED array with flashes lasting little more than a microsecond. The first experiment displayed each letter with a single, simultaneous flash of all the dots forming the letter and established the relation of flash intensity to the probability of letter identification. The second experiment displayed the letters with multiple flashes at different frequencies to determine the probability that the sequence of flashes would be perceived as fused. The third experiment displayed the letters at a frequency that was above the flicker-fusion frequency, varying flash intensity to establish the amount needed to elicit a given probability of letter identification. The fourth experiment displayed each letter twice, once at a frequency where no flicker was perceived and also with steady light emission. The intensity of each flash was fixed and the steady intensity was varied; respondents were asked to judge whether the fused-flicker display and the steady display appeared to be the same brightness. Steady intensity was about double the average flash intensity where the two conditions were perceived as being equal in brightness. This is at odds with Talbot-Plateau law, which predicts that these two values should be equal. The law was formulated relative to a flash lasting half of each period, so it is surprising that it comes this close to being correct where the flash occupies only a millionth of the total period.  相似文献   

14.
Cyclic GMP phosphodiesterase in frog rod outer segments is activated after flash illumination and is inactivated when left in the dark. ATP reduces the initial peak activity caused by dim flashes (with 50 microM ATP being required for a half-maximal effect) and also accelerates inactivation (with 2 microM ATP being required for a half-maximal effect). An acceleration of inactivation caused by ATP addition is 3-7-fold, depending on the preparation, and ATP effect can be observed even 1 min after a dim flash is given. The accelerated inactivation is also flash intensity-dependent. A low intensity of light causes more rapid inactivation than does a high intensity of light. ATP appears to control phosphodiesterase activity in various ways.  相似文献   

15.
The sequence (a) priming flash, (b) dark interval, and (c) red light induces a long-lasting afterdepolarization (PDA) in Balanus photoreceptors. The inward flow of membrane current associated with the decay of PDA was independent of red test flashes, provided that PDA had plateaued at a particular intensity. The influence of wavelength and duration of the priming flash and their interaction with the dark interval were investigated. Increasing the duration of the priming flash produced a systematic increase in PDA duration. The dark interval plays a crucial role in PDA induction. The priming flash duration and the dark interval were reciprocally related, i.e, short flashes followed by long dark intervals induced as much PDA as long priming flashes followed by short dark intervals. The action spectrum for the priming flash was found to correspond to that of the primary photopigment (VP537).  相似文献   

16.
An electronic flash unit is used to deliver, at the beginning of a 10 min dark period and within a few ms, large doses of light to Albizzia julibrissin pinnules, to ascertain their effects on the rate of pinnule closing. In a series of alternating light flashes at 710 and 550 nm, the first 710 nm light flash significantly retards closing. A following light flash at 550 nm negates the far-red induced delay. The second 710 nm light flash delays closing less effectively than the first when given within 4 s after the green flash, but is just as effective when given after 30 s. The delay brought about by the second 710 nm light flash is again abolished by a light flash at 550 nm. A light flash at 660 nm has no effect on pinnule closing by itself and is also ineffective in reversing the far-red induced delay. A series of ten 710 nm light flashes becomes most effective in delaying closure when there is a dark interval of one min between flashes. The closing delay induced by a 710 nm light flash escapes reversal by a 550 nm light flash when the dark interval between the two flashes exceeds 2–3 min. A 750 nm light flash has no retarding effect on pinnule closing, but it becomes effective when preceded by a 660 nm or 550 nm light flash. The results obtained are suggested to be due to light absorbed by phytochrome and an unknown photoreceptor with green, far-red photoreversal property.  相似文献   

17.
Fireflies are well known for the use of bioluminescence for sexual communication. In species using flash signals for pair formation, species and sexual identity are conferred by flash timing parameters such as flash duration, flash interval, flash number, and response delay. In dialog fireflies in North America, the male is the advertiser and the female is the responder. In these species, the male flash signal parameter varies depending on species, but the female flash signal parameter is limited only to response delay. However, in fireflies other than dialog fireflies, sexual flash communication is not well studied. Although many female-advertisement-like fireflies are reported, we have no confirmed case of sexual communication in a female-advertisement species. Here, we report the sexual flash communication of an Asian firefly, Luciola (Hotaria) parvula, in which the female flashes spontaneously. By using an electronic firefly, we confirm experimentally that males are specifically attracted to flashes with a female-specific flash duration. This is the first experimental report of sexual communication of a female advertiser in firefly communication. In this species, females call males usually with spontaneous flashes unlike dialog fireflies.  相似文献   

18.
Electroretinograms were obtained from the all-rod eye of the rat with uniform illumination of the entire retina and stimulus flashes of less than 3 msec. duration. Bloch's law of temporal summation was verified for the b-wave latency by varying the time between two equal intensity flashes and observing that no change occurred in the latency when measured from the midpoint of the two flashes. The results of this and other experiments are described in terms of a simple but general model of the latency-determining mechanism. It is shown that this latency mechanism acts as if it depends on a linear additive process; and also that a hypothetical excitatory substance which triggers activity in the sources of the b-wave must accumulate rapidly in time after the flash, approximately as t8. The rate at which this substance accumulates is accurately represented by the diffusion equation for more than 4 to 6 log units in the flash intensity. This suggests that the rate-determining step in the latency mechanism may be diffusion-limited.  相似文献   

19.
Pulsed, time resolved photoacoustics has sufficient sensitivity to determine oxygen emission and uptake by single turnover flashes to leaves. The advantage over previous methodologies is that when combined with single turnover flashes the kinetics of the thermal and the gas signals can be resolved to 0.1 millisecond and separated. The S-state oscillations of oxygen formation are readily observed. The gas signal from common spongy leaves such as spinach (Spinacia sp.), Japanese andromeda (Pieris japonica), mock orange (Philadelphus coronarius) and viburnum (Viburnum tomentosum), after correction for instrumental rise time, show a lag of only 1 millisecond and a rise time of 5 milliseconds in the formation of oxygen. Thus a recent proposal that the formation of oxygen requires over 100 milliseconds cannot be true for choroplasts in vivo. The rapid emission is correlated with structure of the leaf. At low light flash energies a rapid gas uptake is observed. The uptake has slightly slower kinetics than oxygen evolution, and its magnitude increases with damage to the leaf. The pulse methodology shows that the uptake begins with the very first flash after dark adaption, and allows the detection of a positive signal (oxygen) on the third flash. These observations, the long wavelength of excitation (695 nanometers) and the magnitude of the signal support the contention that the gas uptake is oxygen reduction by electrons from photosystem I. These results show that important physiological aspects of a leaf can be studied by pulsed, time resolved photoacoustics.  相似文献   

20.
Enhancement and phototransduction in the ventral eye of limulus   总被引:9,自引:8,他引:1       下载免费PDF全文
Limulus ventral photoreceptors were voltage clamped to the resting (dark) potential and stimulated by a 20-ms test flash and a 1-s conditioning flash. At a constant level of adaptation, we measured the response to the test flash given in the dark (control) and the incremental response produced when the test flash occurred within the duration of the conditioning flash. The incremental response is defined as the response to the conditioning and test flashes minus the response to the conditioning flash given alone. When the test flash was presented within 100 ms after the onset of the conditioning flash we observed that: (a) for dim conditioning flashes the incremental response equaled the control response; (b) for intermediate intensity conditioning flashes the incremental response was greater than the control response (we refer to this as enhancement); (c) for high intensity conditioning flashes the incremental response nearly equaled the control response. Using 10-μm diam spots of illumnination, we stimulated two spatially separate regions of one photoreceptor. When the test flash and the conditioning flash were presented to the same region, enhancement was present; but when the flashes were applied to separate regions, enhancement was nearly absent. This result indicates that enhancement is localized to the region of illumination. We discuss mechanisms that may account for enhancement.  相似文献   

设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号